Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science Cover Image for Volume 50, Issue 4
April 2009
Volume 50, Issue 4
Free
Visual Psychophysics and Physiological Optics  |   April 2009
Low-Spatial-Frequency Channels and the Spatial Frequency-Doubling Illusion
Author Affiliations
  • Yanti Rosli
    From the Department of Biomedical Sciences, Faculty of Applied Health Sciences, Universiti Kebangsaan Malaysia (UKM), Wilayah Persekutuan, Kuala Lumpur, Malaysia; and the
    Centre for Visual Sciences and ARC Centre of Excellence in Vision Science, School of Biological Sciences, Australian National University (ANU), Canberra, Australia.
  • Suzanne M. Bedford
    Centre for Visual Sciences and ARC Centre of Excellence in Vision Science, School of Biological Sciences, Australian National University (ANU), Canberra, Australia.
  • Ted Maddess
    Centre for Visual Sciences and ARC Centre of Excellence in Vision Science, School of Biological Sciences, Australian National University (ANU), Canberra, Australia.
Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science April 2009, Vol.50, 1956-1963. doi:https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.08-1810
  • Views
  • PDF
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Yanti Rosli, Suzanne M. Bedford, Ted Maddess; Low-Spatial-Frequency Channels and the Spatial Frequency-Doubling Illusion. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2009;50(4):1956-1963. https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.08-1810.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract

purpose. This study examined the number and nature of spatiotemporal channels in the region where the frequency-doubling (FD) illusion would be expected to occur at eight locations spanning the central 30° of the visual field.

methods. The probability of seeing the FD illusion was examined in 17 subjects. Stimuli were presented at 5 octaves of low spatial frequencies, at each of seven flicker frequencies in the range 5.65 to 27.95 Hz. In a single trial, subjects matched the apparent spatial frequency of the flickering test pattern using a two-alternative, forced-choice method. Thirteen subjects were examined for stimuli presented at contrast 0.95. Three or four subjects were examined at each of the contrasts 0.2, 0.4, and 0.8. A factor analysis was conducted on the psychometric functions, quantifying the number and possible spatiotemporal tuning of neural channels present.

results. At contrast 0.95, three factors were able to explain 79.3% of the total variance in the psychometric responses to the 35 test conditions. This simple form of three broad spatiotemporal channels was also found at the other contrasts and in different subjects. The factor scores showed differential distribution of the factors onto the eight different visual field locations. Thus the expression of the three channels differed somewhat across the visual field.

conclusions. The results support earlier reports, that there are several low-spatial-frequency channels below 1 cyc/deg in the periphery. The results may have implications for the FDT and matrix perimeters.

Spatial frequency-doubling (FD) occurs when low-spatial-frequency sinusoidal gratings have their contrast rapidly modulated. Under those conditions, the spatial frequency appears to be approximately twice the original value. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 The FD effect has recently been used in the diagnosis of glaucoma, 8 9 with some evidence being presented that the FDT perimeter provides earlier diagnosis. 10 11 As discussed by Vallam and Metha, 7 there is still a lack of concordance regarding the mechanisms for the generation and the perception of the FD illusion. 
The evidence of the number of channels subserving low spatial frequencies also remains equivocal. Blakemore and Campbell 12 reported that spatially specific adaptation could not be found below 3 cyc/deg. Other research findings provide evidence for the existence of separately adaptable and maskable mechanisms from 0.2 to 1.0 cyc/deg. 13 14 15 16 17 There is also support for the notion that temporal sensitivity is subserved by only a few mechanisms, each having a quite broad bandwidth. 18 19 Hess and Plant 20 gave evidence of two broadly tuned and directionally selective temporal mechanisms, but Hammett and Smith 21 claimed that an additional temporal channel at low spatial frequencies is due to an artifact of differences in the rate of perceptual fading as a function of temporal frequency. 
Thus far, little work has been done to examine under what conditions the FD illusion is seen at different visual field locations, 7 22 and no examination of how many channels might contribute to perception in that spatiotemporal region has been conducted. Therefore, we examine those issues here using factor analysis of psychometric functions from many subjects. 6 23 24 25 26  
Methods
Subjects
Seventeen normal subjects were included in the study; 10 were women. The subjects’ ages ranged from 19 to 47 years. They had corrected visual acuities of 6/9 or better, with no history of clinical visual problems. No visual field defects were detected with an FDT perimeter in any subject (full threshold C-20 Program). The study adhered to the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki. Written informed consent was obtained from each subject. 
Visual Stimuli
Stimuli could be presented at one of eight visual field locations (Fig. 1) . The stimulus layout is related to that used in several studies including some of our published multifocal (mf)ERG studies using FD stimuli. 27 28 The layout also is identical with that of a companion dichoptic mfVEP study (Rosli Y, et al., manuscript in preparation) and a published mfVEP study on multiple sclerosis, 29 by using FD stimuli, and is similar to the layout of the FDT perimeter stimulus ensemble. A secondary objective of the present study therefore was to understand the channels possibly contributing to the mfERG and mfVEP signals in the same regions. Subjects viewed the video monitor at a distance of 30 cm as maintained by a chin rest, and a fixation spot was presented at the screen center. Each of the eight regions could display an achromatic (color temperature 6500° K) sinusoidal grating. The mean luminance was 45 cd/m2. The basal spatial frequencies used were 0.25 (inner regions) and 0.125 (outer regions) cyc/deg, and as such were roughly M-scaled (i.e., scaled with respect to cortical magnification). 30 Five different test ensembles were obtained by multiplying these basal frequencies by 0.25, 0.5, 1.0, 2.0, or 4.0. In all subjects, the right eye was tested, with the left eye covered by a patch. 
The psychophysical tests were conducted by using a temporal two-alternative, forced-choice method (2AFC) wherein a single presentation sequence consisted of three grating patterns that were shown consecutively (Fig. 2)at one of the eight possible locations. Within each of those presentation sequences, the contrast of the gratings was increased from 0 to the test contrast and then back down to 0. Each temporal window was based on a Blackman function. 6 The first pattern in the test sequence was a flickering test pattern of duration 2.5 seconds, followed by two nonflickering comparison patterns (Fig. 2 , insets). The flickering test gratings adopted one of the following sinusoidal contrast modulation frequencies: 5.65, 9.37, 13.08, 16.80, 20.52, 24.23 or 27.95 Hz. The two static comparison gratings were presented at contrasts designed to be subjectively similar to the flickering test patterns. 
The two static comparison patterns had either the same spatial frequency as the modulated test pattern (F) or twice of that (2F). Subjects indicated which of the comparison patterns had a spatial frequency that was closest to that of the test pattern. Thus, the resulting psychometric functions (e.g., Figs. 3 4 ) indicate the probability that the test pattern displayed a spatial frequency-doubled appearance. During the test procedure, the same task could be repeated if the subject was undecided or missed a stimulus. The presentation order of the F and 2F comparison patterns was randomized on all trials. 
The experiments were completed for grating contrasts of 0.2, 0.4, and 0.8 for three or four experienced subjects. Those psychometric functions were derived from 12 repeats of each of the 35 conditions for each region and subject (n = 3360 per subject). For the other experiments on 13 subjects, the flickering stimulus had a contrast of 0.95. From these 13 subjects the psychometric functions were derived from five repeats of each of the 35 conditions per region (n = 1400 per subject). For the contrast 0.95 stimuli, the comparison contrasts were 0.50, 0.43, 0.37, 0.30, 0.23, 0.17, and 0.10 for test modulations of 5.65 to 27.95 Hz. For lower flicker contrast the comparison gratings had their contrasts lowered proportionately. Commercial software was used for data acquisition, analysis, and display (MatLab; The MathWorks, Natick, MA). Stimulus generation was controlled by a graphics board (Vista; Truevision, Shadeland Station, IN). 
Factor Analysis
To estimate how many spatiotemporal channels might contribute to the psychometric functions, and their possible form, we applied factor analysis to our measured psychometric functions. This technique is now fairly standard and is used by ourselves 6 31 and others. 23 24 25 26 Of particular interest was the spatiotemporal form and number of channels present in the spatiotemporal domain in which the FD illusion has been reported. Also of interest was the relative influence of any such channels for each part of the visual field. In principle, relatively few channels may dominate the spatiotemporal domain, their influences possibly being revealed by the covariance (or cross-correlation) between the responses of different subjects to the relatively large numbers of test conditions involved (35 per stimulus region). Factor analysis attempts to exploit such covariance to provide information about any such channels. Simply stated, factor models can present simplified representations of the many data variables in terms of a small number of unobservable variables: In the present case, putative spatiotemporal channels determined the form of the measured psychometric functions. The basic equation of a factor analysis is:  
\[X{=}SL{^\prime}{+}E\]
where X is the data matrix; in the present case its rows were observations (psychometric functions) for the 35 spatiotemporal test conditions defining the columns. S is the matrix of factor scores (i.e., the estimated responses of the channels to each stimulus). 32 L is the matrix of factor loadings: the loadings are weights that are used to combine the factor scores into the psychometric variables (X), in this case representing the putative influences of the channels (factors) on the points of the spatiotemporal domain studied. 32 The loadings were computed from R, the correlation matrix of X by singular value decomposition (SVD), such that R = F × D × G providing:  
\[L{=}F{\times}\mathrm{diag}(D)^{0.5}\]
and the factor scores were given by  
\[S{=}XL(L{^\prime}L)^{{-}1}.\]
 
The columns of F (or G; they are equal in this case) are the eigenvectors of R and the elements of the diagonal matrix D are the eigenvalues. The eigenvalues provide the proportion of the variance in R provided by each eigenvector. Commonly, the eigenvalues are sorted and plotted. If some small number of factors determine the observed variables, then a sharp step should occur in this scree plot (see 5 6 Figs. 7G 7H ), the number of eigenvalues before the step providing an indication of the number (k) of factors to consider, the remaining small components being discarded as noise. Note that before equations 2and 3are computed, k columns of F and D, which provide the k highest eigenvalues, are selected to provide a k-factor model, the selected eigenvectors being the estimated factors; these truncated matrices are called F* and D*. We refer to this SVD-based method as a principle components (PC) factor analysis. 
The adequacy of these models is determined by computing the proportions of variance accounted for and the communalities. 32 The communalities are akin to r 2-values and indicate what proportion of each of the original variables is accounted for by the factor model; hence, the communalities for each of the points in the spatiotemporal domain examined should be uniformly high if the factor model accounts well for all the input variables (see Fig. 7D 7F ). Before forming L (equation 2)the loadings were rotated according the varimax method, 32 in which each rotated factor is a combination of only the highly interdependent input variables. That is, the factors tend to be loaded onto (i.e., reflect) the input variables. 
Note that in X the 2D psychometric functions were first drawn out into a vector of length 35, hence the analysis had no knowledge of the spatiotemporal relationship of points in the psychometric functions. Therefore, any factors that appear as reasonable looking spatiotemporal channels are due only to correlations in the data, and not the form of the analysis. 
A reconstructed correlation matrix R* = F*diag(D)F*′ can be estimated. 32 An adjunct to the PC method is a maximum-likelihood (ML) method which, via an iterative process, modifies F* such that the error between R and R* is minimized. 32 The ML method was used in this study as a confirmatory technique because it provides the possibility of less constrained (nonorthogonal) factors. Major differences in the outcomes for the two methods may call into question the validity of the PC method. 
Results
The mean psychometric functions (across 13 subjects) obtained at contrast 0.95 are shown in Figure 3 . The presented functions are based on a linear interpolation of the original mean psychometric functions to yield twice the density of points. This was reasonable, given that data from 13 subjects was used and so the final number of interpolated points is many fewer than the raw data. The mean psychometric functions (averaged across subjects and stimulus regions) for contrast 0.95 and the three lower contrasts are shown in Figure 4
A large number of psychometric functions from different subjects (and or stimulus regions) are required for factor analysis. In our initial factor analysis for the contrast 0.95 condition (13 subjects), all the regional psychometric functions were transformed into the rows of the matrix X in equation 1 ; thus the resulting factors and factor loadings contain information about what is happening across stimulus regions, as well as across subjects. We call this grand model type-G. A step in the scree plot indicated that six factors might be the minimum number to consider (Fig. 7G) . We found that even when six factors were included the factor loadings were complex (Fig. 5A)and the communalities were uniformly low (mean communalities, 0.59; median communalities, 0.61; Figs. 7C 7E ). The percentages of variance accounted for by the first six factors were 26.7, 9.97, 8.67, 5.77, 4.51, and 3.84 (total, 59.4%). Note that the maximum number of factors to consider is often thought to be those encompassing at least one variable’s worth of variance, in this case 1/35 = 2.86%, so the six-factor model was not unreasonable on these grounds. More factors would have to be included to improve the communalities and proportion of variance accounted for. A possible explanation of the need for a relatively complex model of six or more factors is that somewhat different mechanisms were operating in different regions; alternatively there could be large individual differences (variability) between subjects. 
To address these possibilities, we averaged the data either across subjects or across regions, before entering the results into the factor analysis. We refer to the models based on subject-wise averages as type-S models. In these models the factor loadings remained complex (Fig. 5B) , the proportion of variance accounted for was low (68%), and the communalities were unevenly distributed (not shown). Averaging across subjects eliminated between subject variance, so that cannot be the major source of the complexity of the type-G models. 
We next averaged the psychometric data across stimulus regions, referred to as type-R models (Fig. 6) . For six factors, the type-R models were found to produce uniformly high communalities (mean communalities, 0.92; median communalities 0.94), indicating that all points of the psychometric data were well explained by the factors. Further restricting the model to only three factors still yielded good communalities (mean communalities, 0.79; median communalities 0.83, and Figs. 7D 7F ). The proportions of variance from the three-factor type-R model were 46.8%, 18.7%, and 13.9% (total, 79.3%). The factor loadings for PC and ML factor models were very similar (cf. Figs. 6A 6B ). 
Figures 7Cto 7Fshow communalities and variances accounted for by type-G and -R models. The communalities obtained from the PC model (Figs. 7C 7D)and ML model (Figs. 7E 7F)are presented for point-wise comparison to the mean psychometric data for region 1 (Fig. 7A)and the same data for subject 1 (Fig. 7B) . Note that the each communality indicates the proportion for variance accounted for by the model in each of the 35 data points of the original psychometric data. The communalities were uniformly high for the data that were averaged across regions (type-R model, Fig. 7D 7F ), but not for type-G models (Fig. 7C 7E) , indicating that all parts of the psychometric functions were equally well accounted for by the factor models. Also, both the PC and ML models explain the psychometric the functions very similarly. The scree plots of percent eigenvalues indicate the proportion of the total variance in the data accounted for by each possible factor in the type-G and -R PC models. 
As alluded to earlier, a way to assess what is happening by region is to examine the factor scores for the type-G model (Fig. 8) . These indicate the visual field locations in which the six factor loadings (channel tunings) of Figure 5Ahave their strongest influence. Results from the first six factors are shown from left to right; one plot per factor. Strong negative or positive weights are indicated by either very dark (negative) or very light (positive) tones, while weights close to 0 are gray. 
We also constructed three factor type-R models of the data obtained at lower contrasts. The resultant factor loadings are shown with those of Figure 6A(for contrast 0.95) replotted for comparison (Fig. 9) . The results are quite consistent across contrasts, despite the fact that only one subject was common to the contrast 0.95 and lower contrast data sets, providing a degree of cross validation. 
Discussion
The initial type-G models were complex, with at least six factors required, and even those produced a somewhat complex distribution of factor scores (channel influences) across the visual field (Fig. 8) . The type-S and -R models were examined to understand the sources of that complexity. Models examining psychometric data averaged across subjects (type-S), examined variation across regions only; however, many factors were still required to explain a reasonable proportion of the variance, and the loadings were complex (Fig. 5B) . Evidently the initial complexity of type-G models was mainly due to differences across regions, rather than across subjects. Models examining psychometric data averaged across stimulus regions (type-R) examined variation across subjects only. In this case, the models were simple, with even three factors explaining approximately 80% of the variance. It is of importance that the putative channel influences, the factor loadings, were very similar in the different groups of subjects and at different contrasts (Fig. 9) . As mentioned in the Methods section, because the analysis had no information about the relative position of the psychometric data within the tested spatiotemporal domain; any reasonable-looking loadings are purely a product of the variance structure of data and not the model. 
Taken together, these data suggest that the basic mechanisms were simple but with small variations due to visual field location (Fig. 8) . A partial explanation for this variation might be that the different scaling of the gratings in inner and outer regions was not exactly M-scaled, despite detailed testing 30 ; however, differences also occurred at each eccentricity (Fig. 8) . The presumption in the scaling used here is that the magnification factor should be like that reported for parasol retinal ganglion cells, 30 which may not be correct. Even the results from type-G models indicate that no more than six independent mechanisms are needed to explain most of variance in the 280 data points of the psychometric functions. 
In examining Figure 9 , it is apparent that the parts of the spatiotemporal domain in which the FD illusion is seen are influenced by up to three independent mechanisms. The first factor loading is reminiscent of the high probability region in the psychometric data at low contrasts (cf. Fig. 4D ). Frequency doubling has been reported to be better seen as contrast is reduced to threshold levels, 2 6 at least for the spatial and temporal frequencies examined in those studies. The data of Figure 9provide a degree of cross-validation, given that only one subject was common between the group test and contrast 0.95, and the group tested at lower contrasts. 
Wilson et al. 33 reported that six spatial frequency-tuned mechanisms were required to fit central vision over the range 0.25 to 22.0 cyc/deg; three of those channels had their peaks under 4 cyc/deg, the lowest two possibly having more transient responses. Simpson and McFadden 26 found evidence of three broadly tuned bandpass channels peaking at 4, 8, and 16 cyc/deg. According to Hess and Howell, 34 the lowest spatial channel is reported to be approximately 0.2 cyc/deg rather than 2 cyc/deg, as reported earlier. 12 Although there is a growing support that spatial sensitivity is subserved by a moderately large number of detectors, each with relatively limited spatial bandwidths, temporal sensitivity is thought to subserved by only a few detectors with broader temporal bandwidths. 19  
It has been postulated that temporal vision is subserved by only two broadly tuned detectors labeled for temporal frequency. 35 36 A third high-temporal-frequency detector was suggested after discovering that temporal frequency discrimination using 2-cyc/deg gratings supports the existence of two temporally selective mechanisms, and there may be an additional higher temporal frequency mechanism at approximately 0.2 cyc/deg. 20 Other reports suggest three or more temporal selective channels exist. 18 19 37 38 This notion was challenged by Hammett and Smith 21 who suggested that perceptual fading might account for the improvement of discrimination at high temporal frequencies. Results from Waugh and Hess 39 suggest otherwise; improvement in temporal discrimination still persists without perceptual fading. It is interesting to see that perhaps these high temporal detectors for low spatial frequencies, as suggested by Hess and Plant, 20 are displayed in Figures 5 and 6
In conclusion, our results support earlier reports that there are at least two spatial frequency channels below 1 cyc/deg, 15 17 and lend support for the third high-temporal-frequency channel suggested by other studies, 18 19 20 as opposed to the postulate that temporal vision is subserved by only two broadly tuned temporal channels. 35 36 In addition, each visual field region responds somewhat differently, even regions at the same eccentricity. Thus far, estimating channel characteristics has tended to exploit independent variations across subjects, and so the present results suggest the possibility of exploiting within subject variation across parts of the visual field to infer channels within subjects. The results may be important for clinical devices such as the FDT perimeter which tacitly assumes that similar mechanisms operate at all visual field locations. One option would be to examine the sensitivity and specificity of the different mechanisms implied in this study. Such an examination could lead to the use of stimuli that scaled with visual field eccentricity or that have different flicker rates in particular field locations. That could be a problem for a perimeter because the different sorts of stimuli might be confusing to subjects, and the potential benefits might be swamped by increased variability. This is not an obstacle to multifocal methods in which visual responses are recorded objectively, and multifocal PERGs using M-scaled FD stimuli have been demonstrated. 27 28  
 
Figure 1.
 
An illustration of the visual stimulus layout comprising eight possible stimulus regions, only one of which was presented in any trial. The gratings are presented at different contrasts to permit the eight regions to be distinguished. The small cross indicates the fixation spot presented at the screen center. The symbols r1 to r8 indicate the numbering scheme of the regions.
Figure 1.
 
An illustration of the visual stimulus layout comprising eight possible stimulus regions, only one of which was presented in any trial. The gratings are presented at different contrasts to permit the eight regions to be distinguished. The small cross indicates the fixation spot presented at the screen center. The symbols r1 to r8 indicate the numbering scheme of the regions.
Figure 2.
 
Illustration of the temporal evolution of the 2AFC test sequences. The flicker test was displayed for 2.5 seconds, followed by two optional stimuli. The subject’s task was to choose which of the two test pattern options had the subjectively closest spatial frequency to that of the flickering test pattern. For esthetic purposes the sinusoidal modulation frequency of the test grating illustrated varies more slowly than any used in the tests. Top insets: each panel illustrates the visual appearance of the stimuli in the 2AFC test sequences. The left grating has noise added to it, simulating the appearance of a test pattern modulated by a high temporal frequency. The middle and right gratings show static patterns with contrast that has been determined to be subjectively similar to that of the flickering test pattern.
Figure 2.
 
Illustration of the temporal evolution of the 2AFC test sequences. The flicker test was displayed for 2.5 seconds, followed by two optional stimuli. The subject’s task was to choose which of the two test pattern options had the subjectively closest spatial frequency to that of the flickering test pattern. For esthetic purposes the sinusoidal modulation frequency of the test grating illustrated varies more slowly than any used in the tests. Top insets: each panel illustrates the visual appearance of the stimuli in the 2AFC test sequences. The left grating has noise added to it, simulating the appearance of a test pattern modulated by a high temporal frequency. The middle and right gratings show static patterns with contrast that has been determined to be subjectively similar to that of the flickering test pattern.
Figure 3.
 
Psychometric functions obtained at a contrast of 0.95 averaged across the 13 subjects. The functions describe the probability of seeing the frequency-doubled illusion, light shades represent higher probabilities of seeing the illusion (see calibration bar at bottom). Contours are in steps of 0.1, the highest being 0.9. The symbols r1 to r8 indicate the eight stimulus regions, as indicated in Figure 1 . The layout of the figure roughly follows that of the stimulus ensemble, with data for the inner four regions presented centrally. The abscissa labels correspond to the spatial frequencies of the inner regions, the outer regions having one half those frequencies.
Figure 3.
 
Psychometric functions obtained at a contrast of 0.95 averaged across the 13 subjects. The functions describe the probability of seeing the frequency-doubled illusion, light shades represent higher probabilities of seeing the illusion (see calibration bar at bottom). Contours are in steps of 0.1, the highest being 0.9. The symbols r1 to r8 indicate the eight stimulus regions, as indicated in Figure 1 . The layout of the figure roughly follows that of the stimulus ensemble, with data for the inner four regions presented centrally. The abscissa labels correspond to the spatial frequencies of the inner regions, the outer regions having one half those frequencies.
Figure 4.
 
Comparisons of psychometric functions averaged across subjects and the eight visual field regions for contrasts 0.95, 0.8, 0.4, and 0.2 (AD, respectively). The regions that are lightly shaded correspond to the regions where the probability of seeing the frequency-doubling illusion is the highest (see calibration bar at the bottom). As the contrast decreases, the region where the FD is seen well contracts to the left toward the lowest spatial frequencies.
Figure 4.
 
Comparisons of psychometric functions averaged across subjects and the eight visual field regions for contrasts 0.95, 0.8, 0.4, and 0.2 (AD, respectively). The regions that are lightly shaded correspond to the regions where the probability of seeing the frequency-doubling illusion is the highest (see calibration bar at the bottom). As the contrast decreases, the region where the FD is seen well contracts to the left toward the lowest spatial frequencies.
Figure 5.
 
Factor loadings indicating at what spatial and temporal frequencies each factor strongly influenced the psychometric data. (A) When all the data from each region and subject are entered in a grand analysis (type-G model), at least six factors are necessary to explain a reasonable proportion of the variance (see scree plot, Fig. 7G ). (B) Factor loadings were obtained from data averaged across the 13 subjects (type-S model). For both model types even six factors produced only fair communalities (see Figs. 7C 7E ).
Figure 5.
 
Factor loadings indicating at what spatial and temporal frequencies each factor strongly influenced the psychometric data. (A) When all the data from each region and subject are entered in a grand analysis (type-G model), at least six factors are necessary to explain a reasonable proportion of the variance (see scree plot, Fig. 7G ). (B) Factor loadings were obtained from data averaged across the 13 subjects (type-S model). For both model types even six factors produced only fair communalities (see Figs. 7C 7E ).
Figure 6.
 
Three sets of factor loadings of data from 13 subjects averaged across regions (type-R model). (A) Top row: the loadings, indicating the influences of each factor at each spatial and temporal frequency pair for PC factors. (B) Bottom row: ML loadings. The ML loadings for factors 2 and 3 have been switched (compared to the order that the proportion of variance would indicate), and the sign of the middle set of data has being inverted, to match their PC counterparts. The median communalities for the PC factors were 0.92 and 0.94 and were uniformly high for both PC and ML factors (e.g., Figs. 7C 7D 7E 7F ). With three factors, as shown here, the values were still 0.79 and 0.83, respectively.
Figure 6.
 
Three sets of factor loadings of data from 13 subjects averaged across regions (type-R model). (A) Top row: the loadings, indicating the influences of each factor at each spatial and temporal frequency pair for PC factors. (B) Bottom row: ML loadings. The ML loadings for factors 2 and 3 have been switched (compared to the order that the proportion of variance would indicate), and the sign of the middle set of data has being inverted, to match their PC counterparts. The median communalities for the PC factors were 0.92 and 0.94 and were uniformly high for both PC and ML factors (e.g., Figs. 7C 7D 7E 7F ). With three factors, as shown here, the values were still 0.79 and 0.83, respectively.
Figure 7.
 
Variances explained by type-G and -R models. Top row: exemplary psychophysical results for comparison with the corresponding communalities for type-G and -R models. Communalities indicate the proportion of variance explained for each point in the psychometric function; uniform communalities mean that the factors explain the psychometric function well at all points. (A) Results from region 1 for data averaged across repeats at contrast 0.95. (B) Psychometric data from region 1 of subject 1. The communalities for the PC (C, D) and ML factor analyses (E, F) uniformly explain the points of the whole psychometric function, both where FD is seen and where it is not (cf. A, B and CF). The communalities are higher in the type-R models. Scree plots are shown indicating the proportion of variance in the correlation matrix explained by each PC factors for six (G) and three (H) component models. In both cases there is a distinct step after three factors, indicating that these first three are not noise.
Figure 7.
 
Variances explained by type-G and -R models. Top row: exemplary psychophysical results for comparison with the corresponding communalities for type-G and -R models. Communalities indicate the proportion of variance explained for each point in the psychometric function; uniform communalities mean that the factors explain the psychometric function well at all points. (A) Results from region 1 for data averaged across repeats at contrast 0.95. (B) Psychometric data from region 1 of subject 1. The communalities for the PC (C, D) and ML factor analyses (E, F) uniformly explain the points of the whole psychometric function, both where FD is seen and where it is not (cf. A, B and CF). The communalities are higher in the type-R models. Scree plots are shown indicating the proportion of variance in the correlation matrix explained by each PC factors for six (G) and three (H) component models. In both cases there is a distinct step after three factors, indicating that these first three are not noise.
Figure 8.
 
The mean factor scores across subjects from the type-G analysis. Strong weights are indicated by either very dark or very light tones, while weights close to 0 are gray (see calibration bar). Although the mean factor scores are broadly similar for a given factor, there are differences across quadrants and centrally versus peripherally. This result suggests that the spatiotemporal influences of the factors, as indicated by the factor loadings, vary across the visual field.
Figure 8.
 
The mean factor scores across subjects from the type-G analysis. Strong weights are indicated by either very dark or very light tones, while weights close to 0 are gray (see calibration bar). Although the mean factor scores are broadly similar for a given factor, there are differences across quadrants and centrally versus peripherally. This result suggests that the spatiotemporal influences of the factors, as indicated by the factor loadings, vary across the visual field.
Figure 9.
 
Comparisons of factor loadings for data obtained at different contrasts for type-R models. The loadings suggest that as contrast decreases (from contrast 0.95, top row), the loadings remain similar, supporting the existence of three independent channels. The sign of the third set of loadings was allowed to vary. The numbers of subjects from (A) to (D) were: 13, 4, 4, and 3. Note that only one subject was common to all four data sets, making the similarity between the loadings all the more remarkable.
Figure 9.
 
Comparisons of factor loadings for data obtained at different contrasts for type-R models. The loadings suggest that as contrast decreases (from contrast 0.95, top row), the loadings remain similar, supporting the existence of three independent channels. The sign of the third set of loadings was allowed to vary. The numbers of subjects from (A) to (D) were: 13, 4, 4, and 3. Note that only one subject was common to all four data sets, making the similarity between the loadings all the more remarkable.
The authors thank Andrew James for helpful discussions. 
KellyD. Frequency doubling in visual responses. J Opt Soc Am. 1966;56:1628–1633. [CrossRef]
KulikowskiJ. Apparent fineness of briefly presented gratings: balance between movement and pattern channels. Vision Res. 1975;15:673–680. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
KellyD. Nonlinear visual responses to flickering sinusoidal gratings. J Ophthalmol Soc Am. 1981;71:1051–1055.
ParkerA. Shifts in perceived periodicity induced by temporal modulation and their influence on the spatial frequency tuning of two aftereffects. Vision Res. 1981;21:1739–1747. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
ParkerA. The effects of temporal modulation on the perceived spatial structure of sine wave gratings. Perception. 1983;12:663–682. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
MaddessT, KulikowskiJ. Apparent fineness of stationary compound gratings. Vision Res. 1999;39:3404–3416. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
VallamK, PataridisI, MethaAB. Frequency-doubling illusion under scotopic illumination and in peripheral vision. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2007;48:3413–3418. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
JohnsonCA, SamuelsS. Screening for glaucomatous visual field loss with frequency doubling perimetry. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1997;38:413–425. [PubMed]
MaddessT, HenryG. Performance of non-linear visual units in ocular hypertension and glaucoma. Clin Vis Sci. 1992;7:371–383.
KimTW, ZangwillLM, BowdC, SamplePA, ShahN, WeinrebRN. Retinal nerve fiber layer damage as assessed by optical coherence tomography in eyes with a visual field defect detected by frequency doubling technology perimetry but not by standard automated perimetry. Ophthalmology. 2007;114:1053–1057. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
MedeirosFA, SamplePA, WeinrebRN. Frequency doubling technology perimetry abnormalities as predictors of glaucomatous visual field loss. Am J Ophthalmol. 2004;137:863–871. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
BlakemoreC, CampbellF. On the existence of neurons in the human visual system selectively sensitive to the orientation and size of retinal images. J Physiol. 1969;203:237–260. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
StromeyerC, JuleszB. Spatial frequency masking in vision: critical bands and spread of masking. J Ophthalmol Soc Am. 1972;62:1221–1232.
TolhurstD. Separate channels for the analysis of the shape and movement of a moving visual stimulus. J Physiol. 1973;231:385–402. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
KrandaK, KulikowskiJ. Adaptation to coarse gratings under scotopic and photopic conditions. J Physiol. 1976;257:35P–36P. [PubMed]
GreenM, CorwinT, SchorC. Spatiotemporal variations in the square/sine ration: evidence of independent channels at low spatial frequencies. Vision Res. 1981;21:423–425. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
StromeyerC, KleinS, DawsonB, SpillmanL. Low spatial-frequency channels in human vision: adaptation and masking. Vision Res. 1982;22:225–233. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
MandlerMB. Temporal frequency discrimination above threshold. Vision Res. 1984;24:1873–1880. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
MandlerMB, MakousW. A three channel model of temporal frequency perception. Vision Res. 1984;24:1881–1887. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
HessR, PlantG. Temporal frequency discrimination in human vision: evidence for an additional mechanism in the low spatial and high temporal frequency region. Vision Res. 1985;25:1493–1500. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
HammettS, SmithA. Two temporal channels or three? A re-evaluation. Vision Res. 1992;32:285–291. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
BedfordS, MaddessT, RoseK, JamesA. Correlations between observability of the spatial frequency doubled illusion and a multiregion pattern electroretinogram. Aust N Z J Ophthalmol. 1997;25:S91–S93. [PubMed]
SekulerR, WilsonH, OwsleyC. Structural modeling of spatial vision. Vision Res. 1984;24:689–700. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
PeterzellDH, WernerJS, KaplanPS. Individual differences in contrast sensitivity functions: the first four months of life in humans. Vision Res. 1993;33:381–396. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
PeterzellD, TellerD. Individual differences in contrast sensitivity functions: the lowest spatial frequency channels. Vision Res. 1996;36:3077–3085. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
SimpsonWA, McFaddenSM. Spatial frequency channels derived from individual differences. Vision Res. 2005;45:2723–2727. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
MaddessT, JamesC, GoldbergI, WineS, DobinsonJ. A spatial frequency-doubling illusion based pattern electroretinogram for glaucoma. Invest Ophthlalmol Vis Sci. 2000;41:3818–3826.
MaddessT, JamesAC, GoldbergI, WineS, DobinsonJ. Comparing a parallel PERG, automated perimetry, and frequency-doubling thresholds. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2000;41:3827–3832. [PubMed]
RuseckaiteR, MaddessT, DantaG, JamesAC. Frequency doubling illusion VEPs and automated perimetry in multiple sclerosis. Doc Ophthalmol. 2006;113:29–41. [PubMed]
MaddessT, HemmiJ, JamesAC. Evidence for spatial aliasing effects in the Y-like cells of the magnocellular visual pathway. Vision Res. 1998;38:1843–1859. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
MaddessT, RascherU, SiebkeK, LüttgeU, OsmondB. Spatio-temporal variations in chlorophyll fluorescence during the phases of CAM, and during endogenous rhythms in continuous light, in thick leaves of Kalanchoë daigremontiana. Plant Biol. 2002;4:446–455. [CrossRef]
ReymentR, JoreskogK. Applied Factor Analysis in the Natural Sciences. 1996;371.Cambridge University Press Cambridge, UK.
WilsonH, McFarlaneD, PhilipsG. Spatial frequency tuning of orientation selective units estimated by oblique masking. Vision Res. 1983;23:873–882. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
HessR, HowellE. Detection of low spatial frequencies: A single filter or multiple filters?. Opthalmol Physiol Optom. 1988;8:378–385. [CrossRef]
WatsonA, RobsonJ. Discrimination at threshold: Labelled detectors in human vision. Vision Res. 1981;20:314–347.
ThompsonP. Discrimination of moving gratings at and above detection threshold. Vision Res. 1983;23:1533–1538. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
SnowdenRJ, HessRH. Temporal properties of human visual, number, shapes and spatial covariation. Vision Res. 1992;32:47–59. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
HessRH, SnowdenRJ. Temporal frequency filters in the human peripheral visual field. Vision Res. 1992;32:61–74. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
WaughS, HessRH. Suptrathreshold temporal-frequency discrimination in the fovea and the periphery. J Opt Soc Am A. 1994;11:1199–1212. [CrossRef]
Figure 1.
 
An illustration of the visual stimulus layout comprising eight possible stimulus regions, only one of which was presented in any trial. The gratings are presented at different contrasts to permit the eight regions to be distinguished. The small cross indicates the fixation spot presented at the screen center. The symbols r1 to r8 indicate the numbering scheme of the regions.
Figure 1.
 
An illustration of the visual stimulus layout comprising eight possible stimulus regions, only one of which was presented in any trial. The gratings are presented at different contrasts to permit the eight regions to be distinguished. The small cross indicates the fixation spot presented at the screen center. The symbols r1 to r8 indicate the numbering scheme of the regions.
Figure 2.
 
Illustration of the temporal evolution of the 2AFC test sequences. The flicker test was displayed for 2.5 seconds, followed by two optional stimuli. The subject’s task was to choose which of the two test pattern options had the subjectively closest spatial frequency to that of the flickering test pattern. For esthetic purposes the sinusoidal modulation frequency of the test grating illustrated varies more slowly than any used in the tests. Top insets: each panel illustrates the visual appearance of the stimuli in the 2AFC test sequences. The left grating has noise added to it, simulating the appearance of a test pattern modulated by a high temporal frequency. The middle and right gratings show static patterns with contrast that has been determined to be subjectively similar to that of the flickering test pattern.
Figure 2.
 
Illustration of the temporal evolution of the 2AFC test sequences. The flicker test was displayed for 2.5 seconds, followed by two optional stimuli. The subject’s task was to choose which of the two test pattern options had the subjectively closest spatial frequency to that of the flickering test pattern. For esthetic purposes the sinusoidal modulation frequency of the test grating illustrated varies more slowly than any used in the tests. Top insets: each panel illustrates the visual appearance of the stimuli in the 2AFC test sequences. The left grating has noise added to it, simulating the appearance of a test pattern modulated by a high temporal frequency. The middle and right gratings show static patterns with contrast that has been determined to be subjectively similar to that of the flickering test pattern.
Figure 3.
 
Psychometric functions obtained at a contrast of 0.95 averaged across the 13 subjects. The functions describe the probability of seeing the frequency-doubled illusion, light shades represent higher probabilities of seeing the illusion (see calibration bar at bottom). Contours are in steps of 0.1, the highest being 0.9. The symbols r1 to r8 indicate the eight stimulus regions, as indicated in Figure 1 . The layout of the figure roughly follows that of the stimulus ensemble, with data for the inner four regions presented centrally. The abscissa labels correspond to the spatial frequencies of the inner regions, the outer regions having one half those frequencies.
Figure 3.
 
Psychometric functions obtained at a contrast of 0.95 averaged across the 13 subjects. The functions describe the probability of seeing the frequency-doubled illusion, light shades represent higher probabilities of seeing the illusion (see calibration bar at bottom). Contours are in steps of 0.1, the highest being 0.9. The symbols r1 to r8 indicate the eight stimulus regions, as indicated in Figure 1 . The layout of the figure roughly follows that of the stimulus ensemble, with data for the inner four regions presented centrally. The abscissa labels correspond to the spatial frequencies of the inner regions, the outer regions having one half those frequencies.
Figure 4.
 
Comparisons of psychometric functions averaged across subjects and the eight visual field regions for contrasts 0.95, 0.8, 0.4, and 0.2 (AD, respectively). The regions that are lightly shaded correspond to the regions where the probability of seeing the frequency-doubling illusion is the highest (see calibration bar at the bottom). As the contrast decreases, the region where the FD is seen well contracts to the left toward the lowest spatial frequencies.
Figure 4.
 
Comparisons of psychometric functions averaged across subjects and the eight visual field regions for contrasts 0.95, 0.8, 0.4, and 0.2 (AD, respectively). The regions that are lightly shaded correspond to the regions where the probability of seeing the frequency-doubling illusion is the highest (see calibration bar at the bottom). As the contrast decreases, the region where the FD is seen well contracts to the left toward the lowest spatial frequencies.
Figure 5.
 
Factor loadings indicating at what spatial and temporal frequencies each factor strongly influenced the psychometric data. (A) When all the data from each region and subject are entered in a grand analysis (type-G model), at least six factors are necessary to explain a reasonable proportion of the variance (see scree plot, Fig. 7G ). (B) Factor loadings were obtained from data averaged across the 13 subjects (type-S model). For both model types even six factors produced only fair communalities (see Figs. 7C 7E ).
Figure 5.
 
Factor loadings indicating at what spatial and temporal frequencies each factor strongly influenced the psychometric data. (A) When all the data from each region and subject are entered in a grand analysis (type-G model), at least six factors are necessary to explain a reasonable proportion of the variance (see scree plot, Fig. 7G ). (B) Factor loadings were obtained from data averaged across the 13 subjects (type-S model). For both model types even six factors produced only fair communalities (see Figs. 7C 7E ).
Figure 6.
 
Three sets of factor loadings of data from 13 subjects averaged across regions (type-R model). (A) Top row: the loadings, indicating the influences of each factor at each spatial and temporal frequency pair for PC factors. (B) Bottom row: ML loadings. The ML loadings for factors 2 and 3 have been switched (compared to the order that the proportion of variance would indicate), and the sign of the middle set of data has being inverted, to match their PC counterparts. The median communalities for the PC factors were 0.92 and 0.94 and were uniformly high for both PC and ML factors (e.g., Figs. 7C 7D 7E 7F ). With three factors, as shown here, the values were still 0.79 and 0.83, respectively.
Figure 6.
 
Three sets of factor loadings of data from 13 subjects averaged across regions (type-R model). (A) Top row: the loadings, indicating the influences of each factor at each spatial and temporal frequency pair for PC factors. (B) Bottom row: ML loadings. The ML loadings for factors 2 and 3 have been switched (compared to the order that the proportion of variance would indicate), and the sign of the middle set of data has being inverted, to match their PC counterparts. The median communalities for the PC factors were 0.92 and 0.94 and were uniformly high for both PC and ML factors (e.g., Figs. 7C 7D 7E 7F ). With three factors, as shown here, the values were still 0.79 and 0.83, respectively.
Figure 7.
 
Variances explained by type-G and -R models. Top row: exemplary psychophysical results for comparison with the corresponding communalities for type-G and -R models. Communalities indicate the proportion of variance explained for each point in the psychometric function; uniform communalities mean that the factors explain the psychometric function well at all points. (A) Results from region 1 for data averaged across repeats at contrast 0.95. (B) Psychometric data from region 1 of subject 1. The communalities for the PC (C, D) and ML factor analyses (E, F) uniformly explain the points of the whole psychometric function, both where FD is seen and where it is not (cf. A, B and CF). The communalities are higher in the type-R models. Scree plots are shown indicating the proportion of variance in the correlation matrix explained by each PC factors for six (G) and three (H) component models. In both cases there is a distinct step after three factors, indicating that these first three are not noise.
Figure 7.
 
Variances explained by type-G and -R models. Top row: exemplary psychophysical results for comparison with the corresponding communalities for type-G and -R models. Communalities indicate the proportion of variance explained for each point in the psychometric function; uniform communalities mean that the factors explain the psychometric function well at all points. (A) Results from region 1 for data averaged across repeats at contrast 0.95. (B) Psychometric data from region 1 of subject 1. The communalities for the PC (C, D) and ML factor analyses (E, F) uniformly explain the points of the whole psychometric function, both where FD is seen and where it is not (cf. A, B and CF). The communalities are higher in the type-R models. Scree plots are shown indicating the proportion of variance in the correlation matrix explained by each PC factors for six (G) and three (H) component models. In both cases there is a distinct step after three factors, indicating that these first three are not noise.
Figure 8.
 
The mean factor scores across subjects from the type-G analysis. Strong weights are indicated by either very dark or very light tones, while weights close to 0 are gray (see calibration bar). Although the mean factor scores are broadly similar for a given factor, there are differences across quadrants and centrally versus peripherally. This result suggests that the spatiotemporal influences of the factors, as indicated by the factor loadings, vary across the visual field.
Figure 8.
 
The mean factor scores across subjects from the type-G analysis. Strong weights are indicated by either very dark or very light tones, while weights close to 0 are gray (see calibration bar). Although the mean factor scores are broadly similar for a given factor, there are differences across quadrants and centrally versus peripherally. This result suggests that the spatiotemporal influences of the factors, as indicated by the factor loadings, vary across the visual field.
Figure 9.
 
Comparisons of factor loadings for data obtained at different contrasts for type-R models. The loadings suggest that as contrast decreases (from contrast 0.95, top row), the loadings remain similar, supporting the existence of three independent channels. The sign of the third set of loadings was allowed to vary. The numbers of subjects from (A) to (D) were: 13, 4, 4, and 3. Note that only one subject was common to all four data sets, making the similarity between the loadings all the more remarkable.
Figure 9.
 
Comparisons of factor loadings for data obtained at different contrasts for type-R models. The loadings suggest that as contrast decreases (from contrast 0.95, top row), the loadings remain similar, supporting the existence of three independent channels. The sign of the third set of loadings was allowed to vary. The numbers of subjects from (A) to (D) were: 13, 4, 4, and 3. Note that only one subject was common to all four data sets, making the similarity between the loadings all the more remarkable.
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×