Abstract
purpose. Retinal illuminance varies with stimulus luminance, pupil size, eye size, and transmissivity of the ocular media. There are few published data to inform estimates of retinal illuminance in early infancy, and no concurrent studies of pupil size and ocular length have been described. The goals were to document simultaneously the measured ocular parameters in growing preterm infants and to estimate the potential errors associated with using either stimulus luminance or troland value as a proxy for retinal illuminance in this population.
methods. Ocular biometry including diameters of the eye, vitreous chamber depth (VCD) and dilated pupil diameter was performed on 111 occasions in 33 preterm infants aged between 30 and 55 weeks’ postmenstrual age.
results. Eye size increased rapidly between 30 and 55 postmenstrual weeks and was comparable to that of term-born infants. The ratio of dilated pupil area to VCD2 was highly variable. Retinal illuminance of the infant eye compared with adult eyes was underestimated by both stimulus luminance and troland values.
conclusions. Stimulus luminance and troland values cannot be used to infer retinal illuminance when comparing eyes of markedly differing sizes or transmissivities. Error in estimating retinal illuminance in prematurely born infants is inevitable because of uncertainty regarding media transmissivity, but this discrepancy can be minimized by using directly measured pupil diameter and data presented herein for eye size in this population.
Retinal illuminance is defined as the incident luminous flux per unit of retinal surface area (luminance per square meter).
1 Knowledge of retinal illuminance is an essential requirement for electrophysiological and psychophysical studies of infant vision. It varies with stimulus luminance, pupil size, eye size, and transmissivity of the ocular media. For brief flash stimuli, retinal illuminance is given by
\[E_{\mathrm{r}}{=}\left(\ \frac{A_{\mathrm{p}}}{d^{2}}\right)\ {\tau}_{{\lambda}}L,\]
where
E r is retinal illuminance (lumen seconds per square meter),
A p is pupil area (in square millimeters),
d is posterior nodal distance (PND) of the eye (in millimeters),
2 3 τ
λ is media transmissivity at wavelength λ (dimensionless), and
L is stimulus luminance (in candela-seconds per square meter).
4 5 In our laboratory, to study rod function in the newborn, short-wavelength flashes (λ ≈ 430 nm) are most commonly used.
It has been assumed that the infant and adult ratios of dark-adapted pupil area (
A p) to PND squared (
d 2) are approximately equal,
6 implying that a given stimulus luminance will deliver similar levels of retinal illuminance to the dark-adapted infant and adult eye
6 7 (also assuming intraocular light losses to be the same for the infant and adult eye). If these are reasonable assumptions, it would mean that stimulus luminance at the cornea could be used as a proxy for retinal illuminance, removing the need to account for either eye or pupil size. However, the constancy of the ratio
A p:
d 2 across individual subjects of differing ages and for pharmacologically dilated (rather than dark-adapted) pupils has not been confirmed.
Many infant rod electroretinographic studies (Fulton AB, et al.
IOVS 2004;45:ARVO E-Abstract 1353)
6 7 8 have used the troland as a proxy measure of retinal illuminance:
where
T is troland value (candela-seconds per square meter per square millimeter),
A p is pupil area (in square millimeters), and
L is stimulus luminance (candela-seconds per square meter).
9 Intraocular losses are not accounted for. The troland does not account for eye size and therefore may be an inaccurate proxy for retinal illuminance if eye size varies within the population studied—particularly relevant in a population in which the eye is growing rapidly, as in preterm infants.
We report the results of ocular biometry performed on preterm infants between 30 and 55 weeks’ postmenstrual age. Measurements included diameters of the eye, VCD, and dilated pupil diameter. VCD was taken to represent PND, since the nodal point used to define PND does not equate to any physical landmarks that could be imaged with ultrasound. It is likely that VCD and PND coincide within a fraction of a millimeter: according to eye models, although adult PND is 0.15 mm longer than the VCD,
10 the infant PND is 0.5 mm shorter than the VCD.
11 Because the resolution of an ultrasound scanner is ∼0.2 mm (Siemens Medical Solutions USA, personal communication, December 1998) and repeatability of ultrasonic ocular measures is ∼0.5 mm,
12 VCD is likely to be a very reasonable approximation of PND.
The purposes of the study were to compare ocular length with dilated pupil measurements in individual infants and to estimate the errors associated with using either stimulus luminance or trolands as a proxy for retinal illuminance in this population. Since study of rod function in the newborn in our laboratory is mostly achieved with short-wavelength flashes, media transmissivity has been specified at 430 nm throughout.
Biometry was performed to coincide with ROP screening examinations. For the purposes of the study, screening examinations began as soon as the infant was self-ventilating and clinically stable and were repeated at the discretion of the attending ophthalmologist (usually at 2-week intervals), until the retinas were fully vascularized. Further measurements were made at ∼50 postmenstrual weeks. For each examination, the infant’s pupils were dilated with 1 drop each of 0.5% cyclopentolate and 2.5% phenylephrine, repeated after 30 minutes. The study was approved by the local research ethics committee and informed, written consent was obtained from the parents. The study conformed to the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki.
In this study, eye growth was documented between 30 and 55 postmenstrual weeks in direct conjunction with measurements of dilated pupil diameter.
Our eye growth data agree closely with previous studies including data collected in utero, ex utero, and postmortem, with our 95% prediction intervals enclosing all values from other studies.
3 13 17 20 21 22 At term-corrected age, our prematurely born infants have eye measurements exactly matching published data from term-born infants
20 suggesting that preterm birth per se does not significantly affect eye growth.
Dilated pupil diameters in this study tended to be larger than reported elsewhere, and were also larger than pupil diameters reported in dark-adapted conditions.
23 The combination of anticholinergic (0.5% cyclopentolate) and sympathomimetic (2.5% phenylephrine) drops used in this study is more effective in preterm infants than anticholinergic drops alone.
24 In single-drug dilating protocols, smaller dilated pupil sizes have been reported in newborn term infants than in our term-aged prematurely born infants (5 mm dilation with 0.5% cyclopentolate
18 and 4.5–5 mm with 2.5% phenylephrine alone
19 ). This resulted in larger pupil area:VCD
2 ratios for the present study than in the term infant study.
18 It should be noted that, at term-corrected age, the preterm infants in this study were at least 10 weeks old: it is possible that chronological age, light exposure, or both exert a positive effect on maximum pupillary dilatation. This would also partly explain the tendency for the ratio of pupil area to VCD
2 to increase between 30 and 50 postmenstrual weeks in the group of typical preterm infants reported herein.
The second purpose of the study was to estimate the errors associated with using either stimulus luminance or troland value as a proxy for retinal illuminance in this preterm infant population. The uncertainty of a function (e.g., retinal illuminance as expressed in
equation 1 ) is a result of the uncertainties of its variables (e.g., the measured variability of VCD
2,
A p and τ
λ. Assuming a nominal stimulus, uncertainty in media transmissivity,
14 pupil area, and VCD
2 account for 34%, 46%, and 20% of variability in estimated retinal illuminance, respectively, in these infants.
Our findings have implications for the estimation of retinal illuminance. If the retinal illuminances received by eyes of differing sizes or (particularly) differing media transmissivities are to be compared, only units of retinal illuminance
(equation 1)will be accurate. In the more extreme case of comparing a newborn eye with an adult eye, using stimulus luminance or troland value as a proxy for retinal illuminance will underestimate true retinal illuminance by ∼0.48 or 0.65 log units, respectively, in the newborn eye relative to the adult eye. The effect is less when comparing eyes of more similar sizes, such as during the first 10 weeks of life, or when pupils are less aggressively dilated. Stimulus luminance and troland value underestimate retinal illuminance by 0.03 and 0.14 log units, respectively, in newborn eyes relative to 10-week postterm eyes. The effect would also be less for longer wavelengths of light where transmissivity of the infant and adult eye are similar. For example, stimulus luminance and troland value underestimate retinal illuminance by 0.30 and 0.47 log units, respectively, in newborn eyes relative to adult eyes for light of wavelength 550 nm, 0.18 log units less of an underestimate than with 430 nm light.
It has been assumed here that PND is approximately equal to VCD. If, as in adults,
10 PND is shorter than VCD, then the underestimate of retinal illuminance resulting from using luminance or troland units will be slightly less than just stated.
No data currently exist regarding optical media density in the preterm period. Hence, studies in this population must make assumptions about retinal illuminance. The error in calculating retinal illuminance can be minimized by using directly measured pupil diameter and eye-size data from the present study. Further studies of optical media density in infants, particularly during the preterm weeks, would also be of great use.
Supported by Grant 2501.2651.42 from the Royal National Institute for the Blind.
Submitted for publication May 3, 2007; revised July 18 and August 23, 2007; accepted December 4, 2007.
Disclosure:
H. Mactier, None;
S. Maroo, None;
M. Bradnam, None;
R. Hamilton, None
The publication costs of this article were defrayed in part by page charge payment. This article must therefore be marked “
advertisement” in accordance with 18 U.S.C. §1734 solely to indicate this fact.
Corresponding author: Helen Mactier, Neonatal Unit, Princess Royal Maternity, 8-16, Alexandra Parade, Glasgow G31 2ER, UK;
[email protected].
Table 1. Summary of Typical Dilated Pupil Diameters, VCD, and Media Transmissivity in Infants at 40 and 50 Postmenstrual Weeks and in Adults
Table 1. Summary of Typical Dilated Pupil Diameters, VCD, and Media Transmissivity in Infants at 40 and 50 Postmenstrual Weeks and in Adults
| Term-Aged Infant | 10-Weeks after Term | Adult | Notes/Sources |
Dilated pupil diameter (mm) | 7 (5.5 to 8.5) | 8 (6.5 to 9.5) | 8.5 (7 to 10) | Other dilating protocols achieve less dilation, ie around 5 mm in term-aged infants. 18 ;19 |
VCD (mm) | 10.2 (9 to 11.4) | 11.3 (10.1 to 12.5) | 16.7* (14.7 to 18.7)* | *From Bron AJ et al. 17 |
Ocular media transmissivity at 430 nm | 0.71† (0.86 to 0.59)† | 0.63‡ (0.54 to 0.71)‡ | 0.43‡ (0.47 to 0.39)‡ | †Extrapolated from Hansen and Fulton, 7 Figure 8–3. |
| | | | ‡Interpolated from Hansen and Fulton, 7 Figure 8–3. |
Log RI achieved by a 1-cd-s · m−2 stimulus | −0.59 (−0.22 to −0.96) | −0.62 (−0.30 to −0.94) | −1.07 (−0.76 to −1.37) | Range derived from range limits of pupil diameter, VCD, and transmissivity to give lowest and highest values (see Fig. 6 ) |
Log troland value achieved by a 1-cd-s · m−2 stimulus | 1.56 (1.38 to 1.75) | 1.69 (1.52 to 1.85) | 1.74 (1.59 to 1.90) | Range derived from range limits of pupil diameter to give lowest and highest values (Fig. 6) |
The authors thank the infants and their parents who participated in this study, and John Dudgeon, Consultant Ophthalmologist, and the nursing and medical staff in the pediatric department of The Queen Mother’s Hospital, Glasgow, for their patience and cooperation.
LentnerC eds. Geigy Scientific Tables, Volume 1: Units of Measurement, Body Fluids, Composition of Body, and Nutrition. 1981; 8th ed.Ciba-Geigy Ltd. Basel, Switzerland.
FledeliusHC. Pre-term delivery and the growth of the eye: an oculometric study of eye size around term-time. Acta Ophthalmol Suppl. 1992;204:10–15.
[PubMed]IsenbergSJ, NeumannD, CheongPYY, LingYLF, McCallLC, ZifferAJ. Growth of the internal and external eye in term and preterm infants. Ophthalmology. 1995;102:827–830.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]PughEN. Vision: physics and retinal physiology.AtkinsonRC HerrnsteinRJ LindzeyG LuceRD eds.2nd ed. Stevens’ Handbook of Experimental Psychology, Perception and Motivation. 1988;1:75–163.John Wiley & Sons New York.
WyszeckiG, StilesWS. Color Science. 2000; 2nd ed.John Wiley & Sons, Inc. New York.
BrownAM, DobsonV, MaierJ. Visual acuity of human infants at scotopic mesopic and photopic luminances. Vision Res. 1987;27(10)1845–1858.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]HansenRM, FultonAB. Development of scotopic retinal sensitivity.SimonsK eds. Early Visual Development, Normal and Abnormal. 1993;130–142.Oxford University Press New York.
HansenRM, FultonAB. Electroretinographic assessment of background adaptation in human infants. Vision Res. 1991;31:1501–1507.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]MakousW. Optics and photometry.CarpenterRHS RobsonJG eds. Vision Research: A Practical Guide to Laboratory Methods. 1999;1–49.Oxford University Press Oxford, UK.
GullstrandA. The optical system of the eye.von HelmholtzH eds. Physiological Optics. 1909;350–358.Voss Hamburg, Germany.
LotmarW. A theoretical model for the eye of a new-born infants. Albrecht von Graefes Arch Klin Exp Ophthalmol. 1976;198:179–185.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]BallantyneSA, O’NeillG, HamiltonR, HollmanAS. Observer variation in the sonographic measurement of optic nerve sheath diameter in normal adults. Eur J Ultrasound. 2002;15(3)145–149.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]RobbRM. Increase in retinal surface area during infancy and childhood. J Pediatr Ophthalmol Strabismus. 1982;19(4)16–20.
[PubMed]HansenRM, FultonAB. Psychophysical estimates of ocular media density of human infants. Vision Res. 1989;29:687–690.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]RobinsonR, O’KeefeM. Follow up study on premature infants with and without retinopathy of prematurity. Br J Ophthalmol. 1993;77:91–94.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]LawsDE, HaslettR, AshbyD, O’BrienC, ClarkD. Axial length biometry in infants with retinopathy of prematurity. Eye. 1994;8:427–430.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]BronAJ, TripathiRC, TripathiBJ. The eyeball and its dimensions. Wolff’s Anatomy of the Eye and Orbit. 1997; 8th ed. 211–232.Chapman & Hall London.
MalcolmCA, HamiltonR, McCullochDL, MontgomeryC, WeaverLT. Scotopic electroretinogram in term infants born of mothers supplemented with docosahexanoic acid during pregnancy. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2003;44(8)3685–3691.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]LynchMG, BrownRH, GoodeSM, SchoenwaldRD, ChienDS. Reduction of phenylephrine drop size in infants achieves equal dilation with decreased systemic absorption. Arch Ophthalmol. 1987;105(10)1364–1365.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]LarsenJS. The sagittal growth of the eye. III. Ultrasonic measurement of the posterior segment (axial length of the vitreous) from birth to puberty. Acta Ophthalmol. 1971;49:441–453.
LarsenJS. The sagittal growth of the eye. IV. Ultrasonic measurements of the axial length of the eye from birth to puberty. Acta Ophthalmol. 1971;49:873–886.
BirnholtzJC. Ultrasonic fetal ophthalmology. Early Hum Devel. 1985;12:199–209.
[CrossRef] SalapatekP, BanksMS. Infant sensory assessment: vision.MinifieFD LloydL eds. Communicative and Cognitive Abilities: Early Behavioural Assessment. 1978;61–106.University Park Press Baltimore.
FleckBW, DhillonB, MitchellA. Additive mydriatic effect of 2.5% phenylephrine and 0.5% tropicamide eyedrops in premature babies (Letter). J Pediatr Ophthalmol Strabismus. 1994;31:130–131.
[PubMed]