This is the first physiological study to directly examine optical
properties of the lens during accommodation that has been induced in a
manner approximating the in vivo condition. Given that all intraocular
structures remained in their natural anatomic configurations and that
accommodation was induced in all age groups—that is, stimulation of
the ciliary nerve resulted in shorter focal lengths
(Fig. 2) , using a
method that results in accommodation in vivo, the results presented
herein were taken to represent functional optics as they would be in
the intact eye.
To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to show an
adverse effect of age on lenticular accommodative function in chickens,
with reduction in lenticular accommodation associated concomitantly
with increasing age, a characteristic of presbyopia. Although
age-matching of chickens to humans has not been analyzed, it must be
noted that chickens are precocial birds, opening and using their eyes
the day of hatching. Hens usually begin laying eggs by the end of 5
months, an indication that 1- and 2-year-old chickens may be comparable
to middle-aged humans. Because chickens older than 2 years were not
available, whether even older chickens would show further reduction in
lenticular accommodation capability or an absence of accommodative
response altogether, remains unknown. It should be noted that although
lenticular accommodation was reduced in older chickens, it was not at
an insignificant level. These chickens were still able to accommodate,
just not to the extent that was possible by younger chickens. It should
also be noted that the effect of age on corneal accommodation is not
known.
As with humans, it is also difficult to determine the cause of
presbyopia in chickens. Changes in the accommodative amplitude may be
due to biophysical changes to the lens, weakening of the ciliary
muscle, or both. It must be noted that hardening of the lens, whether
due to changes in thickness or an increase in lenticular protein
concentrations, would have an effect on the ciliary muscle, with
requirements of greater ciliary muscle function. As reported herein,
lenticular focal lengths in 2-year-old chickens were slightly shorter,
although not significantly, than those in 1-year-old chickens, which
raises an intriguing possibility that with more samples or with even
older chickens, lenticular focal lengths would shorten with increasing
age. Physiologically, this is possible if growth of the lens is
accompanied by an increased protein accumulation, causing an increase
in refractive index and/or thickening of the lens. If this were to
occur, it would be an indication that the refractive index of the lens
had increased and that the ciliary muscle could have been affected
because of the increased effort required to squeeze a less flexible or
thicker lens. It should be noted that in human eyes, the suspensory
ligaments play an indirect role during accommodation, and therefore
age-related changes to the ligaments could contribute to presbyopia.
Given that this anatomic structure is not a component in avian
accommodation, its contribution to the presbyopia observed in this
study was eliminated. However, if presbyopia is a function of changes
in lens consistency and/or ciliary muscle contractibility, then the
avian model of presbyopia presented herein may have some relevance to
human presbyopia.
The changes in accommodative amplitude in the current study correlate
well with the results of Glasser et al.,
4 which showed
lenticular accommodative changes of approximately 10 D in 4-week-old
birds. Estimates of the change in accommodation in 4-week-old chickens
in the current study would fall somewhere between approximately 6 and
10 D (estimate from
Fig. 2 ). Differences in dioptric values may be
accounted for by variations in chicken strain and environments in which
the chickens were raised. In their study, Glasser et al. suggested that
the 10-D change in accommodation they observed was probably not the
true extent of lenticular accommodation in the chick, because of
backward movement of the lens, mediated by loss of intraocular pressure
(IOP) and removal of the vitreous, and because they had previously
measured greater accommodative amplitudes using 0.011% nicotine
stimulation.
It remains unknown whether the arguments expressed by Glasser et al.
held true in the current work. The degree of backward movement of the
lens, if any, was not assessed. Thus, it is possible that backward
movement of the lens occurred in the current experiments, caused by
removal of the back of the globe. However, there were several
differences in this study that may have helped to alleviate loss of
IOP. Vitreous was not removed, and the eye was placed near the bottom
of the chamber. Hence, some IOP may have been recovered by the volume
of TS weighing down on the vitreous and, through it, on the lens. In
addition, eyes from hatchlings underwent corneal accommodation (data
not shown), which is an IOP-dependent process that requires 15 to 20 mm
Hg IOP.
13 Together, these observations suggest that in
hatchling eyes at least, the minimum IOP criterion was met and that IOP
loss was attenuated. Finally, it should be noted that BVFLs were
measured for the maximum iridial contraction inducible by a
physiological paradigm and that the great amount of accommodation
observed pharmacologically may be an extremely artificial circumstance.
Although the lens is capable of generating the amount of accommodation
observed, this accommodative amplitude may not be observed naturally or
in vivo.
As expected, resting lenticular focal lengths in young chickens
increased as a function of age
(Fig. 1) , presumably in association with
axial elongation of the eye. However, these focal lengths, as reported
herein, are slightly longer than those reported by Priolo et
al.,
9 who examined optical properties of excised chickens
lenses in vitro as a function of age. This difference is probably
attributable to the isolation of the lens in the previous study, where
disruption of the anatomic structures supporting the lens in vivo
causes changes to the shape of the lens. “Rounding up” of the lens
once it has been free of its supporting anatomy has been shown by
Glasser et al.
4 In addition, a more recent and sensitive
version of the scanning laser monitor was used in the current study,
which may have contributed to the differences observed between the two
studies. However, regardless of the differences, the patterns for
lenticular focal length distribution as a function of age between the
two studies are similar.
The finding that the hysteresis effect was significant only in
2-year-old chickens must be interpreted cautiously. It could be an
indication that the lenticular function is detrimentally affected in
older chickens. However, because poststimulus focal lengths were
collected to ensure that stimulation of the ciliary had no optical or
physiologically deleterious effects, the recovery time between the end
of collecting data for stimulated eyes to the beginning of collection
of poststimulus focal lengths was not controlled for. Presumably,
longer recovery times would result in smaller differences between pre-
and poststimulus focal lengths.
An advantage in the use of the physiological in vivo accommodation
model described herein is its usefulness in directly measuring the
effects of age and accommodation on lenticular SA. As reported, after 7
days, lenticular SAs became monotonic
(Figs. 3 4 6) and negative, an
observation that is in keeping with other
reports.
5 9 It should be noted that high amounts
of negative SA may not necessarily result in poor vision, because the
amount and type of SAs at the cornea currently remain unknown. However,
although it is possible that the effects of the cornea may act to
counter the negative SAs observed in the lens, it is less likely that
the erratic nature and clearly high amount of nonmonotonicity exhibited
by hatchling lenses, regardless of accommodative state
(Figs. 3 6) ,
can be compensated for by the cornea. Taken together, the results
indicate that the lens was not fully developed at this age.
It remains unclear whether the high degree of SAs observed in some
stimulated lenses arose because of changes to the shape of the lens or
because of changes to the refractive index of the lens. Given that
focal lengths were measured for “distant” objects (collimated
light), it may be expected that accommodation would be associated with
degradation of optical quality. That the greatest increases in SA and
degree of nonmonotonicity were observed in hatchlings may be related to
the observation that hatchlings also showed the greatest accommodative
ability. Although it must be noted that an age-associated trend was not
observed for SA and nonmonotonicity, SAs in stimulated lenses were
greater than in their unstimulated counterparts in all age groups,
although only significantly so in some of the age groups.
Accommodation in some birds also includes a corneal component. In
chickens and pigeons, changes to the cornea can account for up to half
of the total amount of accommodation,
14 whereas, in hooded
mergansers, lenticular accommodation plays a dominant
role,
2 especially when these diving ducks are in water and
power from the cornea is neutralized. It must be noted that although
the corneal contribution was not measured in this study, the cornea may
play a significant role to chicken vision during accommodation. Whether
its effect on SA, if it exists, is synergistic with the lens during
accommodation, working to improve optical quality, or is deleterious is
not known.
The authors thank Trefford Simpson, Denise King, and Kelley Moran
for helpful discussions; Trevor German, Robin Jones, and Andrew
Nowinski for technical assistance and the staff at the
University of Guelph Poultry Research Center.