March 2001
Volume 42, Issue 3
Free
Anatomy and Pathology/Oncology  |   March 2001
Endogenous Rhythms in Axial Length and Choroidal Thickness in Chicks: Implications for Ocular Growth Regulation
Author Affiliations
  • Debora L. Nickla
    From the New England College of Optometry, Boston, Massachusetts; and the
  • Christine F. Wildsoet
    School of Optometry, University of California, Berkeley.
  • David Troilo
    From the New England College of Optometry, Boston, Massachusetts; and the
Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science March 2001, Vol.42, 584-588. doi:
  • Views
  • PDF
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Debora L. Nickla, Christine F. Wildsoet, David Troilo; Endogenous Rhythms in Axial Length and Choroidal Thickness in Chicks: Implications for Ocular Growth Regulation. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2001;42(3):584-588.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract

purpose. To determine whether the diurnal rhythms in axial length and choroidal thickness in the chick eye are endogenous circadian rhythms.

methods. Six chickens, 14 days of age, were put into darkness for 4 days. Beginning on the 3rd day, ocular dimensions were measured using high-frequency A-scan ultrasonography, in darkness, at 6-hour intervals over 48 hours. Five age-matched chickens reared in a normal light/dark (L/D) cycle and measured at 6-hour intervals for 5 days were controls.

results. The rhythms in axial length and choroidal thickness persist in constant darkness. The phases of these rhythms are approximately in antiphase to one another, similar to those of eyes in a L/D cycle; however, the peak of the rhythm in axial length occurs slightly earlier relative to that of eyes in L/D (12 PM versus 3 PM; P < 0.05, one-tailed t-test). By the 3rd day in darkness, the rate of growth is significantly higher than that in L/D (117 versus 72μ m/24 hours; P < 0.01), and the choroid becomes significantly thinner (159 versus 210 μm; P < 0.0001).

conclusions. The rhythms in axial length and choroid thickness are circadian rhythms, driven by an endogenous oscillator. The phase of the rhythm in axial length in constant darkness is slightly phase-advanced relative to eyes in L/D and thus is similar to eyes that are deprived of form vision. These findings suggest that in the absence of visual input, the eyes revert to a “default” growth state and that the similarities between the effects of constant darkness and of form deprivation suggest that deprivation may represent a type of“ constant” condition.

The eyes of young chickens show a diurnal rhythm in axial elongation, growing more during the day than during the night. 1 2 3 The thickness of the choroid, the tissue behind the retina, also shows a diurnal rhythm, thickening during the night and thinning during the day, in approximate antiphase to the rhythm in axial length. 2 3 It has been shown that several visual manipulations that alter the rate of ocular growth are associated with consistent effects on the phase relationships between these rhythms: specifically, myopic defocus, induced either by prior form deprivation or positive spectacle lenses, both of which cause a compensatory decrease in growth rate, is associated with the two rhythms shifting into phase with one another; this is effected by a phase delay in the rhythm in axial length and a phase advance in the rhythm in choroid thickness. 2 4 Conversely, form deprivation, which causes an increase in growth rate, results in a phase advance in the rhythm in elongation that shifts the two rhythms into “exact” antiphase with one another. 2  
To more fully understand the significance of the above observations in the context of ocular growth regulation, it is important to know whether these rhythms in dimension are circadian (i.e., driven by an endogenous clock) or whether they are strictly diurnal (driven by the cycle of light and dark [L/D]). To answer this question, we measured eyes at 6-hour intervals for 48 hours, after 2 days in constant darkness and compared these to age-matched animals in a diurnal L/D cycle from a previous study. 2 If the phase relationships between the rhythms in axial length and choroidal thickness are related to altered growth, as our previous studies suggest, it is possible that the phases of the endogenous rhythms may differ from those seen in L/D, because it has long been known that rearing chickens in constant darkness results in abnormally large eyes. 5 6 7 Such results would provide further evidence linking shifts in phase with altered ocular growth rates. 
We here show that both the rhythms in axial elongation and in choroidal thickness persist in constant darkness. The peak in the rhythm in axial length occurs slightly earlier than that for normal eyes in L/D, similar to form-deprived eyes in L/D. 2 Furthermore, constant darkness results in a rapid decrease in choroidal thickness and an increase in ocular growth rate, also similar to form-deprived eyes in L/D. We conclude that the absence of form vision and/or temporal cues in constant darkness causes the eyes to revert to a“ default” growth state, in which growth rate is faster than would be required for emmetropization. We speculate that the similarities to eyes deprived of form vision suggest that form deprivation causes eyes to revert to the default state. It follows that form vision, not light per se, exerts a restraining influence on growth rate, possibly via subtle influences on the phases of these two rhythms. Parts of this work have been presented in abstract form. 8 9  
Methods
Six untreated birds were raised in a normal L/D cycle (12 hours L/12 hours D) until 2 weeks of age, after which they were put into constant darkness for the duration of the study. Food and water were supplied ad libitum. Large amounts of food were scattered on the floor to provide more ready access in darkness. Birds were monitored daily for feeding by checking their crops for the presence of food. After 48 hours in darkness, ocular dimensions were measured at 6-hour intervals for 48 hours (6 AM, 12 PM, 6 PM, and 12 AM) using high-frequency A-scan ultrasonography (for details, see Ref. 2 ). This protocol for assessing the circadian nature of a rhythm follows standard procedure whereby the organism (or tissue) is “released” into constant darkness and measured after 1 to several days. For example, most of the studies on the circadian nature of the rhythm in intraocular pressure measure within 3 days in darkness. 10 11 12  
For ultrasonography, birds were anesthetized with halothane (1% in oxygen). Measurements were done in darkness, with the aid of a dark yellow photographic safe light (the level of illumination was ∼0.5 lux at the bird’s eye); measurements typically took less than 5 minutes, after which birds were returned to darkness. Controls were 5 normal age-matched birds from a previous study that were reared in L/D and measured at 6-hour intervals over 5 days. 2 All animal use was in compliance with the ARVO Statement for the Use of Animals in Ophthalmic and Vision Research. 
Data Analysis
Because these measurements are done on growing eyes, we devised a method to separate the “cyclic component” from the “steady state” growth component to assess the parameters of the rhythms: phase, amplitude, and period. 2 In brief, we assume that the growth curve (as in Fig. 1A ) is composed of a diurnal rhythm superimposed on a steady rate of growth. To separate these, a regression line is fit to the data for each eye; the slope of this line yields the steady state rate of growth (these data are used to compare rates of growth between groups). The regression is then subtracted from the raw data; these residuals represent the pure cyclic component for each eye. From these, one can obtain phase, amplitude, and period by fitting a sine wave to these data. For statistical comparisons of phase, a sine wave with a period of 24 hours is fit to the data for each eye for the 4th day in darkness. This cycle was chosen because the phase of the rhythm is less variable over this last 24-hour period and because it could possibly be a more accurate reflection of the true circadian (free-running) rhythm. For statistical tests of phase, only those data meeting our inclusion criterion, that the mean SD of the residuals of the sine wave fit to the data were less than the SD of the raw data, and less than 60% of the amplitude of the sine wave fit to the data (a similar method was used in Ref. 2 ). These data and the number of eyes used are shown in Table 1 (“From individual eyes”). All descriptions of phase in Results are obtained from these data. For graphical depictions, we show the sine waves fit to the mean of the residuals for all eyes; the period used for these fits was either 24 hours or was left unconstrained, as indicated. The phases obtained from these “mean” sine fits are shown in Table 1 for comparison (“From averaged data”). Amplitude is derived from these fits to the mean of the data. t-Tests were used for all statistical comparisons. 
Results
Axial Length
The rhythm in axial length persists in constant darkness (Fig. 1) . Figure 1A shows the mean axial length and SEs for all eyes as a function of time over the measurement period: at both midnight points there is a mean reduction in length compared with the previous time point (arrows), similar to eyes in L/D. 2 However, after 48 hours in constant darkness the growth rate is significantly higher than for eyes in L/D (117 versus 72 μm/d; t = 3.1, P < 0.01). The cyclic component is shown in the inset on the bottom of the graph (symbols) with two sine waves fit to the data: the solid wave is the diurnal (i.e., 24 hour) fit wave, and the dashed wave is the best fit to the data, which yields a shorter period (20 hours). This shorter period is the result of four eyes showing anomalous 12-hour oscillations over the first cycle, which skews the period. The mean peak occurs at about noon (11 AM from averaged data, 12 PM from individual eyes; Table 1 ). 
To assess the diurnal changes in growth rate as a function of time of day, we subtracted the axial length at each time point from the length at the next time point for each eye (data for both cycles combined); this gives the mean rate per interval (Fig. 1B) . Similar to eyes in L/D, the rate of growth is lowest (negligible) in the interval from 6 PM to midnight (Fig. 1B , midpoint 9 PM: gray bars and white square). However, the time of highest growth rate differs: in darkness it occurs between midnight and 6 AM (midpoint 3 AM, gray bars; 63 μm versus all other intervals: 9 AM and 3 PM, P < 0.05; 9 PM, P < 0.0001), whereas in L/D it occurs between 6 AM and noon (midpoint 9 AM, white square). These differences between groups are significant (dark versus L/D, midpoint 3 AM: 63 versus 26 μm/6 hours; t = 2.8, P < 0.05; midpoint 9 AM: 32 versus 67 μm/6 hours; t = 2.2, P < 0.01). 
Although the means of the data for the diurnal changes in axial length for eyes in L/D appear sinusoidal, the means for eyes in constant darkness do not (Fig. 1B , cf. bars and white squares, and see Ref. 2 ). This is the result of several eyes over the first 24 hours (3rd day in dark) showing anomalous 12-hour oscillations, which is apparent in the mean residuals in the inset of Figure 1A (first 5 data points). However, if we use the data from the 4th day in darkness, the residuals can be well fit to a sine wave with a 24-hour period (solid symbols and solidwave, Fig. 1C ). A comparison of these eyes with eyes in L/D (solid symbols and waves versus open symbols and dashed waves) indicates that the mean peak occurs slightly earlier than in L/D (Table 1 ; 12 PM versus 3 PM; t = 1.76, df = 16, P < 0.05, one-tailed t-test). The mean amplitude in darkness is 30μ m (peak to peak, Fig. 1A , right axis), similar to that for eyes in L/D (37 μm; Ref. 2 ). 
Choroid Thickness
The rhythm in choroidal thickness also persists in constant darkness: choroids become thicker during subjective night and thinner during subjective day (Figs. 2 and 3) , with the mean peak occurring at 12 AM (Table 1 ; data from individual eyes). This is similar to the phase for eyes in L/D, which also show a peak in the middle of the night (Table 1) . The rhythm is approximately sinusoidal, having a mean amplitude of 13 μm (Fig. 2 , right axis), also similar to that of eyes in L/D (16 μm, Ref. 2 ), and a diurnal period (best fit sine wave period = 24.5 hours). Another effect of constant darkness is that the thickness of the choroid decreases over this period by approximately 7 μm/d (159 μm at start versus 140 μm at end; paired t-test, t = 2.1, P = 0.06). This thinning must have begun shortly after the chicks were put into darkness: by the first measurement in darkness (after 48 hours), choroids were already significantly thinner than those in eyes in L/D (159 versus 210 μm, measurement at 6 AM in both conditions; Ref. 2 ; t = 5.2, P < 0.0001). 
We conclude that the endogenous rhythms in axial length and choroidal thickness are approximately in antiphase to one another (12 hours apart; Fig. 3 ), similar to eyes in L/D. This phase difference is significant (t = 7.7; df = 16, P < 0.001). 
Discussion
Rearing chickens in constant darkness produces excessive ocular elongation 5 6 7 and initially, myopia (until the dark-induced corneal flattening produces hyperopia 6 ). A hypothesis consistent with this finding is that the “default” ocular growth rate is abnormally fast and that visual experience in the form of either light, or pattern vision, exercises a restraining influence through effects on some ocular rhythms involved in growth. An understanding of the role of visual input on these rhythms requires understanding their expression in the absence of visual or temporal cues. We report that the rhythms in axial length and choroid thickness persist in constant darkness, which defines them as circadian, endogenous rhythms. 
We show that eyes “released” into constant darkness show several differences from eyes in L/D: (1) there is an increase in the rate of growth, (2) there is a decrease in choroidal thickness, and (3) the peak in the rhythm in axial length occurs slightly earlier. These results imply that in the absence of visual input eyes return to a“ default” condition of excessive growth. Similar effects are also found in eyes that are deprived of form vision by translucent occluders, implying that deprivation may resemble a “constant condition” under which eyes also revert to the “default” state. Because diffusers do not appreciably reduce the amount of light reaching the retina (0.6 log units, Ref. 13 ) but do reduce spatial contrast, it follows that visual transients, in addition to light per se, may function as a Zeitgeber for these rhythms and may influence ocular growth. We hypothesize that the effects of visual transients on ocular growth are effected (at least in part) by shifts in phase in one or more ocular rhythms. We speculate that the small phase advance in the rhythm in axial length in constant darkness and in form deprivation may be causally related to the faster rate of growth in both conditions. This would imply that form vision phase-delays a rhythm underlying the rhythm in elongation (e.g., the rhythm in scleral proteoglycan synthesis 14 ), perhaps influencing the growth rate by virtue of altering the phase relationships between this rhythm and others (such as IOP 15 ). It remains unresolved however, whether these effects on phase are a cause, or a consequence, of the changes in growth rate. It is plausible, for instance, that the phase advance seen in darkness is a reflection of the manifestation of the free-running rhythm, in which a small deviation from a 24-hour period could produce a similar result, making the similarity between darkness and form deprivation merely coincidental. Measurements over a longer period in constant darkness would be required to test this hypothesis. 
The thinning of the choroid produced by constant darkness provides another example in which thin choroids are associated with excessive ocular elongation (form deprivation, 16 negative lenses, 17 and constant light [Wildsoet, unpublished observations] are the others). This inverse relationship between choroidal thickness and eye size has recently been reported in primates as well. 18 19 It is possible that these thinner choroids are the consequence of a simple mechanical stretching resulting from the expanding eye. However, we speculate that the experimentally induced changes in choroid thickness alter the diffusion dynamics between the retina and sclera and in this way modulate the influence of retinal growth factors on the sclera. This hypothesis is more consistent with the notion that choroidal thickness is actively modulated, as suggested by the circadian nature of the rhythm and the phase shifts observed under different visual conditions. 2  
Given the increasing prevalence of myopia in humans, it is of interest to know to what extent these findings might extrapolate to the primate eye as a potential source of new insight into how myopia develops. The recent controversy over the association between the use of nursery night-lights and the development of childhood myopia 20 21 22 gives new impetus to determining the relevance of circadian rhythms in ocular growth regulation. Our present findings show that the rhythms in axial elongation and choroidal thickness persist in constant darkness and are therefore circadian rhythms driven by an endogenous clock. 
 
Figure 1.
 
The circadian rhythm in axial length. (A) Mean axial length as a function of time for all eyes in constant darkness; error bars, SE (black circles, left axis). Dark bars on x axis, subjective night; arrows, midnight. Eyes show a decrease in growth during subjective night. The mean growth rate (117 μm/d) is the mean of the slopes of the regression lines fit to individual eyes. Inset (bottom): mean of the residuals after subtracting the regression (open circles, right axis), with two sine waves fit to the data: solid wave, sine wave with a 24-hour period; dashed wave, best fit sine function with a 20-hourperiod reflecting the 12-hour oscillations in four eyes over the first cycle (first five data points). (B) Mean change in length for all eyes over each of the 6-hour intervals (both cycles combined) for eyes in darkness (gray bars) and eyes in L/D (open squares); error bars, SE. These data represent rates (μm/6 h); the time of greatest growth rate differs for the two groups of eyes, with the “peak” occurring earlier in darkness than in L/D. (C) “Cyclic” component of data (residuals) for eyes in darkness (solid symbols and lines: left y axis) and eyes in L/D (open symbols and dashed lines: right y axis) during the 4th day in darkness. Data (symbols) are the mean of the residuals, and the sine waves fit to the data have a period of 24 hours. The mean peak for eyes in darkness occurs slightly earlier than that for eyes in L/D.
Figure 1.
 
The circadian rhythm in axial length. (A) Mean axial length as a function of time for all eyes in constant darkness; error bars, SE (black circles, left axis). Dark bars on x axis, subjective night; arrows, midnight. Eyes show a decrease in growth during subjective night. The mean growth rate (117 μm/d) is the mean of the slopes of the regression lines fit to individual eyes. Inset (bottom): mean of the residuals after subtracting the regression (open circles, right axis), with two sine waves fit to the data: solid wave, sine wave with a 24-hour period; dashed wave, best fit sine function with a 20-hourperiod reflecting the 12-hour oscillations in four eyes over the first cycle (first five data points). (B) Mean change in length for all eyes over each of the 6-hour intervals (both cycles combined) for eyes in darkness (gray bars) and eyes in L/D (open squares); error bars, SE. These data represent rates (μm/6 h); the time of greatest growth rate differs for the two groups of eyes, with the “peak” occurring earlier in darkness than in L/D. (C) “Cyclic” component of data (residuals) for eyes in darkness (solid symbols and lines: left y axis) and eyes in L/D (open symbols and dashed lines: right y axis) during the 4th day in darkness. Data (symbols) are the mean of the residuals, and the sine waves fit to the data have a period of 24 hours. The mean peak for eyes in darkness occurs slightly earlier than that for eyes in L/D.
Table 1.
 
Phase for Axial Length and Choroidal Thickness
Table 1.
 
Phase for Axial Length and Choroidal Thickness
Condition Component From Averaged Data* From Individual Eyes, †
;l>L/D Axial length 3:30 PM 3:00 PM ± 3.5 (8)
Choroid 12:00 AM 10:30 PM ± 3.7 (6)
Dark Axial length 11:00 AM 12:00 PM ± 2.8 (10)
Choroid 1:00 AM 12:00 AM ± 3.4 (8)
Figure 2.
 
The circadian rhythm in choroid thickness. Mean choroidal thickness as a function of time for all eyes in constant darkness (black squares, left axis); error bars, SE. Dark bars on x axis, subjective night. Choroidal thickness increases during subjective night and decreases during subjective day. Furthermore, there is a steady decrease in thickness over the 2 days of measurement in darkness (mean = −7 μm/d). Inset (bottom): mean of the residuals after subtracting the regressions (open squares, right axis), with the best fit sine wave to the data (24.5 hour period). The rhythm is circadian and sinusoidal.
Figure 2.
 
The circadian rhythm in choroid thickness. Mean choroidal thickness as a function of time for all eyes in constant darkness (black squares, left axis); error bars, SE. Dark bars on x axis, subjective night. Choroidal thickness increases during subjective night and decreases during subjective day. Furthermore, there is a steady decrease in thickness over the 2 days of measurement in darkness (mean = −7 μm/d). Inset (bottom): mean of the residuals after subtracting the regressions (open squares, right axis), with the best fit sine wave to the data (24.5 hour period). The rhythm is circadian and sinusoidal.
Figure 3.
 
The circadian rhythm in axial length (black circles, solid wave) and choroidal thickness (open circles, dashed wave) on the 4th day in darkness. Symbols, the mean of the residuals (subtracting the regression line); waves, the sine waves with a 24 hour period fit to these data. Both sets of data are normalized to the means for each eye to get rid of individual variation in eye size and choroid thickness; error bars, SE. Black bars on x axis, subjective night. The two rhythms are approximately in antiphase to one another and both are sinusoidal.
Figure 3.
 
The circadian rhythm in axial length (black circles, solid wave) and choroidal thickness (open circles, dashed wave) on the 4th day in darkness. Symbols, the mean of the residuals (subtracting the regression line); waves, the sine waves with a 24 hour period fit to these data. Both sets of data are normalized to the means for each eye to get rid of individual variation in eye size and choroid thickness; error bars, SE. Black bars on x axis, subjective night. The two rhythms are approximately in antiphase to one another and both are sinusoidal.
The authors thank Margo Cummings and Heidi Denman for animal care. 
Weiss S, Schaeffel F. Diurnal growth rhythms in the chicken eye: relation to myopia development and retinal dopamine levels. J Comp Physiol A. 1993;172:263–270. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Nickla DL, Wildsoet C, Wallman J. Visual influences on diurnal rhythms in ocular length and choroidal thickness in chick eyes. Exp Eye Res. 1998;66:163–181. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Papastergiou GI, Schmid GF, Riva CE, et al. Ocular axial length and choroidal thickness in newly hatched chicks and one-year-old chickens fluctuate in a diurnal pattern that is influenced by visual experience and intraocular pressure changes. Exp Eye Res. 1998;66:195–205. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Nickla DL, Wildsoet CF, Wallman J. Myopic defocus, but not form-deprivation, causes large phase shifts in diurnal rhythms of choroid thickness and of axial length [ARVO Abstract]. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1996;37(4)S687.Abstract nr 3141.
Lauber JK, Kinnear A. Eye enlargement in birds induced by dim light. Can J Ophthalmol. 1979;14:265–269. [PubMed]
Gottlieb MD, Wentzek LA, Wallman J. Different visual restrictions produce different ametropias and different eye shapes. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1987;28:1225–1235. [PubMed]
Yinon U, Koslowe KC. Hypermetropia in dark reared chicks and the effect of lid suture. Vision Res. 1986;26:999–1005. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Nickla DL, Wildsoet CF, Troilo D. Circadian and diurnal aspects of eye growth in chicks: the rhythms in ocular elongation and choroidal thickness persist in constant dark [ARVO Abstract]. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1998;39(4)S868.Abstract nr 4038.
Nickla D, Wildsoet C, Troilo D. Ocular diurnal rhythms in animal models of eye growth: implications for ocular growth regulation. Proceedings of the VIII International Conference on Myopia. 2000:302–307.
Moore CG, Johnson EC, Morrison JC. Circadian rhythm of intraocular pressure in the rat. Curr Eye Res. 1996;15:185–191. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Rowland JM, Potter DE, Reiter RJ. Circadian rhythm in intraocular pressure: a rabbit model. Curr Eye Res. 1981;1:169–173. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Krishna R, Mermoud A, Baerveldt G, Minckler DS. Circadian rhythm of intraocular pressure: a rat model. Ophthalmic Res. 1995;27:163–167. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Wallman J, Turkel J, Trachtman J. Extreme myopia produced by modest changes in early visual experience. Science. 1978;201:1249–1251. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Nickla DL, Rada JA, Wallman J. Isolated chick sclera shows a circadian rhythm in proteoglycan synthesis perhaps associated with the rhythm in ocular elongation. J Comp Physiol A. 1999;185:81–90. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Nickla DL, Wildsoet C, Wallman J. The circadian rhythm in intraocular pressure and its relation to diurnal ocular growth changes in chicks. Exp Eye Res. 1998;66:183–193. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Wallman J, Wildsoet C, Xu A, et al. Moving the retina: choroidal modulation of refractive state. Vision Res. 1995;35:37–50. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Wildsoet C, Wallman J. Choroidal and scleral mechanisms of compensation for spectacle lenses in chicks. Vision Res. 1995;35:1175–1194. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Hung L-F, Wallman J, Smith E. Vision-dependent changes in the choroidal thickness of Macaque monkeys. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2000;41:1259–1269. [PubMed]
Troilo D, Nickla D, Wildsoet C. Choroidal thickness changes during altered eye growth and refractive state in a primate. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2000;41:1249–1258. [PubMed]
Quinn GE, Shin CH, Maguire MG, Stone RA. Myopia and ambient lighting at night. Nature. 1999;399:113–114. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Zadnik K, Jones LA, Irvin BC, et al. Myopia and ambient night-time lighting. Nature. 2000;404:143–144. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Gwiazda J, Ong E, Held R, Thorn F. Myopia and ambient night-time lighting. Nature. 2000;404:144. [PubMed]
Figure 1.
 
The circadian rhythm in axial length. (A) Mean axial length as a function of time for all eyes in constant darkness; error bars, SE (black circles, left axis). Dark bars on x axis, subjective night; arrows, midnight. Eyes show a decrease in growth during subjective night. The mean growth rate (117 μm/d) is the mean of the slopes of the regression lines fit to individual eyes. Inset (bottom): mean of the residuals after subtracting the regression (open circles, right axis), with two sine waves fit to the data: solid wave, sine wave with a 24-hour period; dashed wave, best fit sine function with a 20-hourperiod reflecting the 12-hour oscillations in four eyes over the first cycle (first five data points). (B) Mean change in length for all eyes over each of the 6-hour intervals (both cycles combined) for eyes in darkness (gray bars) and eyes in L/D (open squares); error bars, SE. These data represent rates (μm/6 h); the time of greatest growth rate differs for the two groups of eyes, with the “peak” occurring earlier in darkness than in L/D. (C) “Cyclic” component of data (residuals) for eyes in darkness (solid symbols and lines: left y axis) and eyes in L/D (open symbols and dashed lines: right y axis) during the 4th day in darkness. Data (symbols) are the mean of the residuals, and the sine waves fit to the data have a period of 24 hours. The mean peak for eyes in darkness occurs slightly earlier than that for eyes in L/D.
Figure 1.
 
The circadian rhythm in axial length. (A) Mean axial length as a function of time for all eyes in constant darkness; error bars, SE (black circles, left axis). Dark bars on x axis, subjective night; arrows, midnight. Eyes show a decrease in growth during subjective night. The mean growth rate (117 μm/d) is the mean of the slopes of the regression lines fit to individual eyes. Inset (bottom): mean of the residuals after subtracting the regression (open circles, right axis), with two sine waves fit to the data: solid wave, sine wave with a 24-hour period; dashed wave, best fit sine function with a 20-hourperiod reflecting the 12-hour oscillations in four eyes over the first cycle (first five data points). (B) Mean change in length for all eyes over each of the 6-hour intervals (both cycles combined) for eyes in darkness (gray bars) and eyes in L/D (open squares); error bars, SE. These data represent rates (μm/6 h); the time of greatest growth rate differs for the two groups of eyes, with the “peak” occurring earlier in darkness than in L/D. (C) “Cyclic” component of data (residuals) for eyes in darkness (solid symbols and lines: left y axis) and eyes in L/D (open symbols and dashed lines: right y axis) during the 4th day in darkness. Data (symbols) are the mean of the residuals, and the sine waves fit to the data have a period of 24 hours. The mean peak for eyes in darkness occurs slightly earlier than that for eyes in L/D.
Figure 2.
 
The circadian rhythm in choroid thickness. Mean choroidal thickness as a function of time for all eyes in constant darkness (black squares, left axis); error bars, SE. Dark bars on x axis, subjective night. Choroidal thickness increases during subjective night and decreases during subjective day. Furthermore, there is a steady decrease in thickness over the 2 days of measurement in darkness (mean = −7 μm/d). Inset (bottom): mean of the residuals after subtracting the regressions (open squares, right axis), with the best fit sine wave to the data (24.5 hour period). The rhythm is circadian and sinusoidal.
Figure 2.
 
The circadian rhythm in choroid thickness. Mean choroidal thickness as a function of time for all eyes in constant darkness (black squares, left axis); error bars, SE. Dark bars on x axis, subjective night. Choroidal thickness increases during subjective night and decreases during subjective day. Furthermore, there is a steady decrease in thickness over the 2 days of measurement in darkness (mean = −7 μm/d). Inset (bottom): mean of the residuals after subtracting the regressions (open squares, right axis), with the best fit sine wave to the data (24.5 hour period). The rhythm is circadian and sinusoidal.
Figure 3.
 
The circadian rhythm in axial length (black circles, solid wave) and choroidal thickness (open circles, dashed wave) on the 4th day in darkness. Symbols, the mean of the residuals (subtracting the regression line); waves, the sine waves with a 24 hour period fit to these data. Both sets of data are normalized to the means for each eye to get rid of individual variation in eye size and choroid thickness; error bars, SE. Black bars on x axis, subjective night. The two rhythms are approximately in antiphase to one another and both are sinusoidal.
Figure 3.
 
The circadian rhythm in axial length (black circles, solid wave) and choroidal thickness (open circles, dashed wave) on the 4th day in darkness. Symbols, the mean of the residuals (subtracting the regression line); waves, the sine waves with a 24 hour period fit to these data. Both sets of data are normalized to the means for each eye to get rid of individual variation in eye size and choroid thickness; error bars, SE. Black bars on x axis, subjective night. The two rhythms are approximately in antiphase to one another and both are sinusoidal.
Table 1.
 
Phase for Axial Length and Choroidal Thickness
Table 1.
 
Phase for Axial Length and Choroidal Thickness
Condition Component From Averaged Data* From Individual Eyes, †
;l>L/D Axial length 3:30 PM 3:00 PM ± 3.5 (8)
Choroid 12:00 AM 10:30 PM ± 3.7 (6)
Dark Axial length 11:00 AM 12:00 PM ± 2.8 (10)
Choroid 1:00 AM 12:00 AM ± 3.4 (8)
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×