Our results show that conventional FDT perimetry is robust to the effects of optical defocus, with 6 D of defocus reducing sensitivity by 0.1 log unit only
(Fig. 1) . Defocus failed to cause a significant decrease in either the PSD or MD index
(Fig. 3) . The shallow, significant slope of approximately 0.23 dB/D, found by Nicolela et al. (Nicolela MT, et al.
IOVS 2002;43:ARVO E-Abstract 2147) for the index MD, provides an acceptable fit for our data (
Q = 1.0). We measured refractive errors centrally, and not for each peripheral location tested, and so it may be thought that the flat curves in
Figure 3 could result from the defocusing lens reducing peripheral defocus in some subjects, while increasing it in others, resulting in little average effect. If this were true, however, the error-bars on the graph would systematically widen as the power of the defocusing lens increased. There is evidence that peripheral retinal contrast sensitivity is more robust to defocus, even once peripheral refractive errors have been corrected,
25 and so it is likely that our foveally measured results represent the maximum effect of defocus for a given retinal location.
Direct comparisons between these results and previous reports investigating the effect of defocus on white-on-white perimetry is difficult, however, because FDT and white-on-white perimetry use different contrast metrics (Michelson versus Weberian contrast, respectively). It has been reported that sensitivity to a conventional size III (0.43°) perimetric target decreases by approximately 0.8 log unit with 6 D of defocus.
26 Given that intersubject variability is only 50% (0.3 log unit) greater for white-on-white perimetry than for FD thresholds,
27 it may be estimated that, relative to normal test variability, white-on-white perimetry is three times (0.5 log unit) more susceptible to the effects of defocus than the commercially available FDT perimeter.
Because there is a noninteger of grating cycles (2.5) used in conventional FDT targets, there is a change in average luminance that modulates at 25 Hz. This luminance artifact arises from the unpaired half cycle and is equal to 2/(5π) of the grating’s peak luminance, thereby making the contrast of the luminance artifact 0.9 log unit below the contrast of the grating. (A half-cycle of a sine-wave grating produces a luminance increase equal to 2/π of the grating’s peak luminance. Averaged over an area of 2.5 cycles [or, 5 half-cycles] in the FDT stimulus, this luminance increase is reduced to 2/[5π] of the grating’s peak luminance.) Contrast thresholds for this artifact alone (
Fig. 2 , circles, no defocus) were approximately 0.1 log unit less than for the FDT grating target (
Fig. 1A , circles, no defocus), and so defocus would need raise grating thresholds approximately 0.8 log unit (0.9 minus 0.1) more than artifact thresholds before the artifact became more detectable than the grating. Although defocus raised grating thresholds at a significantly faster rate than artifact thresholds, the difference was insufficient to make the artifact detectable at the levels of defocus we investigated. Further evidence against the detectability of such an artifact is that detection and resolution thresholds remain comparable at the greatest level of defocus
(Fig. 1A) . Because the luminance artifact does not give information about grating orientation, a divergence in these two thresholds should occur if the artifact was used for stimulus detection. Similar to the conventional FDT targets, the 5° diameter, 0.5 cyc/deg target in this study contained a noninteger of grating cycles and so contained a change in average luminance that modulated at 18 Hz. The differential effect of defocus on the luminance artifact (
Fig 2 ., squares) versus the grating target
(Fig. 1B) was greater than for conventional FDT targets, but was still less than that necessary to make the artifact visible. If reductions in contrast sensitivity are roughly uniform at low spatial frequencies, artifacts should remain invisible in areas of visual field loss caused by disease. Recent work
19 showing that absolute detection thresholds and pattern resolution thresholds are increased by comparable amounts in glaucoma provides evidence that luminance artifacts do not become detectable in areas of visual field damage.
We found that thresholds for higher spatial frequency targets (0.5 cyc/deg), as may be useful in more finely spaced FD test strategies, were more susceptible to defocus than conventional FD perimetry targets
(Fig. 1) . The slopes of the curves relating threshold to defocus were indistinguishable for the 5° and 2° targets, suggesting that it was the spatial frequency of the grating and not the size of the grating that governed this relationship. When higher spatial frequency targets were used in a perimetric test, the MD index became abnormal at high levels of defocus
(Fig. 4) . In our database for the 24-2 FD test, the 5% limit for normality is −2.6 dB (OD), and it would be expected that the average MD would reach this value with 3.5 D of defocus. It should be remembered, however, that defocus less than this amount results in abnormal indices in patients who have MDs less than average, being approximately half of the normal population. Despite the effect on MD, we found no evidence that defocus affects the PSD index in normal subjects, which suggests that defocus results in generalized depression of the visual field. A similar result has been reported for white-on-white perimetry.
28 It may be, however, that defocus affects the PSD in patients with abnormal visual fields, as defocus may make the borders of focal defects less distinct. Studies have found that PSD deteriorates in patients with glaucoma after cataract extraction, despite an improvement in MD,
29 30 which suggests that the reduced image quality in cataract partially masks localized visual field defects. Similarly, the PSD index of abnormal fields is reduced in visual field test strategies that incorporate spatial averaging techniques.
31 32 33
We performed our study on subjects that were younger than those on whom FDT perimetry would typically be conducted, and so it is reasonable to consider how the subject’s age may affect our results. Because pupil diameter decreases with age,
34 this may increase depth of focus for low spatial frequencies,
6 although large intersubject variations in depth of focus effects can occur.
8 Media changes in the eye with age may also ameliorate the effects of defocus, irrespective of pupil size.
35 Overall, older subjects may be less affected by optical defocus than our young test group and so our results probably indicate the maximum effect of defocus on FD perimetry.
In conclusion, we found that the current FDT test is robust to optical defocus, and that defocus does not make low spatial frequency artifacts in the FDT targets visible. With higher spatial frequency targets, as may be useful in test patterns of greater spatial resolution, tolerance to optical defocus is reduced.