Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science Cover Image for Volume 45, Issue 7
July 2004
Volume 45, Issue 7
Free
Visual Neuroscience  |   July 2004
Dark Adaptation of Rod Photoreceptors in Normal Subjects, and in Patients with Stargardt Disease and an ABCA4 Mutation
Author Affiliations
  • Jennifer J. Kang Derwent
    From the Departments of Ophthalmology and Visual Sciences and
    Pritzker Institute of Biomedical Science and Engineering, Illinois Institute of Technology, Chicago, Illinois; the
  • Deborah J. Derlacki
    From the Departments of Ophthalmology and Visual Sciences and
  • John R. Hetling
    From the Departments of Ophthalmology and Visual Sciences and
    Bioengineering, University of Illinois at Chicago, Chicago, Illinois; the
  • Gerald A. Fishman
    From the Departments of Ophthalmology and Visual Sciences and
  • David G. Birch
    Retina Foundation of the Southwest, Dallas, Texas; the
  • Sandeep Grover
    From the Departments of Ophthalmology and Visual Sciences and
  • Edwin M. Stone
    Department of Ophthalmology and Visual Science, The Carver College of Medicine and the Howard Hughes Medical Institute, University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa.
  • David R. Pepperberg
    From the Departments of Ophthalmology and Visual Sciences and
Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science July 2004, Vol.45, 2447-2456. doi:https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.03-1178
  • Views
  • PDF
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Jennifer J. Kang Derwent, Deborah J. Derlacki, John R. Hetling, Gerald A. Fishman, David G. Birch, Sandeep Grover, Edwin M. Stone, David R. Pepperberg; Dark Adaptation of Rod Photoreceptors in Normal Subjects, and in Patients with Stargardt Disease and an ABCA4 Mutation. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2004;45(7):2447-2456. https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.03-1178.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract

purpose. Psychophysical and electroretinographic (ERG) studies indicate that patients with Stargardt disease exhibit abnormally slow rod dark adaptation after illumination that bleaches a substantial fraction of rhodopsin. However, relatively little information is available concerning rod recovery in this disease after weaker adapting (i.e., conditioning) light. With the use of a paired-flash ERG method, properties of the derived rod response to a low-bleach (<1%) but rod-saturating conditioning flash were investigated in seven normal subjects and in five Stargardt patients with identified sequence variations in the ABCA4 gene.

methods. In the first of two experiments, the interval between a fixed conditioning flash (67 or 670 scotopic cd s m−2) and a bright probe flash of fixed strength was varied to determine the falling-phase kinetics of the derived rod response to the conditioning flash. In the second, the instantaneous amplitude-intensity function for the rod response at an intermediate stage of recovery from the conditioning flash was determined by presenting a test flash of various strengths at a fixed time after the conditioning flash, and a probe flash at 200 ms after the test flash.

results. The maximum peak amplitude of the dark-adapted, rod-mediated a-wave determined in Stargardt patients (211 ± 87 μV) was on average lower than that determined in normal subjects (325 ± 91 μV; P = 0.06). The derived rod response to the 670 scotopic cd s m−2 conditioning flash determined in normal subjects and Stargardt patients exhibited a biphasic recovery, and the kinetics of the early stage of this recovery were similar in the two subject groups. For both normal subjects and patients, normalized amplitude-intensity functions describing the dark-adapted derived rod response exhibited half-saturation at approximately 1.5 log scotopic troland second. In both groups, the normalized amplitude-intensity function determined at approximately 2 seconds after the 67 scotopic cd s m−2 conditioning flash and at approximately 9 seconds after the 670 scotopic cd s m−2 conditioning flash exhibited an average desensitization (i.e., an increase of test flash strength at half-saturation) of approximately 0.5 to 0.6 log unit relative to that determined under dark-adapted conditions.

conclusions. The results indicate that, despite a reduction in the average dark-adapted maximum a-wave amplitude in the Stargardt/ABCA4 patients, the early-stage recovery kinetics of the derived rod response to a low-bleaching conditioning flash as well as the lingering rod desensitization produced by such a flash are similar to those determined in normal subjects.

Stargardt disease is characterized by progressive loss in central vision and bilateral atrophic-appearing macular changes surrounded by yellowish-white fundus lesions at the level of the retinal pigment epithelium (RPE). 1 2 3 Recent studies have shown that Stargardt disease is caused by sequence variations in the gene encoding ABCA4 (previously known as ABCR), a retina-specific protein expressed in rod and cone photoreceptors. 4 5 The ABCA4 gene product is an active transporter that in rods facilitates the movement of all-trans retinal, the retinoid product of rhodopsin bleaching, from the lumen of the rod outer segment disc to the rod cytosol. 6 7 8 9 10 This translocation and the subsequent enzymatic reduction of all-trans retinal terminate its ability to combine with opsin, and thus promote the shut-off of excitation in the phototransduction cascade 11 12 (for reviews, see Refs. 13 , 14 ). Consistent with this action of the ABCA4 transporter, many patients with Stargardt disease and ABCA4-associated cone–rod dystrophy require an abnormally long period to dark adapt fully, after adapting illumination that bleaches a major portion of their rhodopsin. 1 15 16 17 18 19 Similarly, the abcr knockout mouse, which lacks the ABCA4 gene product, exhibits abnormally slow recovery of the electroretinographic (ERG) a-wave after substantial rhodopsin bleaching. 20  
A question of interest raised by the available data is whether the prolongation of dark adaptation in ABCA4 Stargardt patients occurs only with bleachings that generate a substantial amount of all-trans retinal photoproduct. The present study was undertaken to compare rod flash sensitivity after weak bleaching illumination in normal subjects and in Stargardt patients with identified ABCA4 mutations, using a paired-flash electroretinographic (ERG) method that allows determination of the instantaneous amplitude-intensity function at defined times after a conditioning flash. 21 22 23 Preliminary results have been reported (Pepperberg DR, et al. IOVS 2001;42:ARVO Abstract 4203). 
Methods
All procedures were in accordance with institutional policies and with the principles embodied in the Declaration of Helsinki. The experiments were conducted with the understanding and written consent of each subject. Before enrollment in the study, each subject underwent an ophthalmic examination that included subject history; determination of best-corrected visual acuity; slit lamp examination of the anterior chamber, lens, and vitreous; determination of intraocular pressure; and fundus examination. Stargardt patients were screened for sequence variation in the ABCA4 gene, using previously described methods. 3 24 Two of the Stargardt patients (subjects 107 and 109) exhibited localized fundus flecks in the parafoveal region; three additional patients (subjects 106, 108, and 110) exhibited more diffuse flecks. These two subgroups of patients would have been classified as having disease at stages 1 and 2, respectively, as previously described. 2  
Electroretinography
Before each experiment, the pupil of the eye to be tested was dilated with 2.5% phenylephrine HCl and 1% tropicamide. An opaque patch was placed over the eye for 45 to 60 minutes to allow for full dark adaptation. Under dim red light, the patch was removed and the corneal surface anesthetized with 0.5% proparacaine HCl. ERGs were recorded with the use of a bipolar electrode (GoldLens; Diagnosys, LLC, Littleton, MA) positioned on the corneal surface. The corneal surface was lubricated with several drops of methylcellulose (Celluvisc; Allergan, Irvine, CA) placed on the electrode surface immediately before its installation. A ground electrode (Model E34D; Grass-Telefactor, West Warwick, RI) was clipped to the ipsilateral ear. The subject was seated at a Ganzfeld dome in a dark room and asked to focus on a small red fixation light positioned axially in the dome. 
A short-wavelength (blue) flash that preferentially stimulated rod photoreceptors (Wratten 47B filter, λmax = 449 nm; Eastman Kodak Co., Rochester, NY) was used as a test flash. The test flash was provided by a Grass photic stimulator (model PS33; Grass-Telefactor). The test flashes ranged in strength from 0.07 to 7.12 scotopic (sc) cd s m−2. The probe flash, provided by a Novatron flash unit (model 2105-C flash unit and 1000 VR power pack; Novatron Inc., Dallas, TX), was a short-wavelength flash (Wratten 47B filter; Eastman Kodak) that ranged in strength from 302 to 2400 sc cd s m−2. A long-wavelength (red) flash (λcut-on = 605 nm; Wratten 26, Eastman Kodak) of intensity photopically matched to the short-wavelength probe was used for determination of the cone contribution to the nominal probe flash response (described later). A second Novatron unit provided the bright, short-wavelength conditioning flash. Light from both the two Novatron flash lamps and the Grass flash lamp passed through heat filters (catalog no. 66.2450; Rolyn Optics Co., Covina, CA) and was attenuated with neutral density filters (Wratten; Saunders Group, Rochester, NY). Flash strengths were calibrated using an integrating photometer (model 1700 with a model SED033 detector, radiance barrel and ZCIE scotopic filter; International Light, Newburyport, MA). Strengths of the conditioning flashes were 67 and 670 sc cd s m−2. Pupil diameters determined for the seven normal subjects tested were in the range of approximately 7.5 to 10 mm, and those for the five Stargardt ABCA4 patients ranged from approximately 7.5 to 9 mm. Determinations of pupil diameter typically were rounded to the nearest millimeter. The conversion of flash luminance L (in sc cd s m−2) to retinal illuminance I (in scotopic troland second [sc td s]) through the relation, I = α L (where α is the pupil area, in square millimeters) was assumed to be the same in normal subjects and Stargardt patients. Equating 1 sc td s with the photoisomerization of 8.6 rhodopsins per rod on average, 25 and assuming that each dark-adapted rod contains ∼108 rhodopsins, 26 the fractional bleaches produced by the 67 and 670 sc cd s m−2 conditioning flashes were ∼3 × 10−4 and ∼3 × 10−3, respectively. 
A software package (DT VEE; Data Translation, Inc., Marlboro, MA) triggered a pulse generator (Pulsemaster A300; World Precision Instruments Inc., Sarasota, FL) that controlled the onset timing of each flash. The signal was amplified 1000-fold at a band-pass of 0.1 to 3000 Hz by a differential AC amplifier (Model CP511; Grass-Telefactor). Recordings were saved on the computer by the software at a sampling rate of 10 kHz. Consecutive trials were separated by a dark-adaptation period of at least 1 minute. The rod-isolated response to the probe flash was determined by computational subtraction of the response to the photopically matched long-wavelength probe flash from the raw probe response. 21 22 27 Further details of the procedure used to determine the derived rod response are described in the Results section. 
Two experiments, both based on the paired-flash paradigm, were conducted to investigate rod recovery from a bright conditioning flash. Experiment 1, described schematically in the upper part of Figure 1 , involved variation of the interval between a bright conditioning flash of fixed intensity presented at time zero and a bright fixed probe flash (short-wavelength probe) presented at a later time t probe. The response obtained with a photopically matched long-wavelength probe was computationally subtracted from the raw probe response and the resultant, presumed rod-isolated probe response was analyzed for amplitude at time t det. A(t), the amplitude of the derived rod response to the conditioning flash at time t, was then obtained from the relation  
\[\mathrm{A}(t)\ {=}\ \mathrm{A}_{\mathrm{moD}}\ {-}\ \mathrm{A}_{\mathrm{m}}(t)\]
 
\[t\ {=}\ t_{\mathrm{probe}}\ {+}\ t_{\mathrm{det}}\]
where Am(t) is the amplitude of the rod-isolated probe response determined in the paired-flash trial, and where AmoD is the rod-isolated probe response amplitude determined under dark-adapted conditions—that is, in the absence of recent presentation of the conditioning flash (probe-alone response). 28 29 As t probe in all experiments greatly exceeded t det (7.0 – 8.4 ms), time t in all of the reported data obtained in the type 1 experiments refers to the conditioning-probe interval t probe
Experiment 2 (lower part of Fig. 1 ) involved three flashes within each of a series of trials: a fixed, bright conditioning flash presented at time zero; a test flash of variable intensity presented at a fixed later time (conditioning-test interval t CT, set equal to 2 or 9 seconds); and a fixed probe flash presented at 200 ms after the test flash. Here the presumed rod-isolated probe response obtained after computational subtraction of the response to a photopically matched probe flash was similarly analyzed for amplitude. A(t′), the derived rod response to the test flash at time t′ after test flash presentation, was determined from the relation  
\[\mathrm{A}(t{^\prime}){=}\mathrm{A}_{\mathrm{moL}}{-}\mathrm{A}_{\mathrm{m}}(t{^\prime})\]
 
\[t{^\prime}{=}200\ \mathrm{ms}{+}t_{\mathrm{det}}\]
where Am(t′) is the amplitude of the rod-isolated probe response determined in the multi-flash trial, and AmoL is the rod-isolated probe amplitude determined on setting the test flash strength to zero in an otherwise identical trial. AmoL thus represents the probe-alone response prevailing at time t′ after the conditioning flash. 29  
Dark Adaptometry
Measurements of visual thresholds were performed with use of a Goldmann-Weekers dark adaptometer and used procedures similar to those previously described. 15 30 Briefly, the test stimulus was a flickering light (0.5 Hz) of short wavelength (λmax near 460 nm), and thresholds for detection were measured using the method of limits (both ascending and descending stimulus strengths). The test stimulus subtended a 2° area, and its location in the inferior visual field ranged from 15° to 45°. For each patient, the test stimulus was positioned to illuminate an area outside a central scotoma determined with kinetic Goldmann perimetry and a II2e target. After pupil dilation and 45 minutes of dark adaptation, thresholds were repeatedly measured until a stable value was obtained. The test eye was then exposed to white light (3.1 log cd m−2, 5 minutes in duration) that bleached approximately 67% of the rhodopsin, and the subsequent course of dark adaptation was determined. 
Results
Genotype Data and a-Wave Characteristics
Table 1 summarizes genotyping results obtained from the Stargardt patients. For patients 106 to 109, two ABCA4 sequence variations felt to be disease-causing were detected and presumed collectively to be on different alleles. Only a single variation was detected for patient 110. All of the allelic variations in the ABCA4 gene found in these patients are significantly more prevalent in patients with Stargardt disease than in the normal population. 24 The ala1038val and gly1961glu alleles are relatively common in Stargardt disease and may have altered frequencies in different populations as a result of founder effects. 31  
Figure 2 shows a-wave responses obtained from a representative normal subject (subject 104; panel A) and from two Stargardt patients (subjects 106 and 109; Figs. 2B 2C , respectively) under dark-adapted conditions. The illustrated data were obtained on presentation of short-wavelength flashes that ranged from 3.02 to 2400 sc cd s m−2. Shown in the right-hand column of Table 1 are maximum amplitudes of the ERG a-wave peak recorded from each of the normal subjects and Stargardt patients using a stimulus of either 955 or 2400 sc cd s m−2. Maximal responses obtained from the Stargardt patients were on average lower than, but did not significantly differ from, those obtained from the normal subjects (P = 0.06). 
Experiment 1
Experiments classified as type 1 were conducted to investigate the kinetics of recovery for the rod response to the conditioning flash (Fig. 1) . Figures 3A 3B 3C 3D 3E 3F show probe responses recorded in representative experiments of this type conducted on two normal subjects (102 and 104) with use of the 670 sc cd s m−2 conditioning flash (henceforth termed C670). Figure 3A shows raw probe responses obtained from subject 102 at varying times after the C670 flash. Here, each numerical label identifies the interval, in seconds, between the conditioning flash and the labeled probe response; waveform PAD is the dark-adapted probe-alone response; and waveform LD is the response to a photopically matched long-wavelength flash presented under dark-adapted conditions. Computational subtraction of waveform LD from waveform PAD yielded response PAD in Figure 3B , which was taken to represent the rod-mediated component of the response to the probe flash (see, e.g., Ref. 22 ). (Henceforth, the term PAD will be taken to represent the dark-adapted probe-alone response after subtraction of the cone contribution as determined under dark-adapted conditions.) Waveform L2 in Figure 3A is the response to the photopically matched long-wavelength probe presented 2 seconds after the conditioning flash in this experiment. This waveform was computationally subtracted from the probe waveforms obtained at early times after the conditioning flash (for subject 102, waveforms 1 and 2) to yield the corresponding rod-mediated probe responses. Here the L2 response was used as the reference since it was obtained under conditions most similar to those of the early time short-wavelength probe responses. For waveforms 3 to 9 in Figure 3A , cone correction of the raw probe response involved computational subtraction of waveform LD obtained under dark-adapted conditions. This analysis assumes that the cone response was fully recovered by 2 seconds after the conditioning flash. As illustrated by the peak-normalized waveforms in Figure 3C , the rod-isolated probe responses obtained at middle and late stages of recovery from the conditioning flash exhibited rising-phase kinetics similar to those of the dark-adapted probe-alone response PAD. 22 28 Figures 3D 3E and 3F show the results of a similar analysis of probe response data obtained from a second normal subject (104). The rod-isolated probe responses of B and E show a growth with time, reflecting recovery of the derived rod response to the conditioning flash. Furthermore, the results shown in Figures 3C and 3F indicate a similarity of response kinetics within each family of responses. Figures 4A 4B 4C 4D 4E 4F illustrate response recovery in type 1 experiments conducted with Stargardt patients 109 and 107. As in Figure 3 , the data show raw probe responses obtained after the C670 flash (Figs. 4A 4D) , the rod-isolated responses obtained after cone correction (Figs. 4B 4E) , and the peak-normalized rod-isolated responses (Figs. 4C 4F)
Recovery data obtained from each subject were normalized to the amplitude of the dark-adapted probe-alone response obtained from that subject. Figure 5A shows the time course of A(t)/AmoD, the normalized derived rod response, as a function of the conditioning-probe interval t probe. Open symbols indicate results obtained with use of the 670 sc cd s m−2 conditioning flash. These data were obtained from the cone-subtracted responses obtained from normal subjects 102 and 104 (Fig. 3) , and from similarly analyzed probe responses obtained in identical experiments on two other normal subjects. The data show that a condition of near-saturation of the rod response persisted for approximately 2 to 3 seconds after the conditioning flash, and half-recovery occurred at approximately 5 to 6 seconds after the conditioning flash. Furthermore, there was an apparent slowing of response recovery during the latter phase of the investigated period. The time course of the recovery determined with the C670 flash can be described by a nested exponential function 28 29 in which E(t), the excitation generated by the conditioning flash, is presumed to decay through a rapid initial reaction and a subsequent slower reaction (cf., e.g., equations 5 and 6 of Ref. 32 ).  
\[\mathrm{A}(t)/\mathrm{A}_{\mathrm{moD}}\ {=}\ 1\ {-}\ \mathrm{exp}{[}{-}{\gamma}_{1}\mathrm{E}(t){]}\]
 
\[\mathrm{E}(t)\ {=}\ \mathrm{exp}({-}t/{\tau}_{1}){+}{\gamma}_{2}{[}\mathrm{exp}({-}t/{\tau}_{2})\ {-}\ \mathrm{exp}({-}t/{\tau}_{1}){]}\]
 
\[\mathrm{A}(t)/\mathrm{A}_{\mathrm{moD}}\ {=}\ 1{-}\mathrm{exp}{\{}{-}{\gamma}_{3}{[}\mathrm{exp}({-}t/{\tau}_{1})\ {+}\ {\gamma}_{4}\mathrm{exp}({-}t/{\tau}_{2}){]}{\}}\]
where γ1– γ4 are dimensionless constants and τ1 and τ2 are exponential lifetimes. The solid curve in Figure 5A , which plots equation 2c with γ3 = 14.72, γ4 = 0.040, τ1 = 1.29 seconds, and τ2 = 22.86 seconds, closely approximates the data. Also shown in Figure 5A are results obtained from two normal subjects with the 67 sc cd s m−2 conditioning flash, henceforth termed the C67 flash (filled symbols). Here, there was no prolonged period of response saturation, and half-recovery occurred at approximately 2 seconds. The dashed curve associated with these data plots equation 2c with γ3 = 5.86, τ1 = 0.79 seconds, and γ4 = 0. Figure 5B shows normalized derived responses to the C670 and C67 flashes determined for the Stargardt patients. Open symbols in Figure 5B represent results obtained from Stargardt patients 109 and 107 (cf. Fig. 4 ) and from two other patients. The equation 2c function was fitted to the three sets of data that were relatively closely grouped (circles, squares, and inverted triangles; the omitted data (triangles) were those obtained from patient 108), yielding the illustrated solid curve, with γ3 = 22.24, γ4 = 0.015, τ1 = 1.14 seconds, and τ2 = 82.22 seconds. Thus, the time course of recovery from the 670 sc cd s m−2 conditioning flash determined for the Stargardt patients was qualitatively similar to that determined for the normal subjects, but characterized by an exponential time constant (τ2) that exceeded, by more than threefold, that determined for the normal subjects (see the Discussion section). Filled circles in Figure 5B show results obtained from a single patient with Stargardt disease with use of the C67 flash. The dashed curve accompanying these results plots equation 2c with γ3 = 13.71, τ1 = 0.35 seconds, and γ4 = 0. 
Experiment 2
Experiments classified as type 2 were designed to measure the instantaneous amplitude-intensity function for the rod response at a defined time after conditioning flash presentation (see Fig. 1 ). Figure 6 illustrates rod-isolated probe responses obtained in type 2 experiments conducted with one normal subject and two Stargardt patients. Shown in the left, middle, and right panels in Figure 6 are, respectively, results obtained under dark-adapted conditions (Figs. 6A 6D 6G) , results obtained with conditioning flash C67 and a conditioning-test interval t CT of 2 seconds (Figs. 6B 6E 6H) , and results obtained with conditioning flash C670 and t CT of 9 seconds (Figs. 6C 6F 6I) . In all cases, the test flash of variable intensity and the fixed probe flash were separated by a fixed interval of 200 ms (Fig. 1) , a period determined in photocurrent and paired-flash ERG studies to correspond with a time shortly after the peak of the weak-flash response. 22 23 33 Shown within each family of responses is the dark-adapted probe-alone response (PAD), and response PA, which was obtained in the absence of test flash presentation. Response PA thus represents the probe-alone response that prevailed under the investigated experimental conditions. 
Figures 7 and 8 show normalized amplitude-intensity functions determined from probe response data of the types shown in Figure 6 . For a given subject and under a given experimental condition, determination of the function involved normalization of the rod-isolated probe amplitudes to the rod-isolated amplitude of the prevailing probe-alone response PA. For combined illustration of the data obtained from different subjects, test flash strengths in these experiments were converted to units of sc td s. Each resultant function describes, as a function of log test flash strength, the amplitude A(t′) of the derived rod response normalized to the prevailing maximum amplitude AmoD or AmoL (see the Methods section). Figures 7A 7B 7C and 8A 8B 8C show results obtained from the normal subjects and Stargardt patients under dark-adapted conditions (A), with conditioning flash C67 and t CT = 2 seconds (B), and with conditioning flash C670 and t CT = 9 seconds (C). Curves fitted to the data of each panel plot the Naka-Rushton relation  
\[\mathrm{A}(t{^\prime})/\mathrm{A}_{\mathrm{mo}}\ {=}\ \mathrm{I}_{\mathrm{test}}(\mathrm{I}_{\mathrm{test},0.5}\ {+}\ \mathrm{I}_{\mathrm{test}})^{{-}1}\]
where Itest is the test flash strength, Amo is either AmoD or AmoL, and Itest,0.5 is the test flash strength at half-saturation of the normalized function. The determined Itest,0.5 thus represents a measure of rod sensitivity to weak (i.e., subsaturating) test flashes. In both Figures 7 and 8 , the fitted curves in A–C are reproduced in D for comparison with one another. Analysis of these functions yielded 1.48 and 1.46 log sc td s for the group average log Itest,0.5 determined under dark-adapted conditions from, respectively, normal subjects and Stargardt patients. Corresponding analysis of the results obtained with the C67 conditioning flash and t CT = 2 seconds yielded 2.00 and 1.96, respectively, for the group average log Itest,0.5 among normal subjects and Stargardt patients. Those obtained with the C670 conditioning flash and t CT = 9 seconds yielded group-average log Itest,0.5 values of 1.92 and 2.14, respectively, for normal subjects and Stargardt patients. Thus, for both groups of subjects, the decreases in rod sensitivity determined with the C67 conditioning flash and t CT = 2 seconds, and with the C670 conditioning flash and t CT = 9 seconds, were approximately 0.5 log unit on average. Table 2 shows log Itest,0.5 determined by the separate fitting of equation 3 to the data obtained from a given subject under the three investigated conditions. For each of these conditions there was no significant difference between the values determined for normal subjects and Stargardt patients (P = 0.96, 0.92 and 0.30, respectively, for the top, middle, and bottom rows of Table 2 ). For each subject for whom log Itest,0.5 was determined under both dark-adapted conditions and at 9 seconds after the C670 conditioning flash (two normal subjects and four Stargardt patients), the values of log Itest,0.5 obtained under the two conditions were also compared through a paired t-test. This analysis indicated a significant difference between the values of log Itest,0.5 determined for the Stargardt patients (P = 0.011), but not between those determined for the normal subjects (P = 0.128). A similar subject-by-subject, paired t-test analysis of data obtained under dark-adapted conditions and at 2 seconds after the C67 conditioning flash showed significant differences for the data obtained from two normal subjects (P = 0.005) and four Stargardt patients (P = 0.007). 
Psychophysically Measured Dark Adaptation
Open circles, filled triangles, filled circles, and open triangles in Figure 9 show the dark adaptometry data obtained, respectively, from Stargardt patients 106, 108, 109, and 110 after a 5-minute adapting exposure that in normal subjects produced an estimated 67% bleach. 15 30 The additional sets of data (solid lines) are results obtained from four normal subjects, one of whom was subject 105 tested in the ERG experiments. For subjects 105, 108, 109, and 110, the dark adaptometry data were obtained from the same eye as that tested in the ERG experiments. Log threshold data obtained from a given subject are referred to the prebleaching (i.e., fully dark-adapted) threshold determined for that subject (Fig. 9 , horizontal dashed line). By comparison with results obtained from the normal subjects, the data obtained from Stargardt patients 106, 109, and 110 showed a modest delay in the final phase of recovery. Those obtained from the fourth patient (subject 108), the individual for whom dark-adapted maximal a-wave amplitude and sensitivity of the amplitude-intensity function were relatively low (Table 1 , Fig. 8 ) and for whom rod recovery kinetics were relatively slow (Fig. 5B and accompanying text), showed a more substantially prolonged course of dark adaptation. Overall, the results obtained from Stargardt patients 106, 109, and 110 indicate a modest but discernible prolongation of desensitization during the final portion of recovery to preillumination baseline. 
Discussion
The results of this study describe properties of rod dark adaptation in normal subjects and in ABCA4-associated Stargardt patients, after a weak-bleaching but rod-saturating conditioning flash. Rod recovery in human subjects after exposure to bright conditioning light has been previously investigated through measurement of the ERG a-wave. 21 34 35 To our knowledge, however, the present study is the first to employ the paired-flash method to determine the instantaneous rod amplitude-intensity function (and thus, the sensitivity parameter Itest,0.5) during dark adaptation in human subjects (type 2 experiments). In the present experiments, we focused on sensitivity determinations at fixed intermediate times associated with recovery of a substantial portion of the maximum, dark-adapted range of the photoresponse (i.e., with recovery of the response to levels well below saturation). A main conclusion from these sensitivity determinations is that at 2 seconds after the C67 conditioning flash and at 9 seconds after the C670 flash—that is, at times when response recovery is near complete (cf. Fig. 5 )—there is a remaining significant desensitization of the rod response function in both normal subjects and Stargardt patients. Furthermore, as determined both with t CT = 2 seconds after the C67 flash and t CT = 9 seconds after the C670 flash, desensitizations of the normalized amplitude-intensity functions exhibited by the investigated Stargardt patients are similar to those exhibited by the normal subjects (Figs. 7 8) . These results leave open the possibility that the period required for full recovery of rod sensitivity exceeds that required for the near-completion of excitation decay. However, properties of the data obtained from the group of trials conducted within a given recording session (i.e., groups of amplitude-intensity data that are generally well described by the Naka-Rushton equation) argue against the possibility that any such period of lingering desensitization exceeded the approximately 1-minute period that separated consecutive trials. Furthermore, despite the lower average a-wave maximum amplitude (Table 1) and the apparently slower recovery from the C670 conditioning flash (described later) determined for the Stargardt patients, occurrence of an ABCA4 mutation in these patients does not substantially affect dark-adapted rod sensitivity (Figs. 7 8 , and accompanying text; Table 2 ). 
In both normal subjects and Stargardt patients, recovery from the C670 conditioning flash was characterized by a biphasic process generally similar to those observed in previous studies of electroretinographically determined rod recovery (e.g., Refs. 28 , 36 , 37 ). Specifically, analysis of the data of Figure 5 through the present equation 2 2 2 indicates that rod recovery from the C670 flash both in normal subjects and in the investigated Stargardt patients is consistent with the partial deactivation of a photoactivated intermediate on a time scale of approximately 1 second (1.29 seconds for normal subjects, 1.14 seconds for Stargardt patients; see text accompanying Fig. 5 ), and further deactivation through a process occurring on a considerably longer time scale (described later). The equation used here to model recovery is identical with that used to characterize the falling phase of the rod response after weak (nonbleaching) stimuli 28 and significant rhodopsin bleaching, 29 except for the substitution of the function E(t) (equation 2b) for the single term used previously. As E(t) describes the decay of an activated species through two sequential first-order reactions, the fit provided by equation 2 2 2 is consistent with the occurrence of two sequential first-order deactivation reactions that rate-limit recovery of the response (cf. Refs. 36 , 37 ). 
The equation 2 2 2 fit to the data obtained with the C670 conditioning flash yielded markedly different values for the slower of the two time constants (τ2 = 22.86 seconds for normal subjects; 82.22 seconds for Stargardt patients). It is of interest to consider this difference in light of the finding that dark-adapted values of weak-flash sensitivity for the two groups of subjects are similar (e.g., Table 2 ). By facilitating the translocation of all-trans retinal (as N-retinylidene-phosphatidylethanolamine 6 8 10 38 ) from the disc lumen to the cytosol of the rod outer segment, the ABCA4 gene product is thought to facilitate the removal of all-trans retinal from opsin and thereby promote the termination of excitation in the phototransduction cascade (see the Introduction). Thus, a possible basis for the relatively slow late phase of electroretinographically determined response recovery in the investigated Stargardt patients after the C670 conditioning flash (i.e., the relatively high value of the fitted τ2) is a sluggish removal of all-trans retinal from the disc lumen. On this interpretation, the general similarity of the dark adaptometry results obtained from the normal subjects and Stargardt patients after extensive bleaching (i.e., the occurrence in three of the Stargardt patients of a modest delay, relative to normal subjects, only in the final phase of dark adaptation; Fig. 9 ) can be explained on the hypothesis that with large bleaches, the amount of all-trans retinal photoproduct present in the disc lumen is sufficient, over all but the final stage of recovery, to saturate (i.e., overwhelm) the all-trans retinal removal process in the rods of normal subjects as well as Stargardt patients, and thus to produce a relative similarity in the time courses of dark adaptation. 
 
Figure 1.
 
Timing of flash stimuli in a given trial of experiments 1 and 2. In experiment 1, the interval t probe between the conditioning flash and probe flash was varied among trials. Experiment 2 involved a fixed-interval t CT between the conditioning and test flashes, a fixed interval between the test and probe flashes (200 ms), and variation of the test flash strength.
Figure 1.
 
Timing of flash stimuli in a given trial of experiments 1 and 2. In experiment 1, the interval t probe between the conditioning flash and probe flash was varied among trials. Experiment 2 involved a fixed-interval t CT between the conditioning and test flashes, a fixed interval between the test and probe flashes (200 ms), and variation of the test flash strength.
Figure 2.
 
ERG responses to short-wavelength flashes. Labels identify the flash strength in sc cd s m−2. Results obtained from normal subject 104 (A), Stargardt patient 106 (B), and Stargardt patient 109 (C).
Figure 2.
 
ERG responses to short-wavelength flashes. Labels identify the flash strength in sc cd s m−2. Results obtained from normal subject 104 (A), Stargardt patient 106 (B), and Stargardt patient 109 (C).
Table 1.
 
Description of Subjects
Table 1.
 
Description of Subjects
Subject Number Age* Sex ABCA4 Variation Dark-Adapted Maximum Peak a-Wave Amplitude (μV), †
Normal subjects
 101 55 M −243
 102 37 F −410
 103 26 M −188
 104 23 F −397
 105 56 F −268
 111 29 F −362
 112 23 M −410
−325 ± 91
Stargardt patients
 106 50 F val849ala, arg2107his −201
 107 41 M gly1961glu, arg2077trp −306
 108 22 M ala60val, 1 bp ins codon 1513 −82
 109 34 M leu541pro/ala1038val,, ‡ gly1961glu −277
 110 51 M gly1961glu −191
−211 ± 87
Figure 3.
 
Recovery of the probe flash response in two normal subjects after a 670 sc cd s m−2 conditioning flash (C670) (experiment 1). (A) Raw responses obtained with probe flash presentation at defined times after C670. Data from normal subject 102. PAD: dark-adapted response to the probe flash delivered alone (dark-adapted probe-alone response). Numerical labels identify the interval t probe, in seconds, between the conditioning and probe flashes. Waveform LD: response to the photopically matched long-wavelength flash presented alone under dark-adapted conditions. Waveform L2: response to a photopically matched long-wavelength flash presented 2 seconds after C670. (B) Rod-isolated probe responses obtained from the waveforms in (A) after subtraction of the presumed cone-mediated component (waveform LD or L2). (C) Rod-isolated probe responses of (B) shown in peak-normalized form. The illustrated responses are those obtained with t probe ≥ 4 seconds. (DF) Data obtained from normal subject 104 with use of the C670 conditioning flash. Format of the data is similar to that in (AC).
Figure 3.
 
Recovery of the probe flash response in two normal subjects after a 670 sc cd s m−2 conditioning flash (C670) (experiment 1). (A) Raw responses obtained with probe flash presentation at defined times after C670. Data from normal subject 102. PAD: dark-adapted response to the probe flash delivered alone (dark-adapted probe-alone response). Numerical labels identify the interval t probe, in seconds, between the conditioning and probe flashes. Waveform LD: response to the photopically matched long-wavelength flash presented alone under dark-adapted conditions. Waveform L2: response to a photopically matched long-wavelength flash presented 2 seconds after C670. (B) Rod-isolated probe responses obtained from the waveforms in (A) after subtraction of the presumed cone-mediated component (waveform LD or L2). (C) Rod-isolated probe responses of (B) shown in peak-normalized form. The illustrated responses are those obtained with t probe ≥ 4 seconds. (DF) Data obtained from normal subject 104 with use of the C670 conditioning flash. Format of the data is similar to that in (AC).
Figure 4.
 
Recovery of the probe flash response in Stargardt patients 109 (AC) and 107 (DF) using the C670 conditioning flash. Format of data is similar to that of Figure 3 .
Figure 4.
 
Recovery of the probe flash response in Stargardt patients 109 (AC) and 107 (DF) using the C670 conditioning flash. Format of data is similar to that of Figure 3 .
Figure 5.
 
Recovery time course of the derived rod response after a conditioning flash. (A) Normal subjects. Open symbols: data obtained from four normal subjects after the 670 sc cd s m−2 conditioning flash. Among these four subjects, the retinal illuminance of this conditioning flash ranged from 3.4 × 104 to 5.3 × 104 sc td s. Filled symbols: data obtained from two normal subjects with the 67 sc cd s m−2 conditioning flash (corresponding retinal illuminance: 3.4 × 103 and 5.3 × 103 sc td s). (B) Stargardt patients. Open symbols: data obtained from four Stargardt patients with use of the 670 sc cd s m−2 conditioning flash (range of retinal illuminance: 3.0 × 104 to 3.4 × 104 sc td s). Filled symbols: data obtained from one patient with use of the 67 sc cd s m−2 conditioning flash (retinal illuminance: 3.4 × 103 sc td s).
Figure 5.
 
Recovery time course of the derived rod response after a conditioning flash. (A) Normal subjects. Open symbols: data obtained from four normal subjects after the 670 sc cd s m−2 conditioning flash. Among these four subjects, the retinal illuminance of this conditioning flash ranged from 3.4 × 104 to 5.3 × 104 sc td s. Filled symbols: data obtained from two normal subjects with the 67 sc cd s m−2 conditioning flash (corresponding retinal illuminance: 3.4 × 103 and 5.3 × 103 sc td s). (B) Stargardt patients. Open symbols: data obtained from four Stargardt patients with use of the 670 sc cd s m−2 conditioning flash (range of retinal illuminance: 3.0 × 104 to 3.4 × 104 sc td s). Filled symbols: data obtained from one patient with use of the 67 sc cd s m−2 conditioning flash (retinal illuminance: 3.4 × 103 sc td s).
Figure 6.
 
Rod-isolated probe responses obtained in paired-flash trials during recovery from a conditioning flash (experiment 2). (AC) Data from normal subject 102 obtained with test flashes over the range of 4.36 to 437 sc td s. (DF): Data from Stargardt patient 109 obtained with test flashes over the range of 3.46 to 347 sc td s. (GI) Data from Stargardt patient 110 obtained with test flashes over the range of 3.46 to 347 sc td s. Data in (A), (D), and (G) were obtained under dark-adapted conditions; data in (B), (E), and (H) were obtained with conditioning flash C67 and t CT = 2 seconds; data in (C), (F), and (I) were obtained with conditioning flash C670 and t CT = 9 seconds. PAD is the dark-adapted probe-alone response; PA is the prevailing probe-alone response. Numerical labels shown in (A), (D), and (G) identify test flash strengths associated with the indicated probe responses. Scale bars shown in (A), (D), and (G) refer, respectively, to data obtained from subjects 102, 109, and 110.
Figure 6.
 
Rod-isolated probe responses obtained in paired-flash trials during recovery from a conditioning flash (experiment 2). (AC) Data from normal subject 102 obtained with test flashes over the range of 4.36 to 437 sc td s. (DF): Data from Stargardt patient 109 obtained with test flashes over the range of 3.46 to 347 sc td s. (GI) Data from Stargardt patient 110 obtained with test flashes over the range of 3.46 to 347 sc td s. Data in (A), (D), and (G) were obtained under dark-adapted conditions; data in (B), (E), and (H) were obtained with conditioning flash C67 and t CT = 2 seconds; data in (C), (F), and (I) were obtained with conditioning flash C670 and t CT = 9 seconds. PAD is the dark-adapted probe-alone response; PA is the prevailing probe-alone response. Numerical labels shown in (A), (D), and (G) identify test flash strengths associated with the indicated probe responses. Scale bars shown in (A), (D), and (G) refer, respectively, to data obtained from subjects 102, 109, and 110.
Figure 7.
 
Amplitude-intensity functions obtained from normal subjects. Identical symbols represent data from the same subject. Data obtained from four subjects under dark-adapted (DA) conditions (A); from two subjects with conditioning flash C67 and t CT = 2 seconds (B); and from four subjects with conditioning flash C670 and t CT = 9 seconds (C). Curves plot equation 3 fitted to the data. (D) Fitted curves reproduced from (AC).
Figure 7.
 
Amplitude-intensity functions obtained from normal subjects. Identical symbols represent data from the same subject. Data obtained from four subjects under dark-adapted (DA) conditions (A); from two subjects with conditioning flash C67 and t CT = 2 seconds (B); and from four subjects with conditioning flash C670 and t CT = 9 seconds (C). Curves plot equation 3 fitted to the data. (D) Fitted curves reproduced from (AC).
Figure 8.
 
Amplitude-intensity functions obtained from Stargardt patients. The format is as in Figure 7 . Identical symbols indicate data obtained from the same subject. Data obtained from five patients in (A), four in (B), and four in (C). Curves plot equation 3 fitted to the data. Inverted triangles: data obtained from patient 108. In (A) the circled inverted triangle was omitted in the curve fitting. (D) Fitted curves reproduced from (AC).
Figure 8.
 
Amplitude-intensity functions obtained from Stargardt patients. The format is as in Figure 7 . Identical symbols indicate data obtained from the same subject. Data obtained from five patients in (A), four in (B), and four in (C). Curves plot equation 3 fitted to the data. Inverted triangles: data obtained from patient 108. In (A) the circled inverted triangle was omitted in the curve fitting. (D) Fitted curves reproduced from (AC).
Table 2.
 
Determinations of Logarithmic Weak-flash Sensitivity (log Itest,0.5)
Table 2.
 
Determinations of Logarithmic Weak-flash Sensitivity (log Itest,0.5)
Experimental Condition Log Itest,0.5 (log sc td s)
Normal Subjects Stargardt Patients
Dark adapted 1.48 ± 0.22 (4) 1.47 ± 0.32 (5)
C67, t CT = 2 s 1.99 ± 0.17 (2) 1.96 ± 0.32 (4)
C670, t CT = 9 s 1.93 ± 0.16 (4) 2.14 ± 0.30 (4)
Figure 9.
 
Psychophysically determined time course of dark adaptation in four normal subjects and four Stargardt patients after illumination estimated in normal subjects to produce approximately 67% bleaching. The four segmented lines indicate results from four visually normal subjects, one of whom was subject 105. Log threshold data obtained from a given subject are referred to the prebleaching (i.e., fully dark-adapted) threshold determined for that subject (horizontal dashed line).
Figure 9.
 
Psychophysically determined time course of dark adaptation in four normal subjects and four Stargardt patients after illumination estimated in normal subjects to produce approximately 67% bleaching. The four segmented lines indicate results from four visually normal subjects, one of whom was subject 105. Log threshold data obtained from a given subject are referred to the prebleaching (i.e., fully dark-adapted) threshold determined for that subject (horizontal dashed line).
The authors thank Gabriel A. Silva, Martin Lindeman, and Christopher Eastman for helpful discussions and technical assistance during the course of this study. 
Klien BA, Krill AE. Fundus flavimaculatus: clinical, functional and histopathologic observations. Am J Ophthalmol. 1967;64:3–23. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Fishman GA. Fundus flavimaculatus: a clinical classification. Arch Ophthalmol. 1976;94:2061–2067. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Fishman GA, Stone EM, Grover S, Derlacki DJ, Haines HL, Hockey RR. Variation of clinical expression in patients with Stargardt dystrophy and sequence variations in the ABCR gene. Arch Ophthalmol. 1999;117:504–510. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Allikmets R, Singh N, Sun H, et al. A photoreceptor cell-specific ATP-binding transporter gene (ABCR) is mutated in recessive Stargardt macular dystrophy. Nat Genet. 1997;15:236–245. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Briggs CE, Rucinski D, Rosenfeld PJ, Hirose T, Berson EL, Dryja TP. Mutations in ABCR (ABCA4) in patients with Stargardt macular degeneration or cone-rod degeneration. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2001;42:2229–2236. [PubMed]
Ahn J, Wong JT, Molday RS. The effect of lipid environment and retinoids on the ATPase activity of ABCR, the photoreceptor ABC transporter responsible for Stargardt macular dystrophy. J Biol Chem. 2000;275:20399–20405. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Ahn J, Beharry S, Molday LL, Molday RS. Functional interaction between the two halves of the photoreceptor-specific ATP binding cassette protein ABCR (ABCA4): evidence for a non-exchangeable ADP in the first nucleotide binding domain. J Biol Chem. 2003;278:39600–39608. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Mata NL, Weng J, Travis GH. Biosynthesis of a major lipofuscin fluorophore in mice and humans with ABCR-mediated retinal and macular degeneration. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2000;97:7154–7159. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Sun H, Smallwood PM, Nathans J. Biochemical defects in ABCR protein variants associated with human retinopathies. Nat Genet. 2000;26:242–246. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Sun H, Nathans J. Mechanistic studies of ABCR, the ABC transporter in photoreceptor outer segments responsible for autosomal recessive Stargardt disease. J Bioenerg Biomembr. 2001;33:523–530. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Hofmann KP, Pulvermüller A, Buczyłko J, Van Hooser P, Palczewski K. The role of arrestin and retinoids in the regeneration pathway of rhodopsin. J Biol Chem. 1992;267:15701–15706. [PubMed]
Sachs K, Maretzki D, Meyer CK, Hofmann KP. Diffusible ligand all-trans-retinal activates opsin via a palmitoylation-dependent mechanism. J Biol Chem. 2000;275:6189–6194. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Leibrock CS, Reuter T, Lamb TD. Molecular basis of dark adaptation in rod photoreceptors. Eye. 1998;12:511–520. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
McBee JK, Palczewski K, Baehr W, Pepperberg DR. Confronting complexity: the interlink of phototransduction and retinoid metabolism in the vertebrate retina. Prog Retin Eye Res. 2001;20:469–529. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Fishman GA, Farbman JS, Alexander KR. Delayed rod dark adaptation in patients with Stargardt’s disease. Ophthalmology. 1991;98:957–962. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Baca W, Fishman GA, Alexander KR, Glenn AM. Dark adaptation in patients with Best vitelliform macular dystrophy. Br J Ophthalmol. 1994;78:430–432. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Birch DG, Peters AY, Locke KL, Spencer R, Megarity CF, Travis GH. Visual function in patients with cone-rod dystrophy (CRD) associated with mutations in the ABCA4 (ABCR) gene. Exp Eye Res. 2001;73:877–886. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Parisi V, Canu D, Iarossi G, Olzi D, Falsini B. Altered recovery of macular function after bleaching in Stargardt’s disease-fundus flavimaculatus: pattern VEP evidence. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2002;43:2741–2748. [PubMed]
Cideciyan AV, Aleman TS, Swider M, et al. Mutations in ABCA4 result in accumulation of lipofuscin before slowing of the retinoid cycle: a reappraisal of the human disease sequence. Hum Mol Genet. 2004;13:525–534. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Weng J, Mata NL, Azarian SM, Tzekov RT, Birch DG, Travis GH. Insights into the function of Rim protein in photoreceptors and etiology of Stargardt’s disease from the phenotype in abcr knockout mice. Cell. 1999;98:13–23. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Birch DG, Hood DC, Nusinowitz S, Pepperberg DR. Abnormal activation and inactivation mechanisms of rod transduction in patients with autosomal dominant retinitis pigmentosa and the pro-23-his mutation. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1995;36:1603–1614. [PubMed]
Pepperberg DR, Birch DG, Hood DC. Photoresponses of human rods in vivo derived from paired-flash electroretinograms. Vis Neurosci. 1997;14:73–82. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Friedburg C, Thomas MM, Lamb TD. Time course of the flash response of dark- and light-adapted human rod photoreceptors derived from the electroretinogram. J Physiol. 2001;534:217–242. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Webster AR, Héon E, Lotery AJ, et al. An analysis of allelic variation in the ABCA4 gene. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2001;42:1179–1189. [PubMed]
Breton ME, Schueller AW, Lamb TD, Pugh EN, Jr. Analysis of ERG a-wave amplification and kinetics in terms of the G-protein cascade of phototransduction. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1994;35:295–309. [PubMed]
Pugh EN, Jr, Lamb TD. Phototransduction in vertebrate rods and cones: molecular mechanisms of amplification, recovery and light adaptation. Stavenga DG DeGrip WJ Pugh EN, Jr eds. Handbook of Biological Physics. 2000;3:183–255. Elsevier Science Amsterdam.
Pepperberg DR, Birch DG, Hood DC. Electroretinographic determination of human rod flash response in vivo. Methods Enzymol. 2000;316B:202–223.
Hetling JR, Pepperberg DR. Sensitivity and kinetics of mouse rod flash responses determined in vivo from paired-flash electroretinograms. J Physiol. 1999;516:593–609. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Kang Derwent JJ, Qtaishat NM, Pepperberg DR. Excitation and desensitization of mouse rod photoreceptors in vivo following bright adapting light. J Physiol. 2002;541:201–218. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Glenn AM, Fishman GA, Gilbert LD, Derlacki DJ. Effect of vitamin A treatment on the prolongation of dark adaptation in Stargardt’s dystrophy. Retina. 1994;14:27–30. [PubMed]
Simonelli F, Testa F, de Crecchio G, et al. New ABCR mutations and clinical phenotype in Italian patients with Stargardt disease. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2000;41:892–897. [PubMed]
Pepperberg DR. Does rod phototransduction involve the delayed transition of activated rhodopsin to a second, more active catalytic state?. Vis Neurosci. 1998;15:1067–1078. [PubMed]
Kraft TW, Schneeweis DM, Schnapf JL. Visual transduction in human rod photoreceptors. J Physiol. 1993;464:747–765. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Cideciyan AV, Zhao X, Nielsen L, Khani SC, Jacobson SG, Palczewski K. Null mutation in the rhodopsin kinase gene slows recovery kinetics of rod and cone phototransduction in man. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1998;95:328–333. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Thomas MM, Lamb TD. Light adaptation and dark adaptation of human rod photoreceptors measured from the a-wave of the electroretinogram. J Physiol. 1999;518:479–496. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Kennedy MJ, Lee KA, Niemi GA, et al. Multiple phosphorylation of rhodopsin and the in vivo chemistry underlying rod photoreceptor dark adaptation. Neuron. 2001;31:87–101. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Jeffrey BG, Mitchell DC, Gibson RA, Neuringer M. n-3 fatty acid deficiency alters recovery of the rod photoresponse in rhesus monkeys. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2002;43:2806–2814. [PubMed]
Parish CA, Hashimoto M, Nakanishi K, Dillon J, Sparrow J. Isolation and one-step preparation of A2E and iso-A2E, fluorophores from human retinal pigment epithelium. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1998;95:14609–14613. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1.
 
Timing of flash stimuli in a given trial of experiments 1 and 2. In experiment 1, the interval t probe between the conditioning flash and probe flash was varied among trials. Experiment 2 involved a fixed-interval t CT between the conditioning and test flashes, a fixed interval between the test and probe flashes (200 ms), and variation of the test flash strength.
Figure 1.
 
Timing of flash stimuli in a given trial of experiments 1 and 2. In experiment 1, the interval t probe between the conditioning flash and probe flash was varied among trials. Experiment 2 involved a fixed-interval t CT between the conditioning and test flashes, a fixed interval between the test and probe flashes (200 ms), and variation of the test flash strength.
Figure 2.
 
ERG responses to short-wavelength flashes. Labels identify the flash strength in sc cd s m−2. Results obtained from normal subject 104 (A), Stargardt patient 106 (B), and Stargardt patient 109 (C).
Figure 2.
 
ERG responses to short-wavelength flashes. Labels identify the flash strength in sc cd s m−2. Results obtained from normal subject 104 (A), Stargardt patient 106 (B), and Stargardt patient 109 (C).
Figure 3.
 
Recovery of the probe flash response in two normal subjects after a 670 sc cd s m−2 conditioning flash (C670) (experiment 1). (A) Raw responses obtained with probe flash presentation at defined times after C670. Data from normal subject 102. PAD: dark-adapted response to the probe flash delivered alone (dark-adapted probe-alone response). Numerical labels identify the interval t probe, in seconds, between the conditioning and probe flashes. Waveform LD: response to the photopically matched long-wavelength flash presented alone under dark-adapted conditions. Waveform L2: response to a photopically matched long-wavelength flash presented 2 seconds after C670. (B) Rod-isolated probe responses obtained from the waveforms in (A) after subtraction of the presumed cone-mediated component (waveform LD or L2). (C) Rod-isolated probe responses of (B) shown in peak-normalized form. The illustrated responses are those obtained with t probe ≥ 4 seconds. (DF) Data obtained from normal subject 104 with use of the C670 conditioning flash. Format of the data is similar to that in (AC).
Figure 3.
 
Recovery of the probe flash response in two normal subjects after a 670 sc cd s m−2 conditioning flash (C670) (experiment 1). (A) Raw responses obtained with probe flash presentation at defined times after C670. Data from normal subject 102. PAD: dark-adapted response to the probe flash delivered alone (dark-adapted probe-alone response). Numerical labels identify the interval t probe, in seconds, between the conditioning and probe flashes. Waveform LD: response to the photopically matched long-wavelength flash presented alone under dark-adapted conditions. Waveform L2: response to a photopically matched long-wavelength flash presented 2 seconds after C670. (B) Rod-isolated probe responses obtained from the waveforms in (A) after subtraction of the presumed cone-mediated component (waveform LD or L2). (C) Rod-isolated probe responses of (B) shown in peak-normalized form. The illustrated responses are those obtained with t probe ≥ 4 seconds. (DF) Data obtained from normal subject 104 with use of the C670 conditioning flash. Format of the data is similar to that in (AC).
Figure 4.
 
Recovery of the probe flash response in Stargardt patients 109 (AC) and 107 (DF) using the C670 conditioning flash. Format of data is similar to that of Figure 3 .
Figure 4.
 
Recovery of the probe flash response in Stargardt patients 109 (AC) and 107 (DF) using the C670 conditioning flash. Format of data is similar to that of Figure 3 .
Figure 5.
 
Recovery time course of the derived rod response after a conditioning flash. (A) Normal subjects. Open symbols: data obtained from four normal subjects after the 670 sc cd s m−2 conditioning flash. Among these four subjects, the retinal illuminance of this conditioning flash ranged from 3.4 × 104 to 5.3 × 104 sc td s. Filled symbols: data obtained from two normal subjects with the 67 sc cd s m−2 conditioning flash (corresponding retinal illuminance: 3.4 × 103 and 5.3 × 103 sc td s). (B) Stargardt patients. Open symbols: data obtained from four Stargardt patients with use of the 670 sc cd s m−2 conditioning flash (range of retinal illuminance: 3.0 × 104 to 3.4 × 104 sc td s). Filled symbols: data obtained from one patient with use of the 67 sc cd s m−2 conditioning flash (retinal illuminance: 3.4 × 103 sc td s).
Figure 5.
 
Recovery time course of the derived rod response after a conditioning flash. (A) Normal subjects. Open symbols: data obtained from four normal subjects after the 670 sc cd s m−2 conditioning flash. Among these four subjects, the retinal illuminance of this conditioning flash ranged from 3.4 × 104 to 5.3 × 104 sc td s. Filled symbols: data obtained from two normal subjects with the 67 sc cd s m−2 conditioning flash (corresponding retinal illuminance: 3.4 × 103 and 5.3 × 103 sc td s). (B) Stargardt patients. Open symbols: data obtained from four Stargardt patients with use of the 670 sc cd s m−2 conditioning flash (range of retinal illuminance: 3.0 × 104 to 3.4 × 104 sc td s). Filled symbols: data obtained from one patient with use of the 67 sc cd s m−2 conditioning flash (retinal illuminance: 3.4 × 103 sc td s).
Figure 6.
 
Rod-isolated probe responses obtained in paired-flash trials during recovery from a conditioning flash (experiment 2). (AC) Data from normal subject 102 obtained with test flashes over the range of 4.36 to 437 sc td s. (DF): Data from Stargardt patient 109 obtained with test flashes over the range of 3.46 to 347 sc td s. (GI) Data from Stargardt patient 110 obtained with test flashes over the range of 3.46 to 347 sc td s. Data in (A), (D), and (G) were obtained under dark-adapted conditions; data in (B), (E), and (H) were obtained with conditioning flash C67 and t CT = 2 seconds; data in (C), (F), and (I) were obtained with conditioning flash C670 and t CT = 9 seconds. PAD is the dark-adapted probe-alone response; PA is the prevailing probe-alone response. Numerical labels shown in (A), (D), and (G) identify test flash strengths associated with the indicated probe responses. Scale bars shown in (A), (D), and (G) refer, respectively, to data obtained from subjects 102, 109, and 110.
Figure 6.
 
Rod-isolated probe responses obtained in paired-flash trials during recovery from a conditioning flash (experiment 2). (AC) Data from normal subject 102 obtained with test flashes over the range of 4.36 to 437 sc td s. (DF): Data from Stargardt patient 109 obtained with test flashes over the range of 3.46 to 347 sc td s. (GI) Data from Stargardt patient 110 obtained with test flashes over the range of 3.46 to 347 sc td s. Data in (A), (D), and (G) were obtained under dark-adapted conditions; data in (B), (E), and (H) were obtained with conditioning flash C67 and t CT = 2 seconds; data in (C), (F), and (I) were obtained with conditioning flash C670 and t CT = 9 seconds. PAD is the dark-adapted probe-alone response; PA is the prevailing probe-alone response. Numerical labels shown in (A), (D), and (G) identify test flash strengths associated with the indicated probe responses. Scale bars shown in (A), (D), and (G) refer, respectively, to data obtained from subjects 102, 109, and 110.
Figure 7.
 
Amplitude-intensity functions obtained from normal subjects. Identical symbols represent data from the same subject. Data obtained from four subjects under dark-adapted (DA) conditions (A); from two subjects with conditioning flash C67 and t CT = 2 seconds (B); and from four subjects with conditioning flash C670 and t CT = 9 seconds (C). Curves plot equation 3 fitted to the data. (D) Fitted curves reproduced from (AC).
Figure 7.
 
Amplitude-intensity functions obtained from normal subjects. Identical symbols represent data from the same subject. Data obtained from four subjects under dark-adapted (DA) conditions (A); from two subjects with conditioning flash C67 and t CT = 2 seconds (B); and from four subjects with conditioning flash C670 and t CT = 9 seconds (C). Curves plot equation 3 fitted to the data. (D) Fitted curves reproduced from (AC).
Figure 8.
 
Amplitude-intensity functions obtained from Stargardt patients. The format is as in Figure 7 . Identical symbols indicate data obtained from the same subject. Data obtained from five patients in (A), four in (B), and four in (C). Curves plot equation 3 fitted to the data. Inverted triangles: data obtained from patient 108. In (A) the circled inverted triangle was omitted in the curve fitting. (D) Fitted curves reproduced from (AC).
Figure 8.
 
Amplitude-intensity functions obtained from Stargardt patients. The format is as in Figure 7 . Identical symbols indicate data obtained from the same subject. Data obtained from five patients in (A), four in (B), and four in (C). Curves plot equation 3 fitted to the data. Inverted triangles: data obtained from patient 108. In (A) the circled inverted triangle was omitted in the curve fitting. (D) Fitted curves reproduced from (AC).
Figure 9.
 
Psychophysically determined time course of dark adaptation in four normal subjects and four Stargardt patients after illumination estimated in normal subjects to produce approximately 67% bleaching. The four segmented lines indicate results from four visually normal subjects, one of whom was subject 105. Log threshold data obtained from a given subject are referred to the prebleaching (i.e., fully dark-adapted) threshold determined for that subject (horizontal dashed line).
Figure 9.
 
Psychophysically determined time course of dark adaptation in four normal subjects and four Stargardt patients after illumination estimated in normal subjects to produce approximately 67% bleaching. The four segmented lines indicate results from four visually normal subjects, one of whom was subject 105. Log threshold data obtained from a given subject are referred to the prebleaching (i.e., fully dark-adapted) threshold determined for that subject (horizontal dashed line).
Table 1.
 
Description of Subjects
Table 1.
 
Description of Subjects
Subject Number Age* Sex ABCA4 Variation Dark-Adapted Maximum Peak a-Wave Amplitude (μV), †
Normal subjects
 101 55 M −243
 102 37 F −410
 103 26 M −188
 104 23 F −397
 105 56 F −268
 111 29 F −362
 112 23 M −410
−325 ± 91
Stargardt patients
 106 50 F val849ala, arg2107his −201
 107 41 M gly1961glu, arg2077trp −306
 108 22 M ala60val, 1 bp ins codon 1513 −82
 109 34 M leu541pro/ala1038val,, ‡ gly1961glu −277
 110 51 M gly1961glu −191
−211 ± 87
Table 2.
 
Determinations of Logarithmic Weak-flash Sensitivity (log Itest,0.5)
Table 2.
 
Determinations of Logarithmic Weak-flash Sensitivity (log Itest,0.5)
Experimental Condition Log Itest,0.5 (log sc td s)
Normal Subjects Stargardt Patients
Dark adapted 1.48 ± 0.22 (4) 1.47 ± 0.32 (5)
C67, t CT = 2 s 1.99 ± 0.17 (2) 1.96 ± 0.32 (4)
C670, t CT = 9 s 1.93 ± 0.16 (4) 2.14 ± 0.30 (4)
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×