All FTs for one volunteer are shown as scatter plots in
Figure 1for the conditions (A) full visual field, high-contrast stimuli, (B) central visual field, low-contrast stimuli, (C) chromatic red/green stimuli (cone-mediated filling-in), and (D) dark-adapted scotopic stimuli (rod-mediated filling-in). For conditions A to C, FT decreased with increasing eccentricity regardless of direction. In addition, reduced scatter of the points tended to be associated with increasing eccentricity. The volunteer could not fill-in the target at zero position (i.e., direct fixation) in the allotted time for chromatic stimuli
(Fig. 1C)but could for conditions A and B. The presence of numerous outliers in the data, usually because of extended FTs, was assumed to result from blinks and unstable fixation, leading to resetting of the filling-in mechanism. For this reason median values were used as an average FT measure for each volunteer. Because median values of each volunteer approximated a normal distribution, parametric statistics were performed and means and standard errors (of median values) were used to describe the data.
Mean FTs for all participants (± SEM) are compared with the mean CS
w measures (±SEM) for luminance-defined stimuli at various points across ±20° of the visual field in
Figure 2and across the central ±5° of the visual field in
Figure 3 . Fading time and CS
w were consistently higher along the horizontal meridian than the vertical meridian at equivalent eccentricities in the peripheral and the central fields
(Figs. 2A 3A) . This was confirmed statistically, for FT (F = 31.8;
P = 0.0005) and CS
w (F = 16.4;
P = 0.004) of full-field, high-contrast stimuli (eccentricities 5°, 10°, and 20° summed) and for FT (F = 7.2;
P = 0.03) and CS
w (F = 13.5;
P = 0.01) of central field low-contrast stimuli (eccentricities 1.25°, 2.5°, and 3.75° summed). Three subjects could not fill-in the high-contrast target at 0°, and one subject could not fill-in any 2.5° target within the 120-second time limit.
For central field, low-contrast stimuli, the anisotropy of FT was greater nearer the fovea (e.g., 1.25°). There was a dip at 0° eccentricity where FT was less than it was at adjacent 1.25° eccentricities. There were no significant differences between inferior and superior hemimeridians or between temporal and nasal hemimeridians for FT or CS
w (inferior versus superior,
P = 1.0 and
P = 1.0 for FT and
P = 0.07 and
P = 1.0 for CS
w; and temporal versus nasal,
P = 1.0 and
P = 1.0 for FT and
P = 0.11 and
P = 0.60 for CS
w, for full-field, high-contrast stimuli and central field, low-contrast stimuli, respectively). Log FT and CS
w were strongly correlated with full-field, high-contrast stimuli (
Fig. 2B ;
r 2 = 0.93,
P = 10
−9) and central field low-contrast stimuli (
Fig. 3B ;
r 2 = 0.84;
P = 10
−6). For full-field, high-contrast stimuli, the 0° central point was in accord with the trend for the rest of data, whereas for central field low-contrast stimuli, the point had a lower log FT than data with equivalent CS
w. This point was not included in the correlation.
Points common to full-field and central-field experiments (2.5° and 5° eccentricities) show that anisotropy of horizontal and vertical fields for FT and CSw were consistent for the two different levels of contrast tested. For example, mean FTs at 2.5° eccentricity, for horizontal and vertical meridians, respectively, were 35.5 seconds and 28.9 seconds for high-contrast stimuli and 13.7 seconds and 11.4 seconds for low-contrast stimuli. For 5° eccentricity, they were 18.4 seconds and 12.0 seconds for high-contrast stimuli and 10.5 and 8.7s for low-contrast stimuli.
Comparison between cone-mediated FTs and cone density distributions described by Curcio et al.
10 are shown in
Figure 4 . As for luminance-defined stimuli, consistent anisotropy between horizontal and vertical fields was evident, with the horizontal field taking longer to fill-in (F = 10.5;
P = 0.01; eccentricities 5°, 10°, and 20° summed). Curcio et al.
10 also describe larger cone densities along the horizontal meridian compared with the vertical meridian at all eccentricities. Although mean FTs appeared elevated for the nasal field compared with the temporal field, there were no significant differences between temporal and nasal hemimeridians (
P = 0.55) or between inferior and superior hemimeridians (
P = 0.99).
In a number of volunteers, filling-in of some central targets could not be accomplished under chromatic conditions in the allotted time. The number of participants in whom median FT values were greater than 120 seconds (where 120 seconds is the maximum duration of the target) was 8 subjects for 0° and 3 subjects for 2.5° temporal, 2 subjects for 2.5° nasal, and 2 subjects for 2.5° superior and 2 subjects for 2.5° inferior. Consequently, the FT at zero eccentricity is not plotted in
Figure 4Aor correlated with cone density
(Fig. 4B) . The relationship between FT and cone density was approximately linear, with a strong positive correlation (
Fig. 4B ;
r 2 = 0.94;
P = 10
−8).
Fading times were more even across the visual field under dark-adapted conditions compared with luminance-defined and chromatic stimuli
(Fig. 5A) . There were no statistically significant differences between horizontal and vertical meridians (F = 1.03;
P = 0.34) or between temporal and nasal hemimeridians (F = 4.2;
P = 0.08); however, the inferior hemimeridian took significantly longer to fill-in than the superior hemimeridian (F = 11.4;
P = 0.01). The relationship between FT and rod density was nonlinear
(Fig. 5B) . The relationship between FT and rod density varied in a predictable way with eccentricity, as shown by
Figure 5C , which demonstrates that the ratio of FT/rod density decreased with increasing eccentricity. Note that the
x-axis of
Figure 5Cis a logarithmic scale. The Weber contrast of the spot used during dark-adapted filling-in is shown in
Figure 5Dfor one volunteer in relation to changes in the detection threshold over the course of the dark adaptation process. A rod-cone break is obvious for the 5° target and, to a lesser degree, for the 20° target, possibly because of the blue spot–stimulating S-cones in the peripheral retina. The contrast of the spot is below the rod-cone break and near the threshold of detection for the 5° target. However, all volunteers saw the appearance and slow fading of all targets presented at each retinal location.
Stability of fixation, assessed using BCEA measurements of the eye movement recording data, was not considerably different when fixating either a spot or a cross
(Fig. 6A) . Rather, intersubject variability was much higher than intrasubject variability between the two tasks. There were no obvious differences in BCEA or FT during central and peripheral filling-in with or without the addition of a distracter
(Fig. 6B) . One subject showed large BCEAs when fixating a central target with or without the distracter (same subject) and also extended fading times (>50 s) compared with other volunteers fixating central targets. The same volunteer also showed long FTs compared with the other volunteers during the main experiments, when filling-in high- and low-contrast luminance-defined targets within the central 5°. However, FTs in the periphery during these trials were normal for this volunteer, as were cone-mediated and rod-mediated FTs. A separate statistical analysis was performed excluding this volunteer. Given that this had little effect on the outcome, data of this volunteer were left in the statistical analysis and graphical data.
BCEAs recorded from one subject during filling-in of targets at 5° and 10° eccentricity (0.251 deg
2 and 0.226 deg
2 , respectively) under dark-adapted conditions were similar to those recorded under light conditions (e.g., 0.258 deg
2 when filling-in a target at 10°).
Numbers of blinks recorded for the four volunteers were 7.6, 0.3, 1.6, and 2.2 blinks/min when directly viewing a spot and 7.7, 0.3, 3.3, and 0.9 blinks/min, respectively, when directly viewing a cross. During attempted filling-in of a central target, the effect of a distracter was to increase the number of blinks in three of the four subjects (1.2, 1.4, 4.9, 7.8 blinks/min increasing to 7.2, 0.3, 17.7, 14.0 blinks/min with the distracter). During attempted filling-in of a peripheral target, the effect of a distracter was less evident (4.1, 1.1, 0.0, 17.8 blinks/min increasing to 5.5, 0.0, 7.3, 9.9 blinks/min with the distracter). This shows that the effect of the distracter on blinks was related to being near the fixation target rather than the filling-in target.