March 2009
Volume 50, Issue 3
Free
Eye Movements, Strabismus, Amblyopia and Neuro-ophthalmology  |   March 2009
Disparity Tuning of Binocular Facilitation and Suppression after Normal versus Abnormal Visual Development
Author Affiliations
  • Anthony M. Norcia
    From the Smith-Kettlewell Eye Research Institute, San Francisco, California; and the
  • Julia Hale
    Bristol Eye Hospital, Bristol, United Kingdom.
  • Mark W. Pettet
    From the Smith-Kettlewell Eye Research Institute, San Francisco, California; and the
  • Suzanne P. McKee
    From the Smith-Kettlewell Eye Research Institute, San Francisco, California; and the
  • Richard A. Harrad
    Bristol Eye Hospital, Bristol, United Kingdom.
Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science March 2009, Vol.50, 1168-1175. doi:https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.08-2281
  • Views
  • PDF
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Anthony M. Norcia, Julia Hale, Mark W. Pettet, Suzanne P. McKee, Richard A. Harrad; Disparity Tuning of Binocular Facilitation and Suppression after Normal versus Abnormal Visual Development. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2009;50(3):1168-1175. https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.08-2281.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract

purpose. To study the pattern of facilitatory and suppressive binocular interactions in stereodeficient patients with strabismus and in healthy controls.

methods. Visual evoked potentials were recorded in response to a Vernier onset/offset pattern presented to one eye, either monocularly or paired dichoptically with a straight vertical square-wave grating, which, when fused with the target in the other eye, gave rise to a percept of a series of bands appearing in depth from an otherwise uniform plane or with a grating that contained offsets that produced a standing disparity and the appearance of a constantly segmented image, portions of which moved in depth.

results. Participants with normal stereopsis showed facilitative and suppressive binocular interactions that depended on which dichoptic target was presented. Patients with longstanding, constant strabismus lacked normal facilitative binocular interactions. The response to a normally facilitative stimulus was reduced below the monocular level when it was presented to the dominant eye of patients without anisometropia, consistent with classical strabismic suppression of the nondominant eye. The dominant eye of strabismic patients without anisometropia retained suppressive input from crossed but not uncrossed disparity stimuli presented to the nondominant eye.

conclusions. Abnormal disparity processing can be detected with the dichoptic VEP method we describe. Our results suggest that suppression in stereoblind, nonamblyopic observers is determined by a binocular mechanism responsive to disparity. In some cases, the sign of the disparity is important, and this suggests a mechanism that can explain diplopia in patients made exotropic after surgery for esotropia.

In a natural scene, many features in the visual field will lie at different horizontal, and sometimes different vertical, positions in the two retinal half-images. Fusion converts the horizontal differences (disparities) to a stereoscopic depth, assigning each feature a unique three-dimensional position in visual space. Fusional mechanisms also change the visual direction so that the fused percept appears at a horizontal and a vertical location midway between the locations in the half-images. It is easy enough to work out the projected location and depth of a feature from the binocular geometry, but how does the binocular system implement this fusion process neurally? Viewed alone, the monocular location of the target is different from the binocular location. What happens to the neural responses to monocular location when the images are fused? 
Numerous psychophysical studies have demonstrated that information in the monocular half-images is lost during normal fusion. 1 2 3 4 In a study that is particularly relevant to this article, McKee and Harrad 5 measured monocular Vernier acuity using a standard target that consisted of two vertical lines, presented one above the other. Predictably, thresholds for detecting misalignment were in the hyperacuity range (<10 arcsec). However, thresholds rose dramatically when this target was paired stereoscopically with a Vernier target in the other eye containing a large fixed offset. Because of the large offset, the upper line appeared at a different depth from the lower line. Provided that the upper line remained fused binocularly (for offsets covering a range of 6 to 60 arcmin of disparity), thresholds were elevated, compared with the monocular Vernier threshold, by as much as 1 log unit. This “fusional suppression” is not a form of dichoptic masking. If the Vernier target is fused with a straight line in the other eye (0 offset), thresholds are very low because the binocular target is now essentially a stereoacuity target. 
Fusional suppression is probably part of the neural network that determines a unique stereoscopic match. A monocular target will weakly stimulate all disparity-tuned neurons that receive input from the target’s retinal location. When a matching feature is presented to the other eye that strongly excites neurons tuned to a particular disparity, these neurons may inhibit or suppress the weaker responses from neurons tuned to other disparities and locations. An inhibitory network of this type will enhance the correct match by reducing the competitors. 
Previous work on healthy observers 6 has found electrophysiological evidence for these inhibitory interactions. Using a variant of the McKee-Harrad stimulus, we presented an oscillating Vernier target in one eye and paired it stereoscopically with a static fusable target in the other eye (Fig. 1) . The visual evoked potential (VEP) is particularly sensitive to the misalignment of contours or surfaces. 7 8 The static target by itself elicited no VEP, but when fused with the oscillating Vernier target, it modulated the Vernier-driven response. The response was enhanced when paired with straight static lines (Fig. 1 , middle) but was suppressed by the offset static target (Fig. 1 , right). As in the psychophysical studies, binocular fusion of a disparate target suppressed the monocular VEP response, and the inhibitory interaction was dependent on the target disparity. 
Because of the misalignment of their eyes during early development, most observers with strabismus have no functional stereopsis. Nevertheless, they do have binocular interactions, primarily of a suppressive or an inhibitory kind. 9 Harrad and Hess 10 explored whether suppression could be explained as normal dichoptic masking with unequal contrast thresholds. They found that even when the difference in thresholds was taken into account, masking by the dominant (fixing) eye was greater than predicted from normal dichoptic masking functions. Strabismic suppression is not explained by dichoptic contrast masking, so it must involve some other form of binocular inhibition. Given that normal fusion involves suppression, strabismic suppression may be a residual component of the nonfunctional stereo system. 11 12 That strabismic suppression is observed between similar contours only 13 and that cross-oriented gratings presented dichoptically produce as much rivalry in strabismic observers as in nonstrabismic observers supports this idea. 11  
In this study, we used the VEP to explore the electrophysiological characteristics of suppression in strabismic patients with and without anisometropia. We found evidence consistent with strabismic suppression as a component of an aberrant stereo network that, when operative, prevents diplopia in the absence of stereopsis. We also found a special case in which suppression failed in a disparity-dependent fashion. 
Materials and Methods
Observers
Nine healthy adult observers with normal monocular and binocular vision and no previous history of amblyopia, patching, or intermittent strabismus consented to participate. Each observer had a corrected LogMAR visual acuity of 0 (20/20) or better in each eye and normal stereopsis on testing with TNO plates. We defined normal stereopsis as ≤60 arcsec; however, all healthy participants had a stereoacuity of ≤30 arcsec. Twenty-six patients with a history of abnormal visual experience during development because of either strabismus with anisometropia (n = 14) or strabismus without anisometropia (n = 12) participated. The stereoacuity of the patients ranged from 120 arcsec to unmeasurable. Patients were considered to be anisometropic if their spherical equivalent refractions differed by more than 0.75 diopters between the two eyes. Patients were considered to be amblyopic if their interocular acuity differed by 0.2 LogMAR or more. Local ethical committee approval was obtained, and each observer gave fully informed consent. The research complied with the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki. 
Stimulus Generation and Apparatus
Details of the apparatus and basic signal acquisition and processing operations are described in detail in a previous publication 14 and are described here only briefly. The active VEP display, described schematically in Figure 1 , consisted of a circular image of 14° diameter. Computer-generated nonius lines for alignment in both the horizontal and the vertical planes were presented around the aperture, and the stimulus was further surrounded by a fusible pattern of small circles that aided accurate superimposition of the images. The observers were asked to physically align the nonius lines by movement of the mirrors and to regularly check their position between stimulus trials. 
VEP Stimulation Protocol
Six stimulus conditions were presented to each eye. Each condition consisted of the same dynamic “test” stimulus presented to one eye, with one of three static images presented to the other eye. The monocular condition consisted of an oscillating Vernier onset/offset stimulus presented to one eye and a mean luminance central field surrounded by the binocular fixation pattern presented to the second eye. In a second condition type (binocular 0 disparity), the oscillating Vernier stimulus was paired with a static, collinear bar pattern. The Vernier offset created either crossed or uncrossed disparities. The third condition type (binocular 5 arcmin) was the same as the second, except that the static bar pattern also contained Vernier offsets that created a standing crossed or uncrossed-disparity pedestal of 5 arcmin. Crossed disparities will be referred to as positive numbers and uncrossed disparities as negative numbers. When presented alone, the static patterns did not produce a VEP response, but when fused with the temporally modulated pattern, they modified the healthy observer’s perception of the stimulus in terms of its position in the lateral and the depth domains. 
The dynamic “test” pattern consisted of vertical randomly generated black and green bars of spatial frequency 1 ± 0.49 cyc/deg, with 80% contrast. The pattern was divided into 1° horizontal bands. An oscillating Vernier pattern was created by laterally shifting alternating bands back and forth, into and out of alignment with the static bands, at a frequency of 2 Hz (square-wave temporal profile). Over a trial period of 10 seconds, the Vernier offsets increased in size in 10 equal logarithmic steps from 0.5 arcmin to 10 arcmin. 
When the test stimulus was combined with a blank mean luminance half-image, the observers perceived purely lateral displacement (Fig. 1 , bottom). When combined with the straight static pattern (Fig. 1B) , the displacement produced a horizontal disparity so that healthy observers perceived the oscillating bands appearing and disappearing in depth from a collinear background. The moving panels jumped to an apparent position that was either in front of or behind the static bands for crossed and uncrossed disparities, respectively. In the other binocular condition type, the static pattern was also divided into bands matching those in the dynamic pattern. These bands were assigned a constant lateral offset of ±5 arcmin in the direction opposite the offset in the dynamic pattern. Thus, as the Vernier oscillation swept from 0.5 to 10 arcmin, the disparity of the bands swept from 5.5 to 15.0 arcmin or −5.5 to −15.0 arcmin. 
VEP Quantification and Statistical Analysis
The complex numbers representing the amplitudes and phases of the 2-Hz first harmonic of the evoked response were coherently averaged over all trials for each stimulus condition for each observer. Coherent averaging uses amplitude and phase information. Group averages were computed in a similar way. For example, the average sine and cosine coefficients were calculated across observers before magnitude was calculated. For the plots of response magnitude (Fig. 2) , we computed an error measure by pooling the errors on the sine and cosine coefficients in quadrature, as has previously been done for single-observer averages. 15 This measure assumes that the sine and cosine coefficients are uncorrelated. However, correlations occur between these measurements in the case of cross-observer averages because of the presence of individual differences in overall response amplitude. For significance testing, therefore, we used multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA), which takes these correlations into account. MANOVA also correctly models the correlation structure of our repeated-measures design. When we plot data in the complex plane, we show two-dimensional standard errors (Fig. 3) . These error bounds are often elliptical because of correlations between real and imaginary values that arise from individual differences in absolute amplitude. Note that these errors are the same as those that would be computed for a between-subjects design and that they do not reflect the within-observer errors used to assess significance when the MANOVA was used to compare responses across conditions measured within a given group. In all the statistical evaluations, we used the last bin of the sweep to test for effects of stimulus condition because this bin generally had the largest response across conditions and observer group. 
Results
Periodic Vernier onset/offset stimuli produce odd and even harmonic responses that increase monotonically with offset size. 6 7 14 The current analysis focuses on the first harmonic component of the response because previous work has shown this component to be selective for relative position in the case of lateral motion 7 and the disparity of dichoptic targets. 6 The second harmonic responses were also examined in the present study, but they did not show clear disparity effects. 
The data will be presented in two formats. First, VEP amplitude will be plotted as a function of the magnitude of the swept parameter (displacement) for ease of visualization of amplitude effects. Second, because the spectral analysis yields phase and amplitude information, the data will be re-plotted in the complex plane so that both amplitude and phase effects can be visualized. Statistical evaluation will be performed on the complex values. 
Effects of Disparate Pedestals in Healthy Observers
In healthy observers, the addition of a zero-disparity, collinear pedestal leads to a larger first harmonic response than that recorded under monocular viewing conditions (F(2,7) = 5.9; P = 0.031), replicating previous reports with similar stimuli. 6 14 This effect can be seen in Figures 2A and 2Bby comparing the monocular data (gray filled circles; Mon) to the binocular zero-disparity pedestal data (black filled squares; Bin0). In this condition, there is a motion-in-depth percept in addition to the perceived alternation between a collinear set of bars and a segmented set in the monocular condition. 
The disparate-pedestal condition (Figs. 2A 2B , black open squares; Bin5) produces a lower amplitude response than does the monocular condition or the zero-disparity pedestal condition, again replicating previous results. 6 14 Although this condition also leads to a percept of motion in depth, it does not result in a perceived change of segmentation, even though the dynamic half-image alternates between collinear and noncollinear states (Fig. 1 , bottom). The level of standing disparity thus controls the nature of the response to the dynamic test, leading to increases or decreases in response amplitude that are well modeled as gain changes. 14  
Combined Amplitude and Phase Effects
Spectral analysis of the VEP yields amplitude and phase values for each level of the sweep. Figure 3plots the data in the complex plane where distance from the origin corresponds to response amplitude. The time/phase origin for the plots is at the positive x-axis, and increasing phase lag/delay is in the counter-clockwise direction. Successive points on the tuning function are connected with lines. 
As the size of the displacement increases, the response increases in amplitude. In addition, the response phase depends on the size of the displacement and on the type of pedestal in the other eye. As the magnitude of the monocular test offset increases, response phase progresses toward the phase origin of the plot, consistent with a speeding of the response as the stimulus becomes progressively suprathreshold. The addition of the zero-disparity pedestal preserves the same progression of phases seen in the monocular condition but with an increased magnitude response at each corresponding displacement (Fig. 3 ; compare magenta and dark blue circles). In contrast, the disparate-pedestal shifts all response phases toward the origin and reduces the amplitude of the response (Fig. 3 , orange circles). The effect of disparity (0 minute vs. 5 minutes) on the response amplitude is significant for each combination of eye and disparity sign in healthy participants (Table 1for significance values). 
Effects of Disparate Pedestals in Strabismus Patients without Anisometropia
In contrast to healthy observers, patients with constant strabismus and no anisometropia showed substantial differences in the pattern of responses between their dominant and nondominant eyes and differences between each of their eyes and either eye of healthy participants. These data are plotted in Figures 2C and 2Dand in Figures 3eto 3h . These patients did not, in general, have amblyopia (9 of 11). We eliminated from the analysis the single patient in this group who had demonstrable stereopsis for comparability with the group of strabismic-anisometropic patients who had no demonstrable stereopsis. 
Monocular Condition.
Monocular response amplitudes in the nondominant eyes of this group of patients are approximately equal to those of the dominant eye, consistent with the relative lack of amblyopia in this group. 
Zero-Disparity Pedestal Condition.
When the dynamic test was in the dominant eye of patients in this group, there was no increase in amplitude in the zero-disparity pedestal condition compared with the monocular condition, unlike our healthy observers (Figs. 2C 3e 3f) . Response amplitudes and phases were, in fact, similar to those measured in the monocular condition. 
When the test was in the nondominant eye and the zero-disparity pedestal was in the dominant eye, the response was lower than in the monocular condition (Figs. 2D 3g 3h) , though this effect did not reach significance (F(2,9) = 2.24; P = 0.162). However, when we compared the pattern of results in the monocular and binocular zero conditions between patients and healthy participants, there was a significant interaction effect between stimulus type and patient group (F(2,17) = 7.66; P < 0.001). The different relationship between monocular and zero disparity responses in the two groups is consistent with strabismic suppression replacing normal facilitation when the test was in the nondominant eye. 
The reduced rather than enhanced zero-disparity pedestal response was also present in the smaller group of nine patients who did not have amblyopia. The reduction of the response to the test in the nondominant eye when the zero-disparity pedestal is presented to the dominant eye is thus caused by active suppression of the nondominant eye by the dominant eye, even when the nondominant eye has normal acuity. 
Disparate Pedestal Condition.
The disparate pedestal reduces the nondominant eye response relative to the monocular response (Fig. 2D) . This reduction, in itself, was similar to what was seen for healthy observers, but it occurred in the absence of the normal binocular enhancement by the zero-disparity pedestal. In these strabismic observers, there was no significant difference between the zero-disparity and disparate-pedestal conditions as there was in healthy observers (Table 1for significance values). We consider it likely that the reduction produced by the 5 arcmin pedestal was also caused by strabismic suppression because the influence of this stimulus was decreased when the pedestal was in the nondominant eye (e.g., Fig. 2C ). 
When the test was in the dominant eye, the nondominant eye has a weaker effect (Fig. 2C) . When the pedestal carried an uncrossed disparity, there was no difference between the zero-disparity pedestal and the disparate-pedestal conditions (Figs. 2C 2D 3e 3f 3g 3h ; Table 1 ) in 3 of 4 comparisons. The nondominant eye does, however, exert an effect on the dominant eye if the disparate pedestal has a crossed disparity (Figs. 2C[open squares], 3f). The effect is a small but significant reduction in amplitude over a range of suprathreshold disparities (Table 1) . The dominant eye response was thus largely independent of the stimulus presented to the nondominant eye, with the exception of a small, suppressive input from crossed disparities. 
Effects of Disparate Pedestals in Strabismus Patients with Anisometropia
Monocular Condition.
Almost all the strabismus patients who were anisometopic also had amblyopia (12 of 14), and all had defective stereopsis. Consequently, there was a large overall difference in response magnitude between the dominant eyes and the nondominant eyes that did not occur in the other patient group (compare Figs. 2E 2F ). Monocular sensitivity differences at the first harmonic are correlated with perceptual measures of Vernier offset sensitivity and with letter acuity. 16  
Zero-Disparity Condition.
The normal increase in amplitude in the binocular zero condition relative to the monocular response was absent when the test was in either eye (Figs. 2E 2F 3i 3j 3k 3l) , as was seen in the strabismus patients who did not have anisometropia, When the test was in the nondominant eye and the zero-disparity pedestal was in the dominant eye, the response did not differ from that of the monocular condition (F(2,31) = 1.439; P = 0.253). 
Disparate Pedestal Condition.
Overall, there were only small differences among the monocular, binocular zero, and disparate-pedestal conditions in the patients with strabismus and anisometropia (Figs. 2E 2F 3i 3j 3k 3l) . In only one condition (test in the dominant eye, crossed disparities; Fig. 3j ; see Table 1 ) was there a significant difference between the disparate and nondisparate-pedestal conditions. This input from the nondominant eye was also present in the strabismus-only group (Figs. 2C 3e) . In the nondominant eye, the response to the disparate-pedestal condition was similar to that in the monocular condition (unlike the same eye of patients without anisometropia, who showed lower responses). Most of these eyes were amblyopic, and the lack of effect of either pedestal when presented in the dominant eye suggests that neither fusional nor strabismic suppression occurred in this patient group. 
Monocular Response Differences in Dominant Eyes
Although the visual acuity of the dominant eyes of both patient groups was comparable, we noted a previously unreported difference in the overall phase of the dominant eye responses of the strabismus patients with and without anisometropia. This is shown in Figure 4for the monocular responses of each group. The curves of the strabismus-only patients are shifted in the lag direction (counterclockwise) compared with those in anisometropic strabismus patients or healthy participants, and the responses in the strabismus-only group are slightly larger than those of patients with anisometropia. The combination of phase shifts and amplitude changes is significant in a between-subjects analysis (F(2,22) = 4.49; P = 0.023). The 30° phase difference between the two strabismic groups corresponded to approximately 30 to 40 ms. In other words, patients with pure strabismic amblyopia experienced a delay. Thus the dynamics of the monocular response of the dominant eye depended on the developmental history and status of the nondominant eye. The major sensory difference between the strabismic groups was the presence of amblyopia in the nondominant eye. 
Discussion
Our results suggest that suppression in stereoblind, nonamblyopic observers is determined by a binocular mechanism responsive to disparity. As in healthy observers, the effect of the static stimulus on the oscillating Vernier target depends on the disparity specified by the binocular configuration, though the normal facilitation by the zero-disparity pedestal is absent. The differential effect of crossed and uncrossed-disparity pedestals on the dominant eye response of the observers with strabismus also argues for a disparity-tuned mechanism. 
The disparity-dependent effects in the dominant eye of the strabismus patients are relevant to the clinical observation that patients who are esotropic and are made exotropic by surgery report double vision. Because of the esotropia, the patient will not have experienced crossed disparities and, therefore, might never have developed a mechanism to suppress images that carry them. By contrast, it would be necessary to have developed a means of suppressing the nearly constant presence of large uncrossed disparities to prevent diplopia. The haploscope allowed us to present crossed and uncrossed disparities in a controlled fashion and thus to observe a residual “unsuppressable” crossed input from the nondominant eye that affected the responses of the dominant eye. Three previous reports have been published of the nondominant eye having an effect on the dominant eye, 10 17 18 but this is the first study to show the presence of disparity-tuned suppressive interactions. 
In contrast to the preservation of suppressive interactions, we found that patients with longstanding, constant strabismus lack normal facilitative binocular interactions. This interaction is elicited in healthy observers in the zero-disparity pedestal condition, which adds a binocular disparity cue to the monocularly visible cues for discontinuity present in the monocular control condition. The same disparity cue that produces enhancement of the monocular response in healthy participants can result in a lower response in the patients, especially when the static pedestal is presented to the dominant eye of strabismus patients who do not have amblyopia (Fig. 2D) . The trend for the responses of this normally facilitative stimulus to be reduced below the monocular level is consistent with classical strabismic suppression. 
A recent psychophysical study found evidence for intact facilitative binocular interactions in patients with strabismic amblyopia. 19 The authors were able to find these interactions after equating the contrast levels in the two eyes of the patients for distance above detection threshold. When they made this compensation, they found normal levels of binocular summation. Another study found evidence for facilitative binocular interactions in patients with strabismic amblyopia using a dichoptic masking task, 18 though an earlier study did not find such evidence. 10 Although contrast-sensitivity differences might have played a role in the loss of facilitation in our patients with amblyopia, it was unlikely that they were present in the group that did not have amblyopia. These patients also lost the facilitation conferred by the zero-disparity pedestal. Consistent with this, Lema and Blake 20 found no evidence of binocular summation in stereoblind persons with equal contrast sensitivity in both eyes. Our nonamblyopic strabismus patients were probably similar to their stereoblind subjects. The degree to which residual binocular facilitation is present may depend on what function is being tested and the patient’s history of abnormal binocular interaction. 
Comparison with Previous VEP Measures of Binocular Interaction
The paradigm we developed was designed to probe the pattern of disparity-dependent binocular interactions. Unlike other VEP paradigms for binocular interaction, such as summation indices 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 or cyclopean random dot responses, 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 we were able to separately assess the contribution of each eye to the interaction. This was possible because we effectively tagged each eye’s input with a different time course rather than using the same time course in each eye. Because one eye’s image is dynamic and the other eye’s input is static, the time-locked evoked response was labeled for the eye of origin. In this way, we could assay the inputs from one eye without directly recording an evoked response from it. In a traditional binocular summation paradigm, monocular responses are recorded from each eye separately, and the sum of these responses is compared with that measured when both eyes see the same image. This method can detect binocular interaction as an additive failure, but it cannot recover the separate contributions of each eye during binocular stimulation. 
The timing format of our method was similar to that of other temporal tagging methods that examine binocular interaction under dichoptic conditions with the target in one eye static and the other dynamic. 12 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 Other techniques use distinct temporal frequencies in each eye. 12 38 40 With these latter methods, responses of each eye are recorded simultaneously at harmonics of the respective eye-tagging frequencies, and definitive evidence for binocular interaction can be obtained by detecting responses at frequencies equal to sums and differences of the eye-tag frequencies. The main difference between our method and previous dichoptic tagging methods was that we focused on binocular interaction in the network of cells responsible for low-level detection of image discontinuities. Our method is thus more focused on spatial relationships than on the contrast processes that have been the focus of previous studies. Our method also provides a measure of monocular Vernier acuity that can be used to quantify degree of amblyopia. 16  
Timing Abnormalities in the Dominant Eyes
We observed an alteration in the response timing in the dominant eyes of patients that depended on whether their strabismus was accompanied by anisometropia. The dominant eyes of the strabismus patients all had normal acuity; thus, reduced spatial acuity was not a factor. At present we have no explanation for the basis of this effect and simply report its presence. The altered response timing could be the result of several factors: genetic or other constitutional differences between anisometropic and nonanisometropic strabismus patients, differences in the pattern of binocular interaction, and differences in treatment history. Anomalies in response timing may provide additional clues about the pathophysiology of strabismus and amblyopia. 
 
Figure 1.
 
Stimulus schematic. A vernier onset/offset target was presented to one eye while the other eye viewed a blank screen (monocular lateral motion), a collinear bar pattern (disparity range, 0.5′–10′), or an offset bar pattern (disparity range, 5.5′–15′). The test stimulus is in the left eye in this illustration, but the tests were presented in both eyes, and a range of crossed and uncrossed disparities was presented in separate conditions. The lower row illustrates the perceived surfaces and motions. In the monocular lateral motion condition, the dynamic offsets appear to move laterally in the fixation plane over a range of displacements between 0.5′ and 10′. In the disparity range 0.5′ to 10′ condition, lateral motion and motion in depth are both seen, and the stimulus alternates between collinear flat plane and a segmented set of panels, illustrated as two depth planes with arrows between them. In the disparity offset 5.5′ to 15′ condition, the offsets in the right eye create a standing crossed disparity, and the dynamic panels alternate between two planes that lie in front of the fixation plane.
Figure 1.
 
Stimulus schematic. A vernier onset/offset target was presented to one eye while the other eye viewed a blank screen (monocular lateral motion), a collinear bar pattern (disparity range, 0.5′–10′), or an offset bar pattern (disparity range, 5.5′–15′). The test stimulus is in the left eye in this illustration, but the tests were presented in both eyes, and a range of crossed and uncrossed disparities was presented in separate conditions. The lower row illustrates the perceived surfaces and motions. In the monocular lateral motion condition, the dynamic offsets appear to move laterally in the fixation plane over a range of displacements between 0.5′ and 10′. In the disparity range 0.5′ to 10′ condition, lateral motion and motion in depth are both seen, and the stimulus alternates between collinear flat plane and a segmented set of panels, illustrated as two depth planes with arrows between them. In the disparity offset 5.5′ to 15′ condition, the offsets in the right eye create a standing crossed disparity, and the dynamic panels alternate between two planes that lie in front of the fixation plane.
Figure 2.
 
Disparity tuning functions for the first harmonic (1F) components for (A, B) participants with normal vision, (C, D) patients with strabismus without anisometropia, and (E, F) patients with strabismus and anisometropia. Data collected when the test was in the dominant eyes are plotted in the first column (dominant eye). Right: data collected when the test was in the nondominant eye. Gray filled circles: monocular data (Mon). Black filled squares: zero-disparity pedestal data (Bin0). Black open squares: five arcmin-disparity pedestal data (Bin5). (A, B) In the participants with normal vision, the first harmonic response of the 0-disparity pedestal data lies above the monocular data, but the 5 arcmin-disparity pedestal data lies below the monocular data. In each of the patient groups, facilitation in the Bin0 condition was reduced. Error bars were calculated by summing the errors on the sine and cosine coefficients computed across observers in quadrature. 47
Figure 2.
 
Disparity tuning functions for the first harmonic (1F) components for (A, B) participants with normal vision, (C, D) patients with strabismus without anisometropia, and (E, F) patients with strabismus and anisometropia. Data collected when the test was in the dominant eyes are plotted in the first column (dominant eye). Right: data collected when the test was in the nondominant eye. Gray filled circles: monocular data (Mon). Black filled squares: zero-disparity pedestal data (Bin0). Black open squares: five arcmin-disparity pedestal data (Bin5). (A, B) In the participants with normal vision, the first harmonic response of the 0-disparity pedestal data lies above the monocular data, but the 5 arcmin-disparity pedestal data lies below the monocular data. In each of the patient groups, facilitation in the Bin0 condition was reduced. Error bars were calculated by summing the errors on the sine and cosine coefficients computed across observers in quadrature. 47
Figure 3.
 
Two-dimensional plots of the complex values underlying response amplitude and phase. The x-axis plots the real/cosine component, and the y-axis plots the imaginary/sine component in units of microvolts. Left two columns: data from the dominant eyes for uncrossed and crossed disparities. Right two columns: data from the nondominant eyes. Data from each observer group are presented as rows. Within each panel, the monocular data are plotted in blue, the zero-disparity pedestal data are plotted in magenta, and the disparate-pedestal data are plotted in orange. As amplitude increases, the response phase shifts toward the phase origin (in the clockwise direction), especially in the monocular and zero-disparity pedestal conditions. Ellipses: dispersion at a nominal 1 SEM.
Figure 3.
 
Two-dimensional plots of the complex values underlying response amplitude and phase. The x-axis plots the real/cosine component, and the y-axis plots the imaginary/sine component in units of microvolts. Left two columns: data from the dominant eyes for uncrossed and crossed disparities. Right two columns: data from the nondominant eyes. Data from each observer group are presented as rows. Within each panel, the monocular data are plotted in blue, the zero-disparity pedestal data are plotted in magenta, and the disparate-pedestal data are plotted in orange. As amplitude increases, the response phase shifts toward the phase origin (in the clockwise direction), especially in the monocular and zero-disparity pedestal conditions. Ellipses: dispersion at a nominal 1 SEM.
Table 1.
 
Significance Values for Comparisons between Zero Disparity and 5 arcmin Disparity Pedestal Conditions
Table 1.
 
Significance Values for Comparisons between Zero Disparity and 5 arcmin Disparity Pedestal Conditions
Eye Disparity F P
Normal Dominant Crossed (2, 7) = 25.65 <0.001
Dominant Uncrossed (2, 7) = 19.87 0.001
Nondominant Crossed (2, 7) = 17.52 0.002
Nondominant Uncrossed (2, 7) = 5.53 0.036
Strabismus Dominant Crossed (2, 9) = 5.68 0.025
Dominant Uncrossed (2, 9) = 0.15 0.862
Nondominant Crossed (2, 9) = 0.15 0.863
Nondominant Uncrossed (2, 9) = 3.10 0.094
Strabaniso Dominant Crossed (2, 12) = 4.35 0.038
Dominant Uncrossed (2, 12) = 1.68 0.296
Nondominant Crossed (2, 12) = 2.58 0.117
Nondominant Uncrossed (2, 2) = 0.36 0.705
Figure 4.
 
Response amplitude and phase in the dominant eyes of participants with normal vision (black; Normal). Patients with strabismus with (dark gray; Aniso) and without (light gray; Strab) anisometropia. Data are from the monocular viewing condition. Response phase in patients with strabismus without anisometropia is shifted to increased lags/time delays compared with those of observers with normal vision. Response phase in the group with strabismus and anisometropia is shifted toward decreased lag/time delay compared with those of participants with normal vision.
Figure 4.
 
Response amplitude and phase in the dominant eyes of participants with normal vision (black; Normal). Patients with strabismus with (dark gray; Aniso) and without (light gray; Strab) anisometropia. Data are from the monocular viewing condition. Response phase in patients with strabismus without anisometropia is shifted to increased lags/time delays compared with those of observers with normal vision. Response phase in the group with strabismus and anisometropia is shifted toward decreased lag/time delay compared with those of participants with normal vision.
TylerCW. Stereoscopic depth movement: two eyes less sensitive than one. Science. 1971;174:958–961. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
TylerCW, FoleyJM. Stereomovement suppression for transient disparity changes. Perception. 1974;3:287–296. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
McKeeSP, LeviDM, BowneSF. The imprecision of stereopsis. Vision Res. 1990;30:1763–1779. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
HarrisJM, McKeeSP, WatamaniukSNJ. Visual search for motion-in-depth: stereomotion does not “pop out” from disparity noise. Nat Neurosci. 1998;1:165–168. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
McKeeSP, HarradRA. Fusional suppression in normal and stereoanomalous observers. Vision Res. 1993;33:1645–1658. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
NorciaAM, McKeeSP, BonnehY, PettetMW. Suppression of monocular visual direction under fused binocular stimulation: evoked potential measurements. J Vision. 2005;5:34–44. [CrossRef]
NorciaAM, WesemannW, MannyRE. Electrophysiological correlates of Vernier and relative motion mechanisms in human visual cortex. Vis Neurosci. 1999;16:1123–1131. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
LeviDM, MannyRE, KleinSA, SteinmanSB. Electrophysiological correlates of hyperacuity in the human visual cortex. Nature. 1983;306:468–470. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
LeviDM, HarwerthRS, SmithEL, 3rd. Humans deprived of normal binocular vision have binocular interactions tuned to size and orientation. Science. 1979;206:852–854. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
HarradRA, HessRF. Binocular integration of contrast information in amblyopia. Vision Res. 1992;32:2135–2150. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
SchorCM. Visual stimuli for strabismic suppression. Perception. 1977;6:583–593. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
NorciaAM, HarradRA, BrownRJ. Changes in cortical activity during suppression in stereoblindness. Neuroreport. 2000;11:1007–1012. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
JampolskyA. Characteristics of suppression in strabismus. AMA Arch Ophthalmol. 1955;54:683–696. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
HaleJ, HarradRA, McKeeSP, PettetMW, NorciaAM. A VEP measure of the binocular fusion of horizontal and vertical disparities. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2005;46:1786–1790. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
VictorJD, MastJ. A new statistic for steady-state evoked potentials. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1991;78:378–388. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
HouC, GoodWV, NorciaAM. Validation study of VEP Vernier acuity in normal-vision and amblyopic adults. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2007;48:4070–4078. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
HoodAS, MorrisonJD. The dependence of binocular contrast sensitivities on binocular single vision in normal and amblyopic human subjects. J Physiol. 2002;540:607–622. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
BakerDH, MeeseTS, HessRF. Contrast masking in strabismic amblyopia: attenuation, noise, interocular suppression and binocular summation. Vision Res. 2008;48:1625–1640. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
BakerDH, MeeseTS, MansouriB, HessRF. Binocular summation of contrast remains intact in strabismic amblyopia. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2007;48:5332–5338. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
LemaSA, BlakeR. Binocular summation in normal and stereoblind humans. Vision Res. 1977;17:691–695. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
SrebroR. The visually evoked response: binocular facilitation and failure when binocular vision is disturbed. Arch Ophthalmol. 1978;96:839–844. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
ApkarianPA, NakayamaK, TylerCW. Binocularity in the human visual evoked potential: facilitation, summation and suppression. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1981;51:32–48. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
PerryNW, Jr, ChildersDG, McCoyJG. Binocular addition of the visual evoked response at different cortical locations. Vision Res. 1968;8:567–573. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
CiganekL. Binocular addition of the visually evoked response with different stimulus intensities in man. Vision Res. 1970;10:479–487. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
LennerstrandG. Binocular interaction studied with visual evoked responses (VER) in humans with normal or impaired binocular vision. Acta Ophthalmol (Copenh). 1978;56:628–637. [PubMed]
CamposEC, ChiesiC. Binocularity in comitant strabismus, II: objective evaluation with visual evoked responses. Doc Ophthalmol. 1983;55:277–293. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
HarterMR, SeipleWH, SalmonL. Binocular summation of visually evoked responses to pattern stimuli in humans. Vision Res. 1973;13:1433–1446. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
JuleszB, KropflW, PetrigB. Large evoked potentials to dynamic random-dot correlograms and stereograms permit quick determination of stereopsis. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S Am. 1980;77:2348–2351. [CrossRef]
Bodis-WollnerI, BarrisMC, MylinLH, JuleszB, KropflW. Binocular stimulation reveals cortical components of the human visual evoked potential. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1981;52:298–305. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
BraddickO, AtkinsonJ, JuleszB, KropflW, Bodis-WollnerI, RaabE. Cortical binocularity in infants. Nature. 1980;288:363–365. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
PetrigB, JuleszB, KropflW, BaumgartnerG, AnlikerM. Development of stereopsis and cortical binocularity in human infants: electrophysiological evidence. Science. 1981;213:1402–1405. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
NorciaAM, TylerCW. Temporal frequency limits for stereoscopic apparent motion processes. Vision Res. 1984;24:395–401. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
LehmannD, SkrandiesW, LindenmaierC. Binocularly evoked potentials: slow, late components to random-dot stereograms and correlograms (dynamic Julesz patterns). Prog Brain Res. 1980;54:286–290. [PubMed]
EizenmanM, WestallCA, GeerI, et al. Electrophysiological evidence of cortical fusion in children with early-onset esotropia. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1999;40:354–362. [PubMed]
WestallCA, EizenmanM, KraftSP, PantonCM, ChatterjeeS, SigesmundD. Cortical binocularity and monocular optokinetic asymmetry in early-onset esotropia. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1998;39:1352–1360. [PubMed]
SkarfB, EizenmanM, KatzLM, BachynskiB, KleinR. A new VEP system for studying binocular single vision in human infants. J Pediatr Ophthalmol Strabismus. 1993;30:237–242. [PubMed]
LehmannD, JuleszB. Lateralized cortical potentials evoked in humans by dynamic random-dot stereograms. Vision Res. 1978;18:1265–1271. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
BaitchLW, LeviDM. Evidence for nonlinear binocular interactions in human visual cortex. Vision Res. 1988;28:1139–1143. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
BaitchLW, LeviDM. Binocular beats: psychophysical studies of binocular interaction in normal and stereoblind humans. Vision Res. 1989;29:27–35. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
SuterS, SuterPS, PerrierDT, ParkerKL, FoxJA, RoesslerJS. Differentiation of VEP intermodulation and second harmonic components by dichoptic, monocular, and binocular stimulation. Vis Neurosci. 1996;13:1157–1166. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
BrownRJ, CandyTR, NorciaAM. Development of rivalry and dichoptic masking in human infants. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1999;40:3324–3333. [PubMed]
HarterMR, SeipleWH, MussoM. Binocular summation and suppression: visually evoked cortical responses to dichoptically presented patterns of different spatial frequencies. Vision Res. 1974;14:1169–1180. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
HarterMR, SeipleWH, SalmonLE. Evoked cortical responses to dichoptically presented patterned light flashes: interocular interaction. TIT J Life Sci. 1972;2:27–33. [PubMed]
WrightKW, FoxBE, EriksenKJ. PVEP evidence of true suppression in adult onset strabismus. J Pediatr Ophthalmol Strabismus. 1990;27:196–201. [PubMed]
LehmannD, FenderDH. Monocularly evoked electroencephalogram potentials: influence of target structure presented to the other eye. Nature. 1967;215:204–205. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
LehmannD, FenderDH. Component analysis of human averaged evoked potentials: dichoptic stimuli using different target structure. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1968;24:542–553. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
NorciaAM, PeiF, BonnehY, HouC, SampathV, PettetMW. Development of sensitivity to texture and contour information in the human infant. J Cognitive Neurosci. 2005;17:569–579. [CrossRef]
Figure 1.
 
Stimulus schematic. A vernier onset/offset target was presented to one eye while the other eye viewed a blank screen (monocular lateral motion), a collinear bar pattern (disparity range, 0.5′–10′), or an offset bar pattern (disparity range, 5.5′–15′). The test stimulus is in the left eye in this illustration, but the tests were presented in both eyes, and a range of crossed and uncrossed disparities was presented in separate conditions. The lower row illustrates the perceived surfaces and motions. In the monocular lateral motion condition, the dynamic offsets appear to move laterally in the fixation plane over a range of displacements between 0.5′ and 10′. In the disparity range 0.5′ to 10′ condition, lateral motion and motion in depth are both seen, and the stimulus alternates between collinear flat plane and a segmented set of panels, illustrated as two depth planes with arrows between them. In the disparity offset 5.5′ to 15′ condition, the offsets in the right eye create a standing crossed disparity, and the dynamic panels alternate between two planes that lie in front of the fixation plane.
Figure 1.
 
Stimulus schematic. A vernier onset/offset target was presented to one eye while the other eye viewed a blank screen (monocular lateral motion), a collinear bar pattern (disparity range, 0.5′–10′), or an offset bar pattern (disparity range, 5.5′–15′). The test stimulus is in the left eye in this illustration, but the tests were presented in both eyes, and a range of crossed and uncrossed disparities was presented in separate conditions. The lower row illustrates the perceived surfaces and motions. In the monocular lateral motion condition, the dynamic offsets appear to move laterally in the fixation plane over a range of displacements between 0.5′ and 10′. In the disparity range 0.5′ to 10′ condition, lateral motion and motion in depth are both seen, and the stimulus alternates between collinear flat plane and a segmented set of panels, illustrated as two depth planes with arrows between them. In the disparity offset 5.5′ to 15′ condition, the offsets in the right eye create a standing crossed disparity, and the dynamic panels alternate between two planes that lie in front of the fixation plane.
Figure 2.
 
Disparity tuning functions for the first harmonic (1F) components for (A, B) participants with normal vision, (C, D) patients with strabismus without anisometropia, and (E, F) patients with strabismus and anisometropia. Data collected when the test was in the dominant eyes are plotted in the first column (dominant eye). Right: data collected when the test was in the nondominant eye. Gray filled circles: monocular data (Mon). Black filled squares: zero-disparity pedestal data (Bin0). Black open squares: five arcmin-disparity pedestal data (Bin5). (A, B) In the participants with normal vision, the first harmonic response of the 0-disparity pedestal data lies above the monocular data, but the 5 arcmin-disparity pedestal data lies below the monocular data. In each of the patient groups, facilitation in the Bin0 condition was reduced. Error bars were calculated by summing the errors on the sine and cosine coefficients computed across observers in quadrature. 47
Figure 2.
 
Disparity tuning functions for the first harmonic (1F) components for (A, B) participants with normal vision, (C, D) patients with strabismus without anisometropia, and (E, F) patients with strabismus and anisometropia. Data collected when the test was in the dominant eyes are plotted in the first column (dominant eye). Right: data collected when the test was in the nondominant eye. Gray filled circles: monocular data (Mon). Black filled squares: zero-disparity pedestal data (Bin0). Black open squares: five arcmin-disparity pedestal data (Bin5). (A, B) In the participants with normal vision, the first harmonic response of the 0-disparity pedestal data lies above the monocular data, but the 5 arcmin-disparity pedestal data lies below the monocular data. In each of the patient groups, facilitation in the Bin0 condition was reduced. Error bars were calculated by summing the errors on the sine and cosine coefficients computed across observers in quadrature. 47
Figure 3.
 
Two-dimensional plots of the complex values underlying response amplitude and phase. The x-axis plots the real/cosine component, and the y-axis plots the imaginary/sine component in units of microvolts. Left two columns: data from the dominant eyes for uncrossed and crossed disparities. Right two columns: data from the nondominant eyes. Data from each observer group are presented as rows. Within each panel, the monocular data are plotted in blue, the zero-disparity pedestal data are plotted in magenta, and the disparate-pedestal data are plotted in orange. As amplitude increases, the response phase shifts toward the phase origin (in the clockwise direction), especially in the monocular and zero-disparity pedestal conditions. Ellipses: dispersion at a nominal 1 SEM.
Figure 3.
 
Two-dimensional plots of the complex values underlying response amplitude and phase. The x-axis plots the real/cosine component, and the y-axis plots the imaginary/sine component in units of microvolts. Left two columns: data from the dominant eyes for uncrossed and crossed disparities. Right two columns: data from the nondominant eyes. Data from each observer group are presented as rows. Within each panel, the monocular data are plotted in blue, the zero-disparity pedestal data are plotted in magenta, and the disparate-pedestal data are plotted in orange. As amplitude increases, the response phase shifts toward the phase origin (in the clockwise direction), especially in the monocular and zero-disparity pedestal conditions. Ellipses: dispersion at a nominal 1 SEM.
Figure 4.
 
Response amplitude and phase in the dominant eyes of participants with normal vision (black; Normal). Patients with strabismus with (dark gray; Aniso) and without (light gray; Strab) anisometropia. Data are from the monocular viewing condition. Response phase in patients with strabismus without anisometropia is shifted to increased lags/time delays compared with those of observers with normal vision. Response phase in the group with strabismus and anisometropia is shifted toward decreased lag/time delay compared with those of participants with normal vision.
Figure 4.
 
Response amplitude and phase in the dominant eyes of participants with normal vision (black; Normal). Patients with strabismus with (dark gray; Aniso) and without (light gray; Strab) anisometropia. Data are from the monocular viewing condition. Response phase in patients with strabismus without anisometropia is shifted to increased lags/time delays compared with those of observers with normal vision. Response phase in the group with strabismus and anisometropia is shifted toward decreased lag/time delay compared with those of participants with normal vision.
Table 1.
 
Significance Values for Comparisons between Zero Disparity and 5 arcmin Disparity Pedestal Conditions
Table 1.
 
Significance Values for Comparisons between Zero Disparity and 5 arcmin Disparity Pedestal Conditions
Eye Disparity F P
Normal Dominant Crossed (2, 7) = 25.65 <0.001
Dominant Uncrossed (2, 7) = 19.87 0.001
Nondominant Crossed (2, 7) = 17.52 0.002
Nondominant Uncrossed (2, 7) = 5.53 0.036
Strabismus Dominant Crossed (2, 9) = 5.68 0.025
Dominant Uncrossed (2, 9) = 0.15 0.862
Nondominant Crossed (2, 9) = 0.15 0.863
Nondominant Uncrossed (2, 9) = 3.10 0.094
Strabaniso Dominant Crossed (2, 12) = 4.35 0.038
Dominant Uncrossed (2, 12) = 1.68 0.296
Nondominant Crossed (2, 12) = 2.58 0.117
Nondominant Uncrossed (2, 2) = 0.36 0.705
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×