Among the 342 subjects whose parents completed the questionnaire, there were 18 sets of siblings: 2 children from each of 17 families and 3 children from one family. One sibling from each of these families was selected randomly for inclusion in the data analysis. Of the 323 subjects thus included, the mean (± SD) age was 8.7 ± 4.4 (median 8.0; range 1–20) years: 35 (10.8%) subjects were <4 years old; 120 (37.2%) subjects, from 4 to <8 years old; 85 (26.3%) subjects, from 8 to <12 years old; and 83 (25.7%) subjects, at least 12 years old. Males accounted for 165 (51.1%) subjects. 173 subjects (53.6%) were white; 82 (25.4%), African-American; 24 (7.4%), other; and 44 (13.6%), unknown due to absent information. A history of premature birth (<36 weeks gestational age) was recorded for 48 (14.9%) subjects.
Reflecting the population of a tertiary care pediatric ophthalmology clinic, half of the subjects (162/323, or 50.2%) had strabismus. The mean age of subjects with strabismus (8.3 ± 0.4 years) did not differ from that of subjects without strabismus (9.1 ± 0.4 years; P = 0.11). Of those 162 subjects with strabismus, 98 (60.5%) had accommodative esotropia, 42 (25.9%) had exotropia, and 22 (13.6%) had other types of strabismus including infantile esotropia, hypertropia, Duane syndrome, a congenital III nerve palsy and a congenital elevator palsy. Compared with those without strabismus, subjects with strabismus had a more hyperopic mean refraction (2.58 ± 0.23 vs. 0.19 ± 0.23 D; P < 0.0001), a lower prevalence of myopia (20 [12.4%] subjects vs. 43 [26.7%] subjects; P = 0.001), and a higher prevalence of hyperopia (101 [62.4%] subjects vs. 25 [15.5%] subjects; P < 0.0001). As expected, the high prevalence of hyperopia (90.8%) and lack of myopia among subjects with accommodative esotropia accounted for the hyperopic refractive shift in subjects with strabismus. Specifically, the mean refraction of subjects with accommodative esotropia (4.28 ± 0.25 D) differed significantly (P < 0.0001, for each comparison) from subjects without strabismus (0.19 ± 0.21 D), with exotropia (−0.20 ± 0.39 D), or with other deviations (0.29 ± 0.54 D). However, the latter three cohorts did not differ among themselves (P > 0.8).
Sixty-three (19.5%) subjects had myopic refractions, and myopic subjects were older (P < 0.0001) as a cohort than the study population overall. Whereas more weighted nearwork activity, a slightly higher BMI, and sleeping with less nighttime lighting at the time of the questionnaire appeared to be associated with myopia, each of these parameters was related to age; and the statistical significance of each association disappeared when adjusted for age (data not shown). Subjects born prematurely did not differ from those born at term with regard to mean refraction, the proportion with myopia or hyperopia, or the proportion with strabismus (data not shown). Nighttime lighting conditions before 2 years, gender, and race were not significantly associated with prevalent myopia (data not shown).
There were no statistically significant differences in age, gender, racial distribution, history of prematurity, weighted nearwork, or nighttime lighting conditions among children (n= 169) from families with at least one parent who ever smoked compared with those (n = 134) from families with nonsmoking parents (data not shown). Reduced birth weight was more likely to be associated with smoking during pregnancy by the mother (P = 0.04) but not the father (P = 0.12) individually, but with interacting effects from each parent (P = 0.007, linear trend test): both parents smoked, 2622 ± 200 g; mother only smoked, 3016 ± 186 g; father only smoked, 3130 ± 132 g; neither parent smoked, 3180 ± 59 g.