April 2005
Volume 46, Issue 4
Free
Clinical and Epidemiologic Research  |   April 2005
Common Visual Defects and Peer Victimization in Children
Author Affiliations
  • Jeremy Horwood
    From the Department of Community Medicine, Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children, University of Bristol, Bristol, United Kingdom; the
  • Andrea Waylen
    From the Department of Community Medicine, Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children, University of Bristol, Bristol, United Kingdom; the
  • David Herrick
    From the Department of Community Medicine, Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children, University of Bristol, Bristol, United Kingdom; the
  • Cathy Williams
    From the Department of Community Medicine, Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children, University of Bristol, Bristol, United Kingdom; the
    Bristol Eye Hospital, Bristol, United Kingdom; and the
  • Dieter Wolke
    From the Department of Community Medicine, Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children, University of Bristol, Bristol, United Kingdom; the
    Jacobs Foundation, Zurich, Switzerland.
Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science April 2005, Vol.46, 1177-1181. doi:https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.04-0597
  • Views
  • PDF
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Jeremy Horwood, Andrea Waylen, David Herrick, Cathy Williams, Dieter Wolke; Common Visual Defects and Peer Victimization in Children. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2005;46(4):1177-1181. https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.04-0597.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract

purpose. To investigate whether wearing glasses, having manifest strabismus, or having a history of wearing an eye patch predisposes preadolescent children to being victimized more frequently at school and whether the impact may be different on boys than on girls.

methods. Data were examined on 6536 children from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) based in the United Kingdom. At 7.5 years, the children undertook a detailed eye examination by orthoptists, including a cover test and visual acuity assessment. At 8.5 years, trained psychologists assessed the children’s bullying involvement as either victim or perpetrator for overt and relational bullying, in a standard interview.

results. Children currently wearing glasses or with a history of wearing eye patches were 35% to 37% more likely to be victims of physical or verbal bullying, even after adjustment for social class and maternal education. No interactions were found between sex and visual problems in the prediction of bullying.

conclusions. For those children who require glasses, opticians should be aware of the risks of bullying, and strategies should be developed and discussed that help reduce their vulnerability.

Bullying victimization occurs when a student is repeatedly exposed to negative actions from other students with the intention to hurt, and it usually involves an imbalance in strength, either real or perceived. 1 It can be overt physical (e.g., hitting), verbal (e.g., name calling) or relational (e.g., social exclusion) 2 3 abuse; becomes moderately stable by early childhood 4 ; and has been found to be associated with psychosocial maladjustment in the victimized, including increased anxiety, depressive feelings, loneliness, lowered self-esteem, and behavioral problems. 5 6 7 Boys experience victimization more often than girls, 8 in particular if they are physically weaker than other boys. 9 For girls, appearance and the lack of close friendships may expose them more to victimization. 2  
Physical characteristics, such as exceptional height or weight, facial disfigurement (e.g., cleft lip and palate), hemiplegic 10 11 12 13 14 15 illnesses such as cancer and epilepsy, have all been found to be associated with teasing 16 17 or persistent victimization in the form of bullying. Visual defects such as strabismus or amblyopia 18 have been suggested to relate to poorer psychosocial functioning, 19 20 poorer interpersonal relationships, and lower self-esteem, 21 22 although these difficulties may improve after correction of strabismus. However, there have been few systematic studies of the adverse psychosocial effects of wearing glasses, although both peers and teachers are reported to attribute more negative characteristics to children with glasses, especially those who are female. 23 24 It is unknown whether negative attitudes toward children who require vision correction or who suffer strabismus result in their more frequent exposure to serious peer rejection such as bullying. 25  
This prospective whole-population study investigated whether wearing glasses, having ever worn an eye patch, or having manifest strabismus predisposes preadolescent children to more frequent victimization and, in particular, whether the impact may be different for boys and girls. 
Methods
Design
We examined data from children of the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) 26 (www.alspac.bris.ac.uk). The cohort consists of children born to residents of the former Avon Health Authority area who had an expected date of delivery between April 1, 1991, and December 31, 1992. Avon includes urban and rural areas, and the population is broadly representative of children in the United Kingdom. 26 The parents have completed regular postal questionnaires about all aspects of their child’s health and development since birth, and, since they were 7.5 years of age, the children have attended annual assessment clinics where they are interviewed and tested. The present study examines data obtained from the children at 7.5 and 8.5 years of age. 
Sample
The ALSPAC sample consists of 14,541 pregnancies that resulted in 14,062 live births, with 13,988 infants still alive at 1 year. Of these, 7,841 children attended the 7.5-year clinic, with 7,599 completing the vision testing session, and 7,171 of the children attended the 8.5-year clinic, with 6,815 returning usable data on bullying victimization. Of the children, 6,536 attended both the 7.5- and 8.5-year clinics. It is recognized that, due to selective withdrawal, this is a biased subsample of the whole cohort (13.4% manual social class compared with 19.5% in the whole cohort). Usable vision and overt victimization data were available for 6,036 of the children and usable vision and relational victimization data for 5,913. 
Procedures
A questionnaire was sent to the parents at the same time as the invitation to attend the ALSPAC 7.5-year assessment clinic. The questionnaire asked whether the child had ever been given patches to wear on one eye (yes/no) and if so, for how many months in total they had worn them. 
At the ALSPAC clinic, an orthoptist asked directly about the child’s wearing glasses and duration of use and performed a detailed examination of each child’s visual status, according to a standard protocol. 27 28 29 Bullying behavior was assessed by trained psychologists at the 8.5-year testing clinic, by a standard interview. 7 Parental social class was assessed from parent questionnaires administered at the birth of each child. 30 Ethical approval was obtained from ALSPAC’s own Ethics Advisory Committee and the three Avon Medical Ethics Committees. Informed consent was obtained from the parents of the children after explanation of the nature of the study. The research adhered to the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki. 
Measures
Vision.
The wearing of glasses was recorded from direct questioning at the 7.5-year assessment and was categorized as follows: no glasses; yes, and brought with them; yes, but not brought with them; yes, previously, but no longer; and not known. If glasses were worn, the amount of time they were worn daily was recorded as either constant (>8 hours); at school (4–8 hours); occasional (2–4 hours); and rarely (0–2 hours). For the statistical analysis, a three-level factor for current frequency of wearing glasses was constructed with levels never, occasionally (occasional or rarely), and frequently (constant wearing or at school). Parents were also asked how many months had passed since the prescription was checked. 
The protocol included a prism cover test at near (33 cm) and far (6 m), both with and without glasses; monocular visual acuity with Early Treatment Diabetic Retinopathy Study (ETDRS) 2000 series LogMAR (logarithm of the minimum angle of resolution) charts (with glasses if worn) and also with a pinhole; and autorefraction, again with and without glasses. Any strabismus was noted as being convergent, divergent, or vertical, and either intermittent or constant. The previously completed questionnaire was checked through with the parents by the orthoptists at the end of the vision-testing session, and any omissions were filled in and areas of uncertainty clarified. Children were classified as strabismic if they had any manifest strabismus in their habitual state (i.e., with glasses, if worn >8 hours a day; otherwise, without glasses) during the clinic visit. The degree of strabismus was described as “small” if the deviation was <20 prism diopters or “large” if >20 prism diopters. 
Children were grouped as having worn patches if their parents reported any history of previous patching at any age. An index of the number of vision problems ranging from 0 (no vision problems) to 3 (eye patching, strabismus and wearing glasses) was constructed. For the statistical analyses, levels 2 and 3 were combined because of the small number of children within individual bullying categories who had all three problems. 
Bullying.
Bullying was assessed with a structured face-to-face interview at the 8.5 year testing clinic, using the Bullying and Friendship Interview Schedule. 7 Researchers explained to children that they were interested in things that had happened in school, or on the way to or from school, during the past 6 months. Children were asked if they had experienced any forms of received overt or relational bullying or if they had used any forms of overt or relational bullying to upset other children (Table 1) . If children had been bullied or had been perpetrators themselves, they were asked how frequently it had occurred. At no time during the interview was the word bullying mentioned. Only behavioral descriptions were used, so as not to prompt the child. All interviewers were fully trained in the measure, and regular checks were made to ensure reliability and avoid any bias. The current paper is concerned solely with the information received about “no bullying involvement” and “victimization” due to overt and relational bullying. 
Parental Social Class and Maternal Education.
Data were obtained on the employment situation of the mother and her partner during pregnancy. This enabled social class categorizations to be derived for the mother and her partner using the 1991 Classification of Operative Procedures (OPCS). 30 Family social class was derived as the higher of the mother’s and partner’s social classes; this was then dichotomized into manual versus nonmanual occupations. 
Information was also obtained during pregnancy on the mother’s educational qualifications 31 and dichotomized into O-level/GCSE (General Certificate of Education; 11 years of education or less) versus A-level or higher (12 or more years of education). 
Family social class and maternal education measures were available for 5621 of the children with usable vision and overt victimization data and 5507 of the children with usable vision and relational victimization data. 
Statistical Analysis
Multiple logistic regression analysis was used to build models to predict (1) overt victimization based on vision category (wearing glasses, having a strabismus, wearing eye patches, and number of defects) and (2) relational victimization based on vision category. Unadjusted and adjusted analyses were performed with child’s sex, maternal education, and family social class corrected for in the latter. The adjusted analyses were repeated excluding those children with other disabilities, including identification of ocular disease, Down syndrome, severe developmental delay, cerebral palsy, known systemic syndrome, illness, or learning difficulties at the 7.5-year vision testing session or because they were reported as having a statement for special educational needs at 7 to 8 years. All statistical analyses were conducted on computer (SPSS, ver. 10.1; SPSS, Chicago, IL). 
Results
Of the 7599 children who completed the vision-testing session, 491 (6.5%) wore glasses frequently, and 185 (2.4%) wore glasses only occasionally. Fifty-seven (0.8%) children had a “large” angle of strabismus and 96 (1.3%) had a “small” angle. Some children (n = 264; 3.5%) had received occlusion treatment for amblyopia. Figure 1shows the overlaps between binary versions of these three vision outcomes in which frequent/occasional glasses wearers were grouped together as were children with large/small angles of strabismus. Of the 6815 children with usable victimization data, 2348 (34.5%) were overt victims. One hundred fifty-two children failed to make sufficient responses in the relational section of the interview to be classified. Of the remainder, 1106 (16.6%) were relational victims. Seven hundred sixteen children were victims of both overt and relational bullying. 
Table 2shows the uncorrected prevalences of victimization due to either overt or relational according to each individual visual category: wearing glasses, strabismus, and having worn an eye patch and also according to number of defects. Unadjusted and adjusted odds ratios (ORs) are shown in Table 3 . It can be seen that, in all visual categories and also for the number of defects, children are most often victims of overt as opposed to relational bullying. There is some evidence for an association between glasses-wearing and overt victimization (P = 0.016) that is not substantially altered on adjustment for sex and socioeconomic status (P = 0.017). Children wearing glasses were more likely to be victimized, with a larger effect for frequent (adjusted OR: 1.35; 95% CI: 1.09–1.69) than occasional (adjusted OR: 1.26; 95% CI: 0.86–1.84) glasses wearers. Similarly, there is some evidence for an association between the number of visual defects and overt victimization (P = 0.006 both with and without adjustment), with children with defects being more likely to be victimized. However, no dose–response relationship was found, the effect size being larger for children with one defect than for those with two or more defects. This is probably due to the construction of the number of defects variable, which is dominated by glasses wearers and children who wore patches (see Fig. 1 ). As an alternative to this approach, strabismus and patching were fitted separately in the model for overt victimization after correction for glasses-wearing. In neither case was the effect significant (P = 0.421 and 0.576, respectively), nor was there any evidence for interactions between the vision defects in predicting overt victimization. Although the uncorrected association between wearing an eye patch and overt victimization was not significant at the 5% level, after adjustment it was (P = 0.048), with patched children being more likely to be victims (adjusted OR: 1.37; 95% CI: 1.01–1.86). 
No significant evidence was found at the 5% level for an association between strabismus and overt victimization or for any associations between the vision defect outcomes and either relational victimization or being a victim of both forms of bullying (P = 0.145). The analysis for strabismus was rerun with only large versus small/no strabismus as an alternative but there was still no statistically significant effect (P = 0.182). Similarly, no evidence was found for interactions between vision defects and sex in predicting overt victimization. The results were not altered after excluding 82 children identified with multiple disabilities from the models for overt victimization and 76 from the models for relational victimization. 
In this age group, when bullying occurs, it is most likely to be overt bullying. However, because a considerable number of children (n = 716) reported that they had experienced both overt and relational bullying, uncorrected prevalences of victimization are provided for orthogonal bullying groups in Table 4 . It can be seen that, for those children who have experienced both types of bullying, there is no suggestion of an increase in bullying in line with more severe visual defects. The trends for those experiencing only overt or only relational are very similar to those in Table 2 , which shows that the significant effects are due to differences in overt but not relational bullying. The logistic model was not repeated for the groups in Table 4 , as adjustment in the previous model made little difference to the estimates. 
Discussion
This prospective study of a large cohort in the United Kingdom found that overall (in line with previous research), approximately a quarter to a third of children reported that they had been the victims of overt bullying. 1 4 5 8 Also in line with previous research was the finding that there was substantial overlap between overt and relational bullying (i.e., a considerable number of children were victims of both). 7 Relatively common visual problems, which necessitated the wearing of glasses or eye patches, were found to increase the risk of overt bullying (either verbal or physical victimization) by ∼35%, even after adjustment for socioeconomic status. Relational bullying did not increase accordingly and nor did combined overt and relational bullying. It may be, therefore, that the wearing of glasses or a patch implies physical weakness to others and so provokes an increase in physical overt bullying as opposed to nonphysical relational victimization. 
Wearing glasses and strabismus were current conditions when the visual status was assessed (a year before the bullying questionnaire was administered), but patching could have occurred at any time previously, making it more difficult to determine mechanisms associated with bullying. It should be noted that usual practice in the local Hospital Eye Service (which provides care for the whole study area) is to terminate patching treatment by 7 to 8 years of age. However these findings indicate that peers not only have more negative attitudes toward children with visual defects, 18 23 24 but that children with such defects are also more likely to be victimized. This is similar to the impact of other deviations in physical appearance reported in previous research. 13 14 17 In our sample of prepubertal children, there were no sex differences in associations between bullying and visual defects (i.e., girls were no more likely to be bullied as a result of wearing glasses or being patched than were boys). There was also no evidence that children with common visual defects were at increased risk for (or indeed were likely to overreport) relational bullying—that is, they did not report that they had experienced more social exclusion than children without visual defects. 
Previously, it has been suggested that interpersonal relationships may be negatively affected in children with strabismus 19 20 21 and bullying has been considered a potential mechanism. We found no evidence that children with small strabismus were at increased risk of victimization by their peers. Children with large strabismus report a prevalence of 40% being victimized, which is higher than those without strabismus and is similar to the increased likelihood of victimization for those who wear glasses frequently (40%) or those with eyes patched for amblyopia (40%). However, even in this large sample, the number of children with large strabismus was far smaller than that of children with glasses or a history of patching. This reflects the reduced statistical power associated with smaller numbers and may explain why the relevant data are not statistically significant. An alternative explanation for the psychosocial disadvantages perceived by individuals with strabismus as opposed to other common visual defects 19 20 may be that abnormalities in “eye contact” 32 interfere with communication skills and the normal development of relationships. 
It is acknowledged that there may be other social and physical factors associated with bullying involvement that have not been accounted for in the present study—for example, changes in social class between birth and later childhood, changes in visual status between 7.5 and 8.5 years, age at the end of patching, ethnicity, other physical characteristics, or intelligence. Future work is needed to investigate their potential influence further. 
In summary, wearing glasses and having a history of eye patching at any time were both associated with a small increase in the perception of being bullied at the age of 8.5 years. These results may provide support for groups aiming to promote preschool screening as a way of diagnosing and treating children with glasses or patching before they start attending school so that, at the time when compliance is most crucial, they are less likely to be exposed to a large number of older children and the attendant risks of bullying. 33 However, for children who require glasses at this age to see clearly or to maintain their ocular alignment, strategies to reduce vulnerability to bullying (other than failure to comply with the treatment) should be devised and promoted, to maximize both visual health and the children’s well-being. Opticians should discuss these problems with parents and possibly provide written information about how to combat bullying victimization. 8 9  
 
Table 1.
 
Forms of Overt and Relational Victimization
Table 1.
 
Forms of Overt and Relational Victimization
Overt victimization Having had belongings stolen; having been threatened or blackmailed; having been hit or beaten up; having been called bad/nasty names; having had nasty tricks played on them.
Relational victimization Other children not wanting to play with them; trying to get them to do something they did not want to do; withdrawing friendship; telling tales on them; spreading lies or nasty rumors; deliberately spoiling games; doing other things to upset them.
Figure 1.
 
Venn diagram for vision outcomes.
Figure 1.
 
Venn diagram for vision outcomes.
Table 2.
 
Uncorrected Prevalences of Victimization within Categories of Vision Defects
Table 2.
 
Uncorrected Prevalences of Victimization within Categories of Vision Defects
Overt Victimization, % (n) Relational Victimization, % (n)
No (n = 3730) Yes (n = 1891) Total (n = 5621) No (n = 4635) Yes (n = 872) Total (n = 5507)
Wears glasses P = 0.015 P = 0.721
 Frequently 59.8 (213) 40.2 (143) 356 85.6 (297) 14.4 (50) 347
 Occasionally 62.1 (72) 37.9 (44) 116 85.1 (97) 14.9 (17) 114
 Never 66.9 (3445) 33.1 (1704) 5149 84.0 (4241) 16.0 (805) 5046
Strabismus P = 0.688 P = 0.057
 Large (>20 pd) 60.0 (24) 40.0 (16) 40 78.4 (29) 21.6 (8) 37
 Small (<20 pd) 67.2 (41) 32.8 (20) 61 94.7 (54) 5.3 (3) 57
 None 66.4 (3665) 33.6 (1855) 5520 84.1 (4552) 15.9 (861) 5413
Ever worn a patch P = 0.051 P = 0.817
 Yes 59.6 (106) 40.4 (72) 178 83.5 (142) 16.5 (28) 170
 No 66.6 (3624) 33.4 (1819) 5443 84.2 (4493) 15.8 (844) 5337
Number of defects P = 0.006 P = 0.453
 Two or more 61.5 (96) 38.5 (60) 156 83.7 (123) 16.3 (24) 147
 One 59.8 (232) 40.2 (156) 388 86.4 (331) 13.6 (52) 383
 None 67.0 (3402) 33.0 (1675) 5077 84.0 (4181) 16.0 (796) 4977
Table 3.
 
ORs for Victimization within Categories of Vision Defects
Table 3.
 
ORs for Victimization within Categories of Vision Defects
Overt Victimization Relational Victimization
Unadjusted OR (95% CI) Adjusted OR (95% CI) Unadjusted OR (95% CI) Adjusted OR (95% CI)
Wears glasses P = 0.016 P = 0.017 P = 0.715 P = 0.662
 Frequently 1.36 (1.09–1.69) 1.35 (1.09–1.69) 0.89 (0.65–1.21) 0.88 (0.64–1.19)
 Occasionally 1.24 (0.85–1.81) 1.26 (0.86–1.84) 0.92 (0.55–1.55) 0.91 (0.54–1.53)
 Never 1.00 Reference 1.00 Reference 1.00 Reference 1.00 Reference
Strabismus P = 0.669 P = 0.746 P = 0.250 P = 0.261
 Yes 1.09 (0.73–1.65) 1.07 (0.71–1.62) 0.70 (0.37–1.32) 0.71 (0.37–1.33)
 No 1.00 Reference 1.00 Reference 1.00 Reference 1.00 Reference
Ever worn a patch P = 0.054 P = 0.048 P = 0.818 P = 0.822
 Yes 1.35 (1.00–1.84) 1.37 (1.01–1.86) 1.05 (0.70–1.58) 1.05 (0.69–1.58)
 No 1.00 Reference 1.00 Reference 1.00 Reference 1.00 Reference
Number of defects P = 0.007 P = 0.006 P = 0.439 P = 0.386
 Two or more 1.27 (0.91–1.76) 1.25 (0.90–1.75) 1.02 (0.66–1.60) 1.02 (0.66–1.60)
 One 1.37 (1.11–1.69) 1.39 (1.12–1.71) 0.83 (0.61–1.12) 0.81 (0.60–1.10)
 None 1.00 Reference 1.00 Reference 1.00 Reference 1.00 Reference
Table 4.
 
Uncorrected Prevalences of Orthogonal Victimization Groups within Categories of Vision Defects
Table 4.
 
Uncorrected Prevalences of Orthogonal Victimization Groups within Categories of Vision Defects
Victimization Status Total (N = 5507)
None (n = 3356) Overt Only (n = 1279) Relational Only (n = 313) Both (n = 559)
Wears glasses P = 0.010
 Frequently 53.9 (187) 31.7 (110) 5.8 (20) 8.6 (30) 347
 Occasionally 57.9 (66) 27.2 (31) 4.4 (5) 10.5 (12) 114
 Never 61.5 (3103) 22.6 (1138) 5.7 (288) 10.2 (517) 5046
Strabismus P = 0.145
 Large (≥20 pd) 51.4 (19) 27.0 (10) 10.8 (4) 10.8 (4) 37
 Small (<20 pd) 61.4 (35) 33.3 (19) 3.5 (2) 1.8 (1) 57
 None 61.0 (3302) 23.1 (1250) 5.7 (307) 10.2 (554) 5413
Ever worn a patch P = 0.085
 Yes 52.9 (90) 30.6 (52) 7.1 (12) 9.4 (16) 170
 No 61.2 (3266) 23.0 (1227) 5.6 (301) 10.2 (543) 5337
Number of defects P = 0.003
 Two or more 53.7 (79) 29.9 (44) 7.5 (11) 8.8 (13) 147
 One 55.6 (213) 30.8 (118) 4.4 (17) 9.1 (35) 383
 None 61.6 (3064) 22.4 (1117) 5.7 (285) 10.3 (511) 4977
The authors thank all the mothers who took part and the midwives for their cooperation and help in recruitment; the whole ALSPAC study team, comprising interviewers, computer technicians, laboratory technicians, clerical workers, research scientists, volunteers, and managers who continue to make the study possible. 
WhitneyI, SmithPK. A survey of the nature and extent of bullying in junior/middle and secondary schools. Educ Res. 1993;35:3–25. [CrossRef]
BjörkqvistK. Sex Differences in physical, verbal, and indirect aggression: a review of recent research. Sex Roles. 1994;30:177–188. [CrossRef]
CrickNR, GrotpeterJK. Relational aggression, gender, and social-psychological adjustment. Child Development. 1995;66:710–722. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
SmithP, AnaniadouK. The nature of school bullying and the effectiveness of school-based interventions. J Appl Psychoanal Studies. 2003;5:189–209. [CrossRef]
WolkeD, WoodsS, SchulzH, StanfordK. Bullying and victimization of primary school children in South England and South Germany: prevalence and school factors. Br J Psychol. 2001;92:673–696. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
HawkerD, BoultonM. Twenty years research on peer victimization and psychosocial maladjustment: a meta-analytic review of cross-sectional studies. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2000;41:441–455. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
WolkeD, WoodsS, BloomfieldL, KarstadtL. The association between direct and relational bullying and behaviour. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2000;41:989–1002. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
WolkeD, StanfordK. Bullying in school children.MesserD MillarS eds. Developmental Psychology. 1999;341–360.Arnold London.
OlweusD. Annotation: Bullying at school: basic facts and effects of a school based intervention program. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1994;35:1171–1190. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
CrozierWR, DimmockP. Name calling and nicknames in a sample of primary school children. Br J Educ Psychol. 1999;69:505–516. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
LahteenmakiP, HuostilaJ, HinkkaS, SalmiT. Childhood cancer patients at school. Eur J Cancer. 2002;38:1227–1240. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
VanceY, EiserC. The school experience of the child with cancer. Child Care Health Dev. 2002;28:5–19. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Neumark-SztainerD, FalknerN, StoryM, PerryC, HannanPJ, MulertS. Weight-teasing among adolescents: correlations with weight status and disordered eating behaviors. Int J Obesity. 2002;26:123–131. [CrossRef]
PearceMJ, BoergersJ, PrinsteinMJ. Adolescent obesity, overt and relational peer victimization, and romantic relationships. Obesity Res. 2002;10:386–393. [CrossRef]
GeorgesonJ, HarrisM, MilichR, YoungJ. “Just teasing”: personality effects on perceptions and life narratives of childhood teasing. Personality Soc Psychol Bull. 1999;25:1254–1267. [CrossRef]
KowalskiR. “I was only kidding!”: victims’ and perpetrators’ perceptions of teasing. Personality Soc Psychol Bull. 2000;26:231–241. [CrossRef]
KeltnerD, CappsL, KringA, YoungR, HeeryE. Just teasing: a conceptual analysis and empirical review. Psychol Bull. 2001;127:229–248. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
PackwoodEA, CruzOA, RychwalskiPJ, KeechRV. The psychosocial effects of amblyopia study. J AAPOS. 1999;3:15–17. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
UretmenO, EgrilmezS, KoseS, PamaukçuK, AkkinC, PalamarM. Negative social bias against children with strabismus. Acta Ophthalmol Scand. 2003;81:138–142. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
OlitskySE, SudeshS, GrazianoA, HamblenJ, BrooksSE, ShahaSH. The negative psychosocial impact of strabismus in adults. J AAPOS. 1999;3:209–211. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
MenonV, SahaJ, TandonR, MehtaM, KhokharS. Study of the psychosocial aspects of strabismus. J Pediatr Ophthalmol Strabismus. 2002;39:203–208. [PubMed]
MruthyunjayaP, SimonJW, PickeringJD, LiningerLL. Subjective and objective outcomes of strabismus surgery in children. J Pediatr Ophthalmol Strabismus. 1996;33:167–170. [PubMed]
TerryRL, MacyRJ. Childrens social judgments of other children who wear eyeglasses. J Soc Behav Personality. 1991;6:965–974.
TerryRL, StocktonLA. Eyeglasses and childrens schemata. J Soc Psychol. 1993;133:425–438. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
SatterfieldD, KeltnerJL, MorrisonTL. Psychosocial aspects of strabismus study. Arch Ophthalmol. 1993;111:1100–1105. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
GoldingJ, PembreyM, JonesR. ALSPAC: the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. I. Study methodology. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. 2001;15:74–87. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
WilliamsC, HarradR, HarveyI, SparrowJ, TeamAS. Screening for amblyopia in preschool children: results of a population-based, randomised controlled trial. Ophthalmic Epidemiol. 2001;8:279–295. [PubMed]
WilliamsC, NorthstoneK, HarradR, SparrowJ, HarveyI,ALSPAC Study Team. Amblyopia treatment outcomes after screening before 3 years vs. at 3 years of age: follow-up from a randomised trial. BMJ. 2002;324:1549–1551. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
WilliamsC, NorthsoneK, HarradR, SparrowJ, TeamAS. Amblyopia treatment outcomes after screening at 3 vs. screening at school entry: observational data from a prospective cohort study. Br J Ophthalmol. 2003;87:988–993. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Office of Population Censuses and Surveys. Standard Occupational Classification. 1991;Her Majesty’s Stationery Office London.
OsbornA, ButlerN, MorrisA. The Social Life of Britain’s Five Year Olds: A Report of the Child Health and Education Study. 1984;Routledge & Kegan London.
ArlinghausS. Eye-contact graphs. Behav Sci. 1985;30:108–117. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
HorwoodA. Compliance with first time spectacle wear in children under eight years of age. Eye. 1998;12:173–178. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1.
 
Venn diagram for vision outcomes.
Figure 1.
 
Venn diagram for vision outcomes.
Table 1.
 
Forms of Overt and Relational Victimization
Table 1.
 
Forms of Overt and Relational Victimization
Overt victimization Having had belongings stolen; having been threatened or blackmailed; having been hit or beaten up; having been called bad/nasty names; having had nasty tricks played on them.
Relational victimization Other children not wanting to play with them; trying to get them to do something they did not want to do; withdrawing friendship; telling tales on them; spreading lies or nasty rumors; deliberately spoiling games; doing other things to upset them.
Table 2.
 
Uncorrected Prevalences of Victimization within Categories of Vision Defects
Table 2.
 
Uncorrected Prevalences of Victimization within Categories of Vision Defects
Overt Victimization, % (n) Relational Victimization, % (n)
No (n = 3730) Yes (n = 1891) Total (n = 5621) No (n = 4635) Yes (n = 872) Total (n = 5507)
Wears glasses P = 0.015 P = 0.721
 Frequently 59.8 (213) 40.2 (143) 356 85.6 (297) 14.4 (50) 347
 Occasionally 62.1 (72) 37.9 (44) 116 85.1 (97) 14.9 (17) 114
 Never 66.9 (3445) 33.1 (1704) 5149 84.0 (4241) 16.0 (805) 5046
Strabismus P = 0.688 P = 0.057
 Large (>20 pd) 60.0 (24) 40.0 (16) 40 78.4 (29) 21.6 (8) 37
 Small (<20 pd) 67.2 (41) 32.8 (20) 61 94.7 (54) 5.3 (3) 57
 None 66.4 (3665) 33.6 (1855) 5520 84.1 (4552) 15.9 (861) 5413
Ever worn a patch P = 0.051 P = 0.817
 Yes 59.6 (106) 40.4 (72) 178 83.5 (142) 16.5 (28) 170
 No 66.6 (3624) 33.4 (1819) 5443 84.2 (4493) 15.8 (844) 5337
Number of defects P = 0.006 P = 0.453
 Two or more 61.5 (96) 38.5 (60) 156 83.7 (123) 16.3 (24) 147
 One 59.8 (232) 40.2 (156) 388 86.4 (331) 13.6 (52) 383
 None 67.0 (3402) 33.0 (1675) 5077 84.0 (4181) 16.0 (796) 4977
Table 3.
 
ORs for Victimization within Categories of Vision Defects
Table 3.
 
ORs for Victimization within Categories of Vision Defects
Overt Victimization Relational Victimization
Unadjusted OR (95% CI) Adjusted OR (95% CI) Unadjusted OR (95% CI) Adjusted OR (95% CI)
Wears glasses P = 0.016 P = 0.017 P = 0.715 P = 0.662
 Frequently 1.36 (1.09–1.69) 1.35 (1.09–1.69) 0.89 (0.65–1.21) 0.88 (0.64–1.19)
 Occasionally 1.24 (0.85–1.81) 1.26 (0.86–1.84) 0.92 (0.55–1.55) 0.91 (0.54–1.53)
 Never 1.00 Reference 1.00 Reference 1.00 Reference 1.00 Reference
Strabismus P = 0.669 P = 0.746 P = 0.250 P = 0.261
 Yes 1.09 (0.73–1.65) 1.07 (0.71–1.62) 0.70 (0.37–1.32) 0.71 (0.37–1.33)
 No 1.00 Reference 1.00 Reference 1.00 Reference 1.00 Reference
Ever worn a patch P = 0.054 P = 0.048 P = 0.818 P = 0.822
 Yes 1.35 (1.00–1.84) 1.37 (1.01–1.86) 1.05 (0.70–1.58) 1.05 (0.69–1.58)
 No 1.00 Reference 1.00 Reference 1.00 Reference 1.00 Reference
Number of defects P = 0.007 P = 0.006 P = 0.439 P = 0.386
 Two or more 1.27 (0.91–1.76) 1.25 (0.90–1.75) 1.02 (0.66–1.60) 1.02 (0.66–1.60)
 One 1.37 (1.11–1.69) 1.39 (1.12–1.71) 0.83 (0.61–1.12) 0.81 (0.60–1.10)
 None 1.00 Reference 1.00 Reference 1.00 Reference 1.00 Reference
Table 4.
 
Uncorrected Prevalences of Orthogonal Victimization Groups within Categories of Vision Defects
Table 4.
 
Uncorrected Prevalences of Orthogonal Victimization Groups within Categories of Vision Defects
Victimization Status Total (N = 5507)
None (n = 3356) Overt Only (n = 1279) Relational Only (n = 313) Both (n = 559)
Wears glasses P = 0.010
 Frequently 53.9 (187) 31.7 (110) 5.8 (20) 8.6 (30) 347
 Occasionally 57.9 (66) 27.2 (31) 4.4 (5) 10.5 (12) 114
 Never 61.5 (3103) 22.6 (1138) 5.7 (288) 10.2 (517) 5046
Strabismus P = 0.145
 Large (≥20 pd) 51.4 (19) 27.0 (10) 10.8 (4) 10.8 (4) 37
 Small (<20 pd) 61.4 (35) 33.3 (19) 3.5 (2) 1.8 (1) 57
 None 61.0 (3302) 23.1 (1250) 5.7 (307) 10.2 (554) 5413
Ever worn a patch P = 0.085
 Yes 52.9 (90) 30.6 (52) 7.1 (12) 9.4 (16) 170
 No 61.2 (3266) 23.0 (1227) 5.6 (301) 10.2 (543) 5337
Number of defects P = 0.003
 Two or more 53.7 (79) 29.9 (44) 7.5 (11) 8.8 (13) 147
 One 55.6 (213) 30.8 (118) 4.4 (17) 9.1 (35) 383
 None 61.6 (3064) 22.4 (1117) 5.7 (285) 10.3 (511) 4977
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×