Useable latency data were obtained from 183 participants and useable velocity data from 188. In 176 participants, useable data were available for both latency and velocity. In these 176 participants, average age group values for saccadic reaction time (latency) decreased from 439 ms at 3 years to 172 ms at 14 years, followed by a period of relative stability to age 50, and finally a gradual increase to 264 ms at ≥80 years
(Fig. 1) . The univariate ANOVAs indicate that there is a significant effect (
P < 0.000) of age on latency for all the grouping methods
(Table 2) . Therefore, the unbalanced data and different grouping methods had little effect on the outcome. Post hoc tests for the Developmental group indicate that subjects <5 years of age had latencies that were significantly longer than those in all other age groups (
P < 0.000), 5- to <10-year-olds’ latencies were longer than all other age groups except <5 years and 60 to <90 years (
P < 0.000), and 35 to <60 years were shorter than 60 to <90 years (
P = 0.023). Post hoc tests on the Children group indicated that all ages up to 7 years were different (
P < 0.05) from all others except those on either side. For example, 4 years was not significantly different from 3 or 5 years, but was different from every other age group. Between 7 and 14 years of age, there were no significant differences between any of the age groups. Saccadic latency considered as a function of stimulus size shows a significant interaction (
n = 90,
P = 0.001;
n = 49,
P = 0.031) between age and stimulus size
(Table 3)such that latencies are longer for larger saccades in younger participants
(Fig. 2) .
When one examines the average gain of all the tested saccades, accuracy appears to be unchanged with approximately 10% hypometria through out the lifespan (probabilities vary from
P = 0.523 to
P = 0.851 depending on the grouping method;
Table 2 ). When accuracy is considered in relation to stimulus amplitude, it can be seen that balancing the participant numbers affected the outcome
(Table 3) . For the Balanced
n Subset
(Table 3) , there was a significant effect of age on saccadic accuracy (
P < 0.000) and on saccade size (
P = 0.001), as in the unbalanced data. However, the effect of size increased with age, and the interaction term is statistically significant (
P = 0.013) in the balanced
n subset. After the age of 50, saccades greater than 20° had hypometria exceeding 20% and the mean hypometria was nearly 40% by the ninth decade. Small saccades (<5°) were not nearly as significantly affected. Under the age of 20 there was a tendency to overshoot target steps less than 10° and to undershoot target steps greater than 10°
(Fig. 3) .
Average age group saccadic asymptotic peak velocity (
V max) increases during childhood from 446 deg/s at age 3 to a peak of 610 deg/s at 14 years and then gradually declines with age to approximately 345 deg/s for participants ≥80 years
(Fig. 4) . Similarly, the slope at the origin (
K) initially increased from 8.0 at 3 years to 11.1 at 14 years and then declined to 6.1 for participants ≥80 years. Given that both
V max and
K change and yet the data continue to be described by an exponential function, the indication is that the velocity changes are applicable to saccades of all sizes, not just the large ones.
The univariate ANOVAs indicate that there is a significant effect (
P < 0.05) of age on asymptotic peak velocity with all the grouping methods
(Table 2) , and the unbalanced data has minimal effect on the outcome. Post hoc pair-wise comparisons for the Developmental group (which has balanced subject numbers) indicate that the 10- to <15-year age group made faster saccades than all other age groups (
P < 0.05), suggesting a peak in asymptotic peak velocity between 10 and 15 years of age. Examining the Children grouping, we found a gradual increase in
V max with increasing age
(Fig. 5)between the ages of 3 and 14 years. The data can be fit with the linear function
y = 10.6
x + 448 (F = 18.05,
P < 0.000).