Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science Cover Image for Volume 50, Issue 3
March 2009
Volume 50, Issue 3
Free
Eye Movements, Strabismus, Amblyopia and Neuro-ophthalmology  |   March 2009
Sex-Specific Lateralization of Contraction Anisocoria in Transient Pupillary Light Reflex
Author Affiliations
  • Xiaofei Fan
    From the Departments of Biological Engineering and
  • Judith H. Miles
    Child Health and the
    Thompson Center for Autism and Neurodevelopmental Disorders, University of Missouri-Columbia, Columbia, Missouri.
  • Nicole Takahashi
    Child Health and the
    Thompson Center for Autism and Neurodevelopmental Disorders, University of Missouri-Columbia, Columbia, Missouri.
  • Gang Yao
    From the Departments of Biological Engineering and
Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science March 2009, Vol.50, 1137-1144. doi:https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.08-2329
  • Views
  • PDF
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Xiaofei Fan, Judith H. Miles, Nicole Takahashi, Gang Yao; Sex-Specific Lateralization of Contraction Anisocoria in Transient Pupillary Light Reflex. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2009;50(3):1137-1144. https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.08-2329.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract

purpose. Contraction anisocoria describes a phenomenon in which the pupil of a directly illuminated eye constricts more than the pupil of the consensual (not illuminated) eye. The purpose of this study was to investigate the lateralization of contraction anisocoria in young female and male subjects.

methods. Infrared binocular pupillography was used to measure pupillary light reflex (PLR) in 44 healthy children (23 girls, 21 boys) from 6 to 16 years of age. Measurements were conducted in both light-adapted and dark-adapted conditions with different stimulus intensities. Relative constriction amplitude was obtained by dividing the maximal pupil area change by the initial static pupil area. Contraction anisocoria was calculated by subtracting relative constriction amplitude in the consensual eye from that of the direct eye. Values of contraction anisocoria obtained by stimulating a subject’s right or left eye were compared to determine any potential lateralization.

results. It was found that stimulating the right eye led to larger contraction anisocoria than stimulating the left eye. Such right-side lateralization of contraction anisocoria is much greater in males than in females. In addition, the effects of sex were related to the ambient light level and stimulus intensity.

conclusions. These results provide evidence that contraction anisocoria is more laterally asymmetric in males than in females.

Sex-specific dimorphism is observed in many brain structures, such as the corpus callosum, splenium, and neocortex, as revealed in positron emission tomography (PET) and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies. A recent study by Amunts et al. 1 shows the same sexually dimorphic pattern exists in human visual cortex. They reported that the male visual cortices are more asymmetrical than female and that they are lateralized toward the right hemisphere. Similarly, hemispheric lateralization and sex effects have been observed in autonomic cardiovascular control. 2 3 Such sex-specific trends in lateralization may be a common feature for both cortical and subcortical physiological functions. 
In this study, we investigated sex-specific lateralization of the pupillary light reflex (PLR), the phenomenon by which pupil size changes with luminance. Pupil size is controlled by two antagonistic muscles, the sphincter and the dilator, which are innervated by the parasympathetic and sympathetic nervous systems. 4 In the classic view, the constriction process involved in PLR is mediated by the parasympathetic pathway, involving the Edinger-Westphal (EW) nucleus, the ciliary ganglion, and the short ciliary nerves. The pupilloconstrictor neurons in the EW nucleus act as a signal generator whose output is modulated by a number of inputs; the major one is the afferent projection from the retina through the pretectal olivary nucleus (PON) in midbrain. As an output of multiple neuronal systems, PLR is a precise measure of neural network functions, and it has been applied in many neurology and psychophysiology studies. 5 6 7 8 9 10  
Here, we focus our study on the phenomenon of contraction anisocoria in healthy male and female subjects. Contraction anisocoria refers to the difference between the pupillary constrictions in the stimulated eye (direct) and the contralateral eye (consensual). It was initially considered as an abnormality in the reflex pathway, 11 12 but subsequent studies have proved that a small yet statistically significant difference in constriction amplitude between the direct and consensual eyes exists in healthy subjects. 13 14 15 16 17 The small contraction anisocoria in healthy subjects using full-field stimulation has been hypothetically attributed to the asymmetries at the two decussations, 13 14 15 16 17 as illustrated in Figure 1 . The first occurs at the optical chiasm, where the ratio of crossed and uncrossed optic nerve fibers is estimated to be approximately 53:47, as reported in a histologic study in humans. 18 The second decussation takes place in the midbrain, where each PON projects predominantly to the contralateral side. 19 20 21 Overall, the EW nucleus at the same side of the stimulated (direct) eye receives a stronger signal, thus inducing a greater constriction. However, the specific distribution of the pupil-serving retinal ganglion cells and the associated decussations at the chiasm and further in the midbrain are still not thoroughly known. 16  
To investigate any potential lateralization in contraction anisocoria, we compared contraction anisocoria obtained by stimulating the left and right eyes separately. The effects of sex and the side of the stimulated eye were analyzed to determine whether contraction anisocoria has a sex-specific pattern in the lateralization of the response. 
Methods
Subjects
Forty-four healthy children (23 girls, 21 boys) participated in this study. The subjects ranged from 6 to 16 years of age (10.4 ± 2.7 [mean ± SD]). The detailed age distribution is shown in Figure 2 . All subjects had normal vision, including visual acuity or correctable visual acuity at 20/20 or better. All were in good physical health and lacked any history of eye disorders. Most of the participants (19 girls, 18 boys) were tested between 2:00 pm and 6:30 pm, and the other seven (3 boys, 4 girls) were tested between 10:00 am and 12:00 pm. 
All participants were recruited from Columbia, Missouri, or its surrounding areas. The study adhered to the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki. The experimental protocol was approved by the university institutional review board. All participants and their parents signed informed consent forms before the tests. 
PLR Apparatus
As shown in Figure 3 , the pupillogram recording system consists of two independent pupil-imaging channels for each eye. Each channel has an illumination port and an imaging port. The two ports are combined with a cold mirror that reflects visible light and transmits infrared light. 
A green LED of 530 nm (LXK2-PD12-R00; Lumileds Lighting, San Jose, CA) was used in each channel as the stimulation light source. The LEDs illuminated an optical diffuser to provide a uniform on-axis illumination with a visual field of 5.7°. A wide range of stimulation light intensity from 107.4 to 1013.6 photons/cm2/s was achieved by controlling the LED current and using neutral density filters. Two different intensities of 109.3 and 1012.5 photons/cm2/s were actually used in this study. All intensities were calibrated with a photograph diode (818-ST-UV/CM; Newport Corp., Irvine, CA) coupled with a power meter (2835-C; Newport Corp.) located at the same location as the subject’s eye. The pulse width (100 ms) of the stimulation was programmed through a custom computer interface. 
An infrared LED of 880 nm wavelength (604-WP34SF4C; Mouser Electronics, Mansfield, TX) was installed in each imaging channel to illuminate the subject’s pupil. Video cameras (PC164CEX-2; Supercircuits Inc., Austin, TX) with high sensitivity in the infrared range were used. The magnification of the imaging system was calibrated by imaging a standard calibrated ruler. Pupil images were captured by a video card at 30 frames/s. The two cameras (one in each channel) were synchronized by use of a common trigger signal. 
Procedure
Measurements were conducted in light-adapted and dark-adapted conditions. Subjects were first seated in the testing room for at least 5 minutes. The room had an illumination level of 30 cd/m2. When instructed, each subject placed his or her head into a chin-and-forehead rest located on the height-adjustable platform. Before each measurement, the subjects had 10 to approximately 15 seconds to look into the imaging channels for adaptation. To minimize eye movement during tests, subjects were asked to look at pictures (cartoon images) shown on a computer screen 4 feet away through the imaging channels in our PLR device (Fig. 3) . The pictures on the screen were switched every 30 seconds. The color of the screen was adjusted to pure red by disabling the green and blue channels, and its brightness and contrast were reduced (0.01∼0.02 cd/m2) to avoid affecting pupil size. After finishing all the light-adapted tests, all lights in the testing room were turned off. Subjects stayed in the dark room for 20 minutes for dark adaptation before the tests were repeated. 
Accommodation effects were not monitored in this study. Subjects were instructed to stare at the monitor during the tests. Because of the short light flash (100 ms) applied, the chance of the fixation on the flash may not be significant. Indeed, the pupillary constriction amplitude was strongly dependent on stimulus intensities and adaptation in our tests, suggesting our measured constrictions were induced by the stimulation light. 
In the light-adapted tests, a stimulus intensity of 1012.5 photons/cm2/s was applied. In the dark-adapted tests, two stimulus intensities of 109.3 and 1012.5 photons/cm2/s were applied. Only one eye was stimulated in each test. Under each stimulus condition, the left eye was stimulated first, and then the right eye was stimulated. PLRs of the direct and consensual eyes were acquired at the same time. Image recording started 1 second before the stimulus and lasted for 10 seconds. Four measurements were conducted at each testing condition, and the interval between two consecutive measurements was 1.5 minutes. Between the consecutive measurements, the subject was free to move his or her head from the testing device. 
PLR Parameters
Pupil size was automatically computed with the use of custom software from saved image stacks. Briefly, the pixels located at the pupil boundary in each image were first determined using a histogram-based segmentation method. A fast elliptic-fitting algorithm was then applied to fit the entire pupil boundary, and the pupil area was calculated as the area of the fitted ellipse. Average pupil diameter could be computed if the pupil area was approximated as a circle. 
The initial static pupil size was obtained by averaging the pupil sizes calculated from the 30 images acquired before the stimulus onset. Anisocoria was calculated by subtracting the left pupil diameter from the right pupil diameter. Relative constriction amplitude was obtained by dividing the maximal pupil area change (i.e., the difference between the initial static pupil area and the minimum pupil area after stimulus) by the initial static pupil area. Although some previous studies 13 16 used the raw pupil size to calculate contraction anisocoria, our data suggested that using the relative percentage constriction can minimize any variations caused by the difference in the initial pupil size. Contraction anisocoria was then derived by subtracting relative constriction amplitude in the consensual eye from that of the direct eye. Therefore, negative contraction anisocoria indicated the relative constriction amplitude was larger in the consensual eye than in the direct eye. 
Statistical Analysis
Distributions of relative constriction amplitude, anisocoria, and contraction anisocoria obtained in all participants were verified to conform to normal distributions using the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test. A mixed-factor analysis of variance (ANOVA) with repeated measures was then applied to study sex as the between-subjects factor. In addition, ambient illumination level was used as a within-subjects factor for anisocoria, whereas the eye stimulated and the stimulus condition (including ambient light level and stimulus intensity) were used as within-subjects factors to study contraction anisocoria. To analyze relative constriction amplitude, the eye measured (direct or consensual eye) was combined with the factor of eye stimulated and was included as a within-subject factor in the ANOVA model, and the interactions were examined for all major factors. ANOVA was carried out with mixed procedure (SAS version 9.1.3; SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC). Follow-up post hoc t-tests were then carried out in all the interested pairwise comparisons. Statistics from multiple comparison procedures were Bonferroni corrected by multiplying the obtained P values by the number of pairwise comparisons. P < 0.05 was considered significant. 
Results
Anisocoria
Table 1summarizes the average pupil diameter of the two eyes and the calculated anisocoria values. All results are shown in the form of mean ± SE. To show the data distribution, all measurements including outliers are shown in a box plot in Figure 4A . Because measurements obtained when stimulating the left or the right eye can be used to calculate initial pupil size, the total number of measurements used in each subject to calculate anisocoria were 8 and 16 at light-adapted and dark-adapted conditions, respectively. Of all measurements in the 44 subjects, only three single measurements, each from a different subject, were excluded from the calculation because they fell more than three times the interquartile range above the third quartile or below the first quartile. 22  
Mean anisocoria was not significantly greater than zero in dark-adapted or light-adapted conditions. ANOVA results indicated no significant effects from sex, ambient light level, or their interaction (P = 0.0999, P = 0.6788, and P = 0.2936, respectively). Figure 4Bshows the distribution of anisocoria measured in light-adapted and dark-adapted conditions. The interquartile ranges (25th–75th percentiles) of the standard deviation for individual subject (within-subject variance) were 0.23 mm to 0.38 mm. It can be seen that the measurements obtained at the two different adaptation conditions were highly correlated (r = 0.88). 
Constriction Amplitude
Relative constriction amplitude depends on stimulus intensity and adaptation. As expected, relative constriction amplitude was larger in the dark-adapted conditions than in the light-adapted conditions, and constriction increased with stimulus intensity. Box plots of all measured constriction amplitudes are shown in Figure 5 . Interquartile ranges (25th–75th percentiles) of standard deviations for individual subjects were 2.71% to 4.85%. Table 2summarizes the values (in the form of mean ± SE) of direct and consensual relative constriction amplitude measured in female and male subjects. The main effect of stimulus condition was highly significant (P < 0.0001). 
The “effect of eye” (a combination of the eye stimulated and the eye measured) was significant (P < 0.0001). Multiple pairwise comparisons were performed to investigate the difference in constriction amplitude observed by stimulating different eyes. Constriction amplitudes in the direct eye (stimulated) were significantly larger in the right eyes than in the left eyes (P < 0.0011). Conversely, constriction amplitudes showed no statistical difference in the consensual eyes (P = 0.9578). These two P values were Bonferroni corrected by multiplying the raw P values by 2. 
We also observed a significant interaction of sex by stimulus condition (P < 0.0001) in ANOVA. However, post hoc pairwise t-tests indicated no significant sex difference in any of the three stimulus conditions (P = 1.0000, P = 1.0000, and P = 0.1601 for light-adapted 1012.5, dark-adapted 109.3, and 1012.5 photons/cm2/s, respectively). All P values were Bonferroni corrected by multiplying by 3. 
As discussed, relative constriction amplitude was used in our analysis to reduce any variations caused by potential differences in initial pupil size. As a quantitative comparison, the obtained constrictions were 0.86 ± 0.38 mm, 0.76 ± 0.31 mm, and 2.12 ± 0.31 mm (mean ± SD) at light-adapted 1012.5, dark-adapted 109.3, and 1012.5 photons/cm2/s, respectively. Relative variations (the ratio of the SD and the mean value) were 41.6%, 40.5%, and 14.6% at these three conditions. If the relative constriction amplitude was used, the calculated relative variations were reduced to 33.1%, 31.4%, and 11.3%, respectively. Similar results were obtained for pupil area calculations. Nevertheless, the same statistical conclusion was reached when using the absolute constriction amplitude in our ANOVA. 
Contraction Anisocoria
Contraction anisocoria measured in boys and girls by stimulating left and right eyes at three stimulus conditions are summarized in Table 3 . Values of contraction anisocoria are shown in the form of mean ± SE. P values indicate how significantly the measurements are different from zero. Four measurements of each subject were used to calculate the contraction anisocoria for each stimulated eye at each of the three test conditions. In all measurements of the 44 subjects, three single measurements from two subjects were excluded because the measurements were extreme outliers. 22  
We found the mean of contraction anisocoria was not significantly greater than zero in all subjects when the left eye was stimulated at light-adapted 1012.5 photons/cm2/s (P = 0.0606) and in male subjects at dark-adapted 109.3 photons/cm2/s (P = 0.4105). In all other conditions, contraction anisocoria was significant, and constrictions in the direct eye were approximately 0.4% to 2.8% larger than in the consensual eye. 
Each subject could be classified into one of three categories based on contraction anisocoria. 13 According to the criterion described by Smith et al., 13 contraction anisocoria exists when the difference of pupil diameter change in the two eyes is greater than twice the spatial resolution, which was 0.06 mm in our imaging system. A subject has bilateral contraction anisocoria if contraction anisocoria always exists regardless of which eye is stimulated. Unilateral contraction anisocoria means contraction anisocoria existed only when one eye was stimulated. If the contraction anisocoria obtained by stimulating neither side was greater than the threshold, the subject was classified as having no contraction anisocoria. Results are shown in Table 4 . Overall, 61.4%, 75.0%, and 79.5% subjects showed contraction anisocoria at the three stimulus conditions of light-adapted 1012.5 photons/cm2/s, dark-adapted 109.3 photons/cm2/s, and 1012.5 photons/cm2/s, respectively. 
Sex Difference in Contraction Anisocoria
Figure 6shows the distributions of contraction anisocoria in each subject obtained by stimulating different eyes at three stimulus conditions. The interquartile range (25th-75th percentile) of the measurement SD for individual subjects was 1.04% to 2.36%. In girls, the distribution of contraction anisocoria was similar under dark-adapted conditions regardless of which eye was stimulated, which is also shown by the population distribution in Table 4 . In contrast, asymmetry of contraction anisocoria was readily observed in boys, especially at dark-adapted 109.3 photons/cm2/s. Overall, contraction anisocoria in boys was larger than in girls. F statistics in ANOVA indicated that the effect of sex (P = 0.0301), the interaction of sex by stimulated eye (P < 0.0001), and the interaction of sex by stimulated eye by stimulus condition (P = 0.0088) were all significant. 
To further illustrate the sex-related differences, the averaged values (Table 3)of contraction anisocoria in boys and girls are shown in Figure 7 . There was no significant difference between eyes in contraction anisocoria in girls. In boys, stimulating the right eye produced a significantly larger contraction anisocoria than stimulating the left eye under a dark-adapted stimulus of 109.3 photons/cm2/s (right vs. left, 2.90% vs. −0.26%; P < 0.0001). Such an asymmetry pattern in boys also appeared at the other two conditions—light-adapted 1012.5 photons/cm2/s (right vs. left, 1.40% vs. 0.42%) and dark-adapted 1012.5 photons/cm2/s (right vs. left, 2.12% vs. 1.21%), though the difference was not statistically significant (Fig. 7) . Furthermore, under dark-adapted conditions, contraction anisocoria in boys was significantly larger than in girls when the right eyes were stimulated (at 109.3 photons/cm2/s: 2.90% [boys] vs. 0.90% [girls], P = 0.0002; at 1012.5 photons/cm 2 /s: 2.12% [boys] vs. 0.73% [girls], P = 0.0232). All these P values were Bonferroni corrected by multiplying the raw P values by 12. Such a right-over-left preponderance in boys can also be seen in Table 4
Discussion
Several studies have revealed that contraction anisocoria can be detected in healthy subjects. Smith et al. 13 were the first to report a small but significant contraction anisocoria in healthy subjects (∼6.1% of constriction amplitude). In their study of 72 subjects, 84.7% showed contraction anisocoria. They also found that contraction anisocoria was not influenced by stimulus intensity when expressed as a percentage of constriction amplitude. 
Our stimulus configuration provided illumination over the macular region only. By stimulating the right and left eyes separately, we made several interesting observations. First, contraction anisocoria seemed to be lateralized to the right eye because its mean value was always significantly larger than zero when the right eye was stimulated, but this was not true when the left eye was stimulated (Table 3 , summary). Under most stimulus conditions, stimulating the right eye led to larger contraction anisocoria than did stimulating the left eye (Tables 3 4) . In addition, we found that such right-side lateralization of contraction anisocoria was much greater in male subjects than in female subjects. 
Right lateralization of constriction amplitude in the direct eye (stimulated) was also observed by Bär et al. 23 The same results were obtained in the present study (Table 2) . In addition, our results indicated that such a difference was not present in the consensual eye, which was not measured in the study of Bär et al. 23 Because pupillary constriction is primarily controlled by the parasympathetic component of the autonomic nervous system, these results imply a lateralization of autonomic functions. In fact, several studies 2 23 have confirmed the hemispheric lateralization of autonomic controls in cardiovascular functions. 
Our results cannot be explained by the existing knowledge on contraction anisocoria because neither sex- nor stimulus intensity-related lateralization has been described at the two decussations at the optical chiasm and the PON. However, the potential involvement of cortical level pathways in PLR 24 25 is worth some discussion. Barbur 4 suggested that there might be a projection from the primary visual cortex (V1) to the PON and a projection from extrastriate visual cortical areas to the EW nucleus. A recent anatomic study 1 has shown that the male visual cortex is rightward biased, whereas the female visual cortex is more symmetric. Such a difference in the visual cortex may be related to the asymmetric contraction anisocoria observed in male subjects. In addition, the topographic segregation of the signal input from rods and cones at human visual cortex caused by retinotopic mapping has been reported. 26 It is known that rods are active at dark-adapted conditions but are suppressed by high-intensity stimuli. On the other hand, cones become active under light-adapted conditions or when stimulated by high-intensity light. Coincidentally, our observed sex effect seems related to the ambient light level and stimulus intensity. At dark-adapted 109.3 photons/cm2/s, the sexually dimorphic pattern of contraction anisocoria is significant. However, this trend is largely reduced at dark-adapted 1012.5 photons/cm2/s and light-adapted 1012.5 photons/cm2/s. If the cortical areas receiving signals from rods and cones make different contributions to the PLR pathway, it may explain the observed illuminance-dependent difference in contraction anisocoria. 
We would like to point out that the dominant eye effect was not examined in our subjects. It is not likely that our testing results were biased by such effects. Several comprehensive studies have shown that the distribution of the dominant eye has no correlation with sex or age 27 28 and that the dominant eye effect may have no specific function in vision. 29 In addition, we did not monitor subjects’ fixation in this study though subjects were instructed to stare at the screen during the tests. However, as described in section 2.3, the influence of accommodation-induced near pupil response was unlikely to be significant in our results. 
In summary, our study revealed sex-specific lateralization patterns in contraction anisocoria. However, further functional and anatomic studies are needed for full understanding of the biological basis of this phenomenon. 
 
Figure 1.
 
An illustration of the parasympathetic pupillary pathways. Only the afferent signals from the right eye are illustrated. Arrows: direction of signal transduction. Thick line: stronger signal (not in proportion). CG, ciliary ganglions; EW, Edinger-Westphal nucleus; OC, optic chiasm; PON, pretectal olivary nucleus.
Figure 1.
 
An illustration of the parasympathetic pupillary pathways. Only the afferent signals from the right eye are illustrated. Arrows: direction of signal transduction. Thick line: stronger signal (not in proportion). CG, ciliary ganglions; EW, Edinger-Westphal nucleus; OC, optic chiasm; PON, pretectal olivary nucleus.
Figure 2.
 
Age distribution of all participants. Histograms are grouped by sex.
Figure 2.
 
Age distribution of all participants. Histograms are grouped by sex.
Figure 3.
 
Schematic diagram of the experimental setup for PLR recording. LED, stimulus light sources; IR LED, illumination light sources; BS, beam splitter; NDF, neutral density filter; HM, hot mirror; D, diffuser; CCD, near-infrared charge-coupled device camera.
Figure 3.
 
Schematic diagram of the experimental setup for PLR recording. LED, stimulus light sources; IR LED, illumination light sources; BS, beam splitter; NDF, neutral density filter; HM, hot mirror; D, diffuser; CCD, near-infrared charge-coupled device camera.
Table 1.
 
Average Pupil Size, and Anisocoria of Subjects Measured in Light- and Dark-adapted Conditions
Table 1.
 
Average Pupil Size, and Anisocoria of Subjects Measured in Light- and Dark-adapted Conditions
Sex Condition Pupil Diameter (mm) Anisocoria (mm)
Left Right
Female Light-adapted 6.25 ± 0.11 6.22 ± 0.11 −0.04 ± 0.05
Dark-adapted 6.85 ± 0.11 6.80 ± 0.11 −0.05 ± 0.05
Male Light-adapted 6.27 ± 0.12 6.32 ± 0.12 0.05 ± 0.06
Dark-adapted 6.74 ± 0.11 6.75 ± 0.11 0.001 ± 0.06
Figure 4.
 
Pupil size and anisocoria. (A) Box plot of pupil size in terms of side of eye, sex, and ambient light level. Boxes indicate the 25th and 75th percentiles, and whiskers show 1.5 times the interquartile range. The bar at the center of each box indicates the median value. (×) The maximal and the minimal data points. (B) The scatter plot of anisocoria measured under dark adaptation and light adaptation. Data points shown are mean values measured in each subject.
Figure 4.
 
Pupil size and anisocoria. (A) Box plot of pupil size in terms of side of eye, sex, and ambient light level. Boxes indicate the 25th and 75th percentiles, and whiskers show 1.5 times the interquartile range. The bar at the center of each box indicates the median value. (×) The maximal and the minimal data points. (B) The scatter plot of anisocoria measured under dark adaptation and light adaptation. Data points shown are mean values measured in each subject.
Figure 5.
 
Box plots of relative constriction amplitude of both sexes at three stimulus conditions. (A) Light-adapted 1012.5 photons/cm2/s, (B) dark-adapted 109.3 photons/cm2/s, and (C) dark-adapted 1012.5 photons/cm2/s.
Figure 5.
 
Box plots of relative constriction amplitude of both sexes at three stimulus conditions. (A) Light-adapted 1012.5 photons/cm2/s, (B) dark-adapted 109.3 photons/cm2/s, and (C) dark-adapted 1012.5 photons/cm2/s.
Table 2.
 
Relative Constriction Amplitudes of Subjects Measured by Stimulating Different Eyes at Three Stimulus Conditions
Table 2.
 
Relative Constriction Amplitudes of Subjects Measured by Stimulating Different Eyes at Three Stimulus Conditions
Sex Direct Eye Light Adapted, % Dark Adapted, %
1012.5 photons/cm2/s 109.3 photons/cm2/s 1012.5 photons/cm2/s
Direct Consensual Direct Consensual Direct Consensual
Female Left 26.00 ± 1.38 25.34 ± 1.38 20.81 ± 1.37 20.05 ± 1.37 50.98 ± 1.37 50.33 ± 1.38
Right 26.77 ± 1.37 25.51 ± 1.37 22.32 ± 1.38 21.25 ± 1.38 51.94 ± 1.38 51.15 ± 1.37
Male Left 24.82 ± 1.44 24.39 ± 1.44 20.22 ± 1.44 19.67 ± 1.44 55.04 ± 1.44 53.89 ± 1.44
Right 25.14 ± 1.43 23.75 ± 1.43 22.69 ± 1.44 19.90 ± 1.44 55.93 ± 1.44 54.03 ± 1.44
Table 3.
 
Contraction Anisocoria Measured in Subjects at Three Stimulus Conditions
Table 3.
 
Contraction Anisocoria Measured in Subjects at Three Stimulus Conditions
Sex Direct Eye Light Adapted Dark Adapted
1012.5 photons/cm2/s 109.3 photons/cm2/s 1012.5 photons/cm2/s
Mean ± SE P Mean ± SE P Mean ± SE P
Female Left 0.56 ± 0.32 0.0816 0.84 ± 0.30 0.0062* 0.96 ± 0.30 0.0018*
Right 1.19 ± 0.30 0.0001* 0.90 ± 0.31 0.0045* 0.73 ± 0.30 0.0145*
Male Left 0.42 ± 0.31 0.1836 0.26 ± 0.32 0.4105 1.21 ± 0.32 0.0002*
Right 1.40 ± 0.31 <0.0001* 2.90 ± 0.32 <0.0001* 2.12 ± 0.32 <0.0001*
Table 4.
 
Number and Percentage of Subjects Classified into Each of Three Categories of Contraction Anisocoria at Different Stimulus Conditions
Table 4.
 
Number and Percentage of Subjects Classified into Each of Three Categories of Contraction Anisocoria at Different Stimulus Conditions
Sex Category Light Adapted Dark Adapted
1012.5 photons/cm2/s 109.3 photons/cm2/s 1012.5 photons/cm2/s
No. % No. % No. %
Female None 10 43.5 7 30.4 7 30.4
Bilateral 3 13.0 2 8.7 2 8.7
Unilateral (left) 3 13.0 6 26.1 7 30.4
Unilateral (right) 7 30.4 8 34.8 7 30.4
Male None 7 33.3 4 19.0 2 9.5
Bilateral 2 9.5 2 9.5 4 19.0
Unilateral (left) 3 14.3 1 4.8 5 23.8
Unilateral (right) 9 42.9 14 66.7 10 47.6
Figure 6.
 
Scatter plots of contraction anisocoria of both sexes as found by stimulating different eyes at three stimulus conditions: (A) light-adapted 1012.5 photons/cm2/s, (B) dark-adapted 109.3 photons/cm2/s, and (C) dark-adapted 1012.5 photons/cm2/s. Data points shown are mean values measured in each subject.
Figure 6.
 
Scatter plots of contraction anisocoria of both sexes as found by stimulating different eyes at three stimulus conditions: (A) light-adapted 1012.5 photons/cm2/s, (B) dark-adapted 109.3 photons/cm2/s, and (C) dark-adapted 1012.5 photons/cm2/s. Data points shown are mean values measured in each subject.
Figure 7.
 
Contraction anisocoria obtained by stimulating different eyes at three stimulus conditions in (A) female subjects and (B) male subjects. The error bars indicate the SE. LA, light adapted, DA, dark adapted.
Figure 7.
 
Contraction anisocoria obtained by stimulating different eyes at three stimulus conditions in (A) female subjects and (B) male subjects. The error bars indicate the SE. LA, light adapted, DA, dark adapted.
AmuntsK, ArmstrongE, MalikovicA, et al. Gender-specific left-right asymmetries in human visual cortex. J Neurosci. 2007;27(6)1356–1364. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
HilzMJ, DütschM, PerrineK, NelsonPK, RauhutU, DevinskyO. Hemispheric influence on autonomic modulation and baroreflex sensitivity. Ann Neurol. 2001;49(5)575–584. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
KirchnerA, PauliE, HilzMJ, NeundörferB, StefanH. Sex differences and lateral asymmetry in heart rate modulation in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2002;73(1)73–75. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
BarburJL. Learning from the pupil: studies of basic mechanisms and clinical application.ChalupaLM WernerJS eds. The Visual Neurosciences. 2003;641–656.MIT Press Cambridge, MA.
YuM, KautzMA, ThomasML, JohnsonD, HotchkissER, RussoMB. Operational implications of varying ambient light levels and time-of-day effects on saccadic velocity and pupillary light reflex. Ophthal Physiol Opt. 2007;27(2)130–141. [CrossRef]
TsujimuraS, WolffsohnJS, GilmartinB. Pupil response to color signals in cone-contrast space. Curr Eye Res. 2006;31(5)401–408. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
LinkB, JünemannA, RixR, et al. Pupillographic measurements with pattern stimulation: the pupil’s response in normal subjects and first measurements in glaucoma patients. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2006;47(11)4947–4955. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
WilhelmBJ, WilhelmH, MoroS, BarburJL. Pupil response components: studies in patients with Parinaud’s syndrome. Brain. 2004;125(Pt10)2296–2307.
GamlinPD, ZhangH, ClarkeRJ. Luminance neurons in the pretectal olivary nucleus mediate the pupillary light reflex in the rhesus monkey. Exp Brain Res. 1995;106(1)169–176. [PubMed]
YoungR, HanB, WuP. Transient and sustained components of the pupillary responses evoked by luminance and color. Vision Res. 1993;33(4)437–446. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
LowensteinO. Alternating contraction anisocoria. AMA Arch Neurol Psychiatry. 1954;72(6)742–757. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
JonesIS. Anisocoria: attempted induction by unilateral illumination. Arch Ophthal. 1949;42(3)249–253. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
SmithS, EllisC, SmithS. Inequality of the direct and consensual light reflexes in normal subjects. Br J Ophthalmol. 1979;63(7)523–527. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
CoxTA, DrewesCP. Contraction anisocoria resulting from half-field illumination. Am J Ophthalmol. 1984;97(5)577–582. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
WyattHJ, MusselmanJF. Pupillary light reflex in humans: evidence for an unbalanced pathway from nasal retina, and for signal cancellation in brainstem. Vision Res. 1981;21(4)513–525. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
SchmidR, WilhelmB, WilhelmH. Naso-temporal asymmetry and contraction anisocoria in the pupillomotor system. Graefes Arch Clin Exp Ophthalmol. 2000;238(2)123–128. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
SmithSA, SmithSE. Contraction anisocoria: nasal versus temporal illumination. Br J Ophthalmol. 1980;64(12)933–934. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
KupferC, ChumbleyL, DownerJC. Quantitative histology of optic nerve, optic tract and lateral geniculate nucleus of man. J Anat. 1967;101(pt 3)393–401. [PubMed]
GamlinPD, ClarkeRJ. The pupillary light reflex pathway of the primate. J Am Optom Assoc. 1995;66(7)415–418. [PubMed]
PiersonRJ, CarpenterMB. Anatomical analysis of pupillary reflex pathways in the rhesus monkey. J Comp Neurol. 1974;158(2)121–144. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
SteigerHJ, Büttner-EnneverJA. Oculomotor nucleus afferents in the monkey demonstrated with horseradish peroxidase. Brain Res. 1979;160(1)1–15. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
TukeyJW. Exploratory Data Analysis. 1977;Addison-Wesley Reading, MA.
BärKJ, BoettgerMK, TillS, DolicekJ, SauerH. Lateralization of pupillary light reflex parameters. Clin Neurophysiol. 2005;116(4)790–798. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
PapageorgiouE, TiciniLF, HardiessG, et al. The pupillary light reflex pathway: cytoarchitectonic probabilistic maps in hemianopic patients. Neurology. 2008;70(12)956–963. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
CoweyA, AlexanderI, HeywoodC, KentridgeR. Pupillary responses to coloured and contourless displays in total cerebral achromatopsia. Brain. 2008;131:2153–2160. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
HadjikhaniN, TootellRB. Projection of rods and cones within human visual cortex. Hum Brain Mapp. 2000;9(1)55–63. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
LongoniAM, OrsiniL. Lateral preferences in preschool children: a research note. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1988;29(4)533–539. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
GreenspanKA. Relationships among age, sex, and lateral dominance for 3- to 6-year-old children performing unilateral stance. Percept Mot Skills. 1990;71(2)615–623. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
MappAP, OnoH, BarbeitoR. What does the dominant eye dominate? A brief and somewhat contentious review. Percept Psychophys. 2003;65(2)310–317. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1.
 
An illustration of the parasympathetic pupillary pathways. Only the afferent signals from the right eye are illustrated. Arrows: direction of signal transduction. Thick line: stronger signal (not in proportion). CG, ciliary ganglions; EW, Edinger-Westphal nucleus; OC, optic chiasm; PON, pretectal olivary nucleus.
Figure 1.
 
An illustration of the parasympathetic pupillary pathways. Only the afferent signals from the right eye are illustrated. Arrows: direction of signal transduction. Thick line: stronger signal (not in proportion). CG, ciliary ganglions; EW, Edinger-Westphal nucleus; OC, optic chiasm; PON, pretectal olivary nucleus.
Figure 2.
 
Age distribution of all participants. Histograms are grouped by sex.
Figure 2.
 
Age distribution of all participants. Histograms are grouped by sex.
Figure 3.
 
Schematic diagram of the experimental setup for PLR recording. LED, stimulus light sources; IR LED, illumination light sources; BS, beam splitter; NDF, neutral density filter; HM, hot mirror; D, diffuser; CCD, near-infrared charge-coupled device camera.
Figure 3.
 
Schematic diagram of the experimental setup for PLR recording. LED, stimulus light sources; IR LED, illumination light sources; BS, beam splitter; NDF, neutral density filter; HM, hot mirror; D, diffuser; CCD, near-infrared charge-coupled device camera.
Figure 4.
 
Pupil size and anisocoria. (A) Box plot of pupil size in terms of side of eye, sex, and ambient light level. Boxes indicate the 25th and 75th percentiles, and whiskers show 1.5 times the interquartile range. The bar at the center of each box indicates the median value. (×) The maximal and the minimal data points. (B) The scatter plot of anisocoria measured under dark adaptation and light adaptation. Data points shown are mean values measured in each subject.
Figure 4.
 
Pupil size and anisocoria. (A) Box plot of pupil size in terms of side of eye, sex, and ambient light level. Boxes indicate the 25th and 75th percentiles, and whiskers show 1.5 times the interquartile range. The bar at the center of each box indicates the median value. (×) The maximal and the minimal data points. (B) The scatter plot of anisocoria measured under dark adaptation and light adaptation. Data points shown are mean values measured in each subject.
Figure 5.
 
Box plots of relative constriction amplitude of both sexes at three stimulus conditions. (A) Light-adapted 1012.5 photons/cm2/s, (B) dark-adapted 109.3 photons/cm2/s, and (C) dark-adapted 1012.5 photons/cm2/s.
Figure 5.
 
Box plots of relative constriction amplitude of both sexes at three stimulus conditions. (A) Light-adapted 1012.5 photons/cm2/s, (B) dark-adapted 109.3 photons/cm2/s, and (C) dark-adapted 1012.5 photons/cm2/s.
Figure 6.
 
Scatter plots of contraction anisocoria of both sexes as found by stimulating different eyes at three stimulus conditions: (A) light-adapted 1012.5 photons/cm2/s, (B) dark-adapted 109.3 photons/cm2/s, and (C) dark-adapted 1012.5 photons/cm2/s. Data points shown are mean values measured in each subject.
Figure 6.
 
Scatter plots of contraction anisocoria of both sexes as found by stimulating different eyes at three stimulus conditions: (A) light-adapted 1012.5 photons/cm2/s, (B) dark-adapted 109.3 photons/cm2/s, and (C) dark-adapted 1012.5 photons/cm2/s. Data points shown are mean values measured in each subject.
Figure 7.
 
Contraction anisocoria obtained by stimulating different eyes at three stimulus conditions in (A) female subjects and (B) male subjects. The error bars indicate the SE. LA, light adapted, DA, dark adapted.
Figure 7.
 
Contraction anisocoria obtained by stimulating different eyes at three stimulus conditions in (A) female subjects and (B) male subjects. The error bars indicate the SE. LA, light adapted, DA, dark adapted.
Table 1.
 
Average Pupil Size, and Anisocoria of Subjects Measured in Light- and Dark-adapted Conditions
Table 1.
 
Average Pupil Size, and Anisocoria of Subjects Measured in Light- and Dark-adapted Conditions
Sex Condition Pupil Diameter (mm) Anisocoria (mm)
Left Right
Female Light-adapted 6.25 ± 0.11 6.22 ± 0.11 −0.04 ± 0.05
Dark-adapted 6.85 ± 0.11 6.80 ± 0.11 −0.05 ± 0.05
Male Light-adapted 6.27 ± 0.12 6.32 ± 0.12 0.05 ± 0.06
Dark-adapted 6.74 ± 0.11 6.75 ± 0.11 0.001 ± 0.06
Table 2.
 
Relative Constriction Amplitudes of Subjects Measured by Stimulating Different Eyes at Three Stimulus Conditions
Table 2.
 
Relative Constriction Amplitudes of Subjects Measured by Stimulating Different Eyes at Three Stimulus Conditions
Sex Direct Eye Light Adapted, % Dark Adapted, %
1012.5 photons/cm2/s 109.3 photons/cm2/s 1012.5 photons/cm2/s
Direct Consensual Direct Consensual Direct Consensual
Female Left 26.00 ± 1.38 25.34 ± 1.38 20.81 ± 1.37 20.05 ± 1.37 50.98 ± 1.37 50.33 ± 1.38
Right 26.77 ± 1.37 25.51 ± 1.37 22.32 ± 1.38 21.25 ± 1.38 51.94 ± 1.38 51.15 ± 1.37
Male Left 24.82 ± 1.44 24.39 ± 1.44 20.22 ± 1.44 19.67 ± 1.44 55.04 ± 1.44 53.89 ± 1.44
Right 25.14 ± 1.43 23.75 ± 1.43 22.69 ± 1.44 19.90 ± 1.44 55.93 ± 1.44 54.03 ± 1.44
Table 3.
 
Contraction Anisocoria Measured in Subjects at Three Stimulus Conditions
Table 3.
 
Contraction Anisocoria Measured in Subjects at Three Stimulus Conditions
Sex Direct Eye Light Adapted Dark Adapted
1012.5 photons/cm2/s 109.3 photons/cm2/s 1012.5 photons/cm2/s
Mean ± SE P Mean ± SE P Mean ± SE P
Female Left 0.56 ± 0.32 0.0816 0.84 ± 0.30 0.0062* 0.96 ± 0.30 0.0018*
Right 1.19 ± 0.30 0.0001* 0.90 ± 0.31 0.0045* 0.73 ± 0.30 0.0145*
Male Left 0.42 ± 0.31 0.1836 0.26 ± 0.32 0.4105 1.21 ± 0.32 0.0002*
Right 1.40 ± 0.31 <0.0001* 2.90 ± 0.32 <0.0001* 2.12 ± 0.32 <0.0001*
Table 4.
 
Number and Percentage of Subjects Classified into Each of Three Categories of Contraction Anisocoria at Different Stimulus Conditions
Table 4.
 
Number and Percentage of Subjects Classified into Each of Three Categories of Contraction Anisocoria at Different Stimulus Conditions
Sex Category Light Adapted Dark Adapted
1012.5 photons/cm2/s 109.3 photons/cm2/s 1012.5 photons/cm2/s
No. % No. % No. %
Female None 10 43.5 7 30.4 7 30.4
Bilateral 3 13.0 2 8.7 2 8.7
Unilateral (left) 3 13.0 6 26.1 7 30.4
Unilateral (right) 7 30.4 8 34.8 7 30.4
Male None 7 33.3 4 19.0 2 9.5
Bilateral 2 9.5 2 9.5 4 19.0
Unilateral (left) 3 14.3 1 4.8 5 23.8
Unilateral (right) 9 42.9 14 66.7 10 47.6
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×