Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science Cover Image for Volume 50, Issue 3
March 2009
Volume 50, Issue 3
Free
Eye Movements, Strabismus, Amblyopia and Neuro-ophthalmology  |   March 2009
Modulation of Saccade Curvature by Ocular Counterroll
Author Affiliations
  • Konrad P. Weber
    From the Departments of Neurology,
  • Christopher J. Bockisch
    From the Departments of Neurology,
    Ophthalmology, and
    Otorhinolaryngology, Zurich University Hospital, Zurich, Switzerland.
  • Itsaso Olasagasti
    From the Departments of Neurology,
  • Dominik Straumann
    From the Departments of Neurology,
Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science March 2009, Vol.50, 1158-1167. doi:https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.08-2453
  • Views
  • PDF
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Konrad P. Weber, Christopher J. Bockisch, Itsaso Olasagasti, Dominik Straumann; Modulation of Saccade Curvature by Ocular Counterroll. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2009;50(3):1158-1167. https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.08-2453.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract

purpose. On close inspection, it can be seen that most saccadic trajectories are not straight but curve slightly; in other words, they are not single-axis ocular rotations. The authors asked whether saccade curvatures are systematically influenced by static ocular counterroll (OCR).

methods. OCR was elicited by static whole-body roll position. Eight healthy human subjects performed horizontal and vertical saccades (10° amplitude; 0° and 10° eccentricity; head-fixed coordinate system) in upright and ear-down whole-body roll positions (45° right, 45° left). Three-dimensional eye movements were recorded with modified dual-search coils at 1000 Hz.

results. Saccade curvature was systematically modulated by OCR depending on saccade direction. In the horizontal-vertical plane, primarily vertical saccades were modulated with downward saccades curving toward the upper ear and upward saccades curving toward the lower ear. Modulation of saccade curvature in the torsional direction correlated significantly with OCR only in abducting saccades.

conclusions. No universal mechanism, such as visual-motor coordinate transformation or kinematic characteristics of the saccadic burst generator, alone could explain the complex modulation pattern of saccade curvature. OCR-induced changes of the ocular motor plant, including transient force imbalances between agonist eye muscles (vertical rectus and oblique muscles) and shifting eye muscle pulleys, are suitable to explain the found direction-dependent modulation pattern.

It is commonly assumed that a saccade between two visual targets consists of a single-axis rotation of the ocular globe about a head-fixed axis. Mathematically, a single-axis rotation implies that the positional trajectory between movement onset and offset is straight when expressed as quaternion or rotation vectors. 1 2 3 Listing’s law states that, with the head not moving, these rotation vectors lie in a plane, called Listing’s plane. 4 Tweed and Vilis 5 have shown that visually guided saccades, at least to a first approximation, are single-axis rotations and obey Listing’s law. 
If the orientation of the ocular rotation axis changes during a saccade, its trajectory becomes curved. Saccades with curvatures in the horizontal-vertical plane can be elicited by displacing the visual target shortly after its appearance in a so-called double-step paradigm. 6 Clearly, such curved saccades cannot result from a single-axis rotation. However, single-axis rotations are not a necessary condition for saccades to obey Listing’s law. In fact, Minken et al. 7 showed that even strongly curved double-step saccades lie in Listing’s plane though their rotation axes change during displacement. 
On close examination, even normal saccades are slightly curved. 8 Systematic analysis of saccadic transients in the horizontal-vertical plane demonstrated curved trajectories predominantly during oblique saccades. 9 Three-dimensional (3D) measurements of saccades also revealed transient curvatures in the torsional direction, called blips. 10 Blips evoked by horizontal saccades systematically depend on direction and gaze elevation; blips evoked by vertical saccades are idiosyncratic but consistent on repetition. 11 In contrast to deviations in the horizontal-vertical plane, torsional transients by definition violate Listing’s law. 
In the present study, we asked whether saccade curvature depends on torsional eye position. A torsional offset that is added to eye positions before, during, and after saccades can be introduced by tilting the subject’s head in the roll plane. 12 Given that this ocular counterroll (OCR), which leads to a shift of Listing’s plane along the naso-occipital axis, 12 13 modifies the configuration of the extraocular muscles and their pulleys, 14 we expected increasing saccadic curvatures if they were the result of altered ocular motor plant characteristics. Alternatively, central mechanisms of signal transformation from retinal input to ocular motor output could also lead to predictable saccadic curvatures, depending on OCR. 15  
The study of saccade curvature requires high-resolution and artifact-free measurements of ocular trajectories in three dimensions. The dual-search coil technique is considered the gold standard because of its high temporal and spatial resolution. 16 17 Nonetheless, time and again, the validity of the torsional signal with standard search coils in humans has been questioned. 18 Therefore, we opted to base our analysis on measurements with search coils with a modified exiting wire 19 to ensure maximal torsional accuracy and validated the main findings with 3D video-oculography. 
Methods
Subjects
Eight healthy subjects (29–45 years; three women, five men) volunteered for the experiment. Informed consent was obtained from all subjects after the experimental procedure was explained. The protocol was approved by a local ethics committee and was conducted in accordance with the ethical standards laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki for research involving human subjects. 
Setup
Subjects were seated on a turntable with three servo-controlled, motor-driven axes (Acutronic, Jona, Switzerland). Pillows and safety belts minimized movements of the body. The head was restrained with an individually molded thermoplastic mask (Sinmed, Reeuwijk, Netherlands). 
Three-dimensional movements of the viewing eye were recorded with dual-search coils manufactured by Skalar (Delft, Netherlands). The coils were mounted on the eye after administration of topical anesthesia with oxybuprocaine hydrochloride 0.4% eye drops (Novartis Ophthalmics, Hettlingen, Switzerland). Dual-search coils were calibrated in vitro on a gimbal system before each experiment. 11 A chair-fixed coil frame (side length, 0.5 m) that produced three orthogonal magnetic fields with frequencies of 80, 96, and 120 kHz surrounded the subject’s head. A digital signal processor computed a fast Fourier transform in real-time on the digitized search coil signal to determine the voltage induced in the coil by each magnetic field (system manufactured by Primelec, Regensdorf, Switzerland). Eye position signals were sampled at 1000 Hz and were digitized with 12-bit precision. Coil orientation could be determined with a precision of approximately 0.05° and an error of less than 7% over a range of ±30°. Typical noise levels were lower than 0.05° (root-mean-square deviation). 
Experimental Procedure
Recordings were made in dim light. Subjects monocularly tracked a jumping laser target projected on a spherical screen at a distance of 1.4 m. In an alternate fashion, either eye was the viewing eye while the fellow eye was covered. For each eye, horizontal and vertical saccades with amplitudes of 10° were recorded at −10°, 0°, and 10° eccentricity. The laser dot jumped every 3 seconds back and forth without randomization. After every 10 saccades, a break of 10 seconds was provided for blinking. In total, 10 saccades for each direction and eccentricity were recorded. Saccades were recorded in upright, 45° left-ear-down, and 45° right-ear-down whole-body roll positions in a fixed order (6 × 10 minutes). Targets for horizontal and vertical saccades were presented in a head-fixed coordinate system (the position of the targets rotated with the subject). OCR at gaze straight ahead was determined for each eye in 45° left-ear-down and 45° right-ear-down whole-body roll positions and was referenced to zero eye torsion defined in an upright position. Because ocular torsion gradually adapts to roll tilt, torsional eye positions were determined at least 10 seconds after change of body position. 20  
Data Analysis
Ocular traces were analyzed with MatLab software (version 7.0.1; MathWorks, Natick, MA). Three-dimensional eye positions were low-pass filtered with a Savitzky-Golay filter 21 (quadratic polynomial; 11 ms window size) and were expressed as rotation vectors 1 22 in a head-fixed coordinate system that rotated with the subject. A rotation vector, r, described the instantaneous orientation of the eye as a single rotation from the reference position looking straight ahead. The rotation vector r was oriented parallel to the axis of this rotation, and its length was defined by tan(ρ/2), where ρ was the angle of rotation. Note that a single-axis rotation was represented by a straight line in rotation vector space. The components of the rotation vector r t,v,h represented torsional, vertical, and horizontal eye rotations. Signs of the rotation vector components were determined by the right-hand rule: clockwise, downward, and leftward rotations, as seen from the subject, are positive. In the figures, the signs of axes were defined accordingly, and rotation vector components were expressed in degrees by r deg = tan−1(r) · 360/π. From rotation vectors, angular velocity vectors ω t,v,h were derived. 23 Angular velocity vectors point along the instantaneous rotation axis, and their lengths were proportional to the rotational speed. 
Saccades were sorted according to gaze direction. Each saccade, including 1 second of presaccadic and 3 seconds postsaccadic fixation, was processed by a computer program and was interactively selected. Selection criteria included start and end positions in proximity to the visual targets, maintained before and after saccadic fixation, and absence of blinks. On average, 79% ± 6% SD of 720 saccades per subject were accepted for further analysis. Horizontal, vertical, and torsional traces were aligned to the median eye position during the 250-ms fixation period preceding the saccade. Individual traces were shifted along the time axis such that they aligned at the instant saccades passed a level of 3°. 
Video Setup for Complementary Eye Movement Recordings
Three-dimensional eye movements were binocularly recorded at 200 Hz with 3D video-oculography 24 (Eye Tracker Version 1C/2003; Chronos Vision, Berlin, Germany) mounted on the thermoplastic mask. For every experimental condition, the system was calibrated at 0° ± 10° horizontal and vertical positions. 25 To optimize pupil tracking and torsion analysis, pupils were constricted with pilocarpine 0.5% eye drops. Raw video data were processed with the iris tracker software (version 2.1.6.1; Chronos Vision). Pupil tracking to obtain horizontal and vertical eye positions was performed using an algorithm based on the Hough transform and ellipse fitting. 24 Ocular torsion was determined by iris segment cross-correlation. 26  
Results
To analyze the 3D kinematic properties of saccades, horizontal, vertical, and torsional components of rotation vectors were plotted against each other in different planes, corresponding to different views of the trajectories. Figure 1depicts horizontal and torsional curvatures during 10° downward saccades in upright, 45° left-ear-down, and 45° right-ear-down whole-body roll positions for a typical subject. In the vertical-horizontal plane (Figs. 1A 1A 1C) , the view from the subject’s perspective, downward saccades of the viewing left eye were curved to the right in an upright position (Fig. 1B) . The horizontal curvature became larger in left-ear-down position (Fig. 1A)and even reversed in right-ear-down position (Fig. 1C) . Curvature increased slightly with 10° adduction (right gaze) and decreased with 10° abduction (Fig. 1B) , compared between overlays of left and right gaze. In the vertical-torsional plane (Figs. 1D 1E 1F) , there was little curvature compared with the trajectories in the vertical-horizontal plane (Figs. 1A 1B 1C) . In this example, intorsional curvature increased slightly in left-ear-down position (Fig. 1D) . Upward saccades of the same left eye (data not shown) deviated to the right as well with the subject in upright position. Contrary to downward saccades, rightward curvature of upward saccades increased in the right-ear-down position and decreased in the left-ear-down position. 
Figure 2shows horizontal saccades of the same subject as in Figure 1 . Adducting rightward 10° saccades of the viewing left eye were almost straight in the horizontal-vertical plane in the upright position (Fig. 2B) . In contrast to vertical saccades, trajectories were only slightly modulated by roll position, with increasing upward curvature in the right-ear-down position (Figs. 2A 2C) . Similarly, compared with vertical saccades, the effect of gaze eccentricity was less pronounced during horizontal saccades: trajectories tended to bend downward at 10° downgaze (Fig. 2B) . In the horizontal-torsional plane, adducting saccades made along the horizontal meridian showed a small extorsional curvature (Fig. 2E) . These torsional deviations were similar in both ear-down positions (Figs. 2D 2F) . Abducting saccades showed a reversed pattern with a small downward curvature that increased in right-ear-down position (data not shown). In abducting saccades, intorsional curvature slightly increased at the contralateral ear-down position. 
Figure 3illustrates how horizontal curvature of a typical downward saccade of a left eye was analyzed. Saccade onset was defined 4 ms before angular velocity crossed the threshold of 30°/s. 27 Saccade offset was defined 4 ms after velocity declined below this level (Fig. 3C , dashed vertical lines). This criterion excluded postsaccadic drift and possible catch-up saccades from the analysis. The moment of peak acceleration was determined by the maximum of the derivative of angular velocity vector length (Fig. 3D , solid vertical line). Typically, peak acceleration was reached early during the course of the saccade (Fig. 3A , arrow). A negative transient in the horizontal eye position represented the rightward curvature of the saccade (Fig. 3B , dark-gray trace). As a result of the horizontal transient, the velocity trace of the horizontal component appeared biphasic (Fig. 3C , dark-gray trace). 
To analyze the behavior of the ocular rotation axis during saccades, we used angular velocity vectors. 23 Note that angular velocity vectors are oriented horizontally during vertical displacements and vertically during horizontal displacements; directions of vectors follow the right-hand rule. For example, the angular velocity vector of a downward saccade points to the left along the (positive) ωv-axis. 
Figure 4shows angular velocity vectors of three typical downward saccades in a left eye. In the upright position, angular velocity vectors formed a loop with an early negative and later a positive vertical component added to the main positive horizontal component (Fig. 4B ; same saccade as in Fig. 3 ). This loop reflects the rightward curvature of the downward saccade. The nonlinear trajectory implies that the orientation of the ocular rotation axis was not stable during the saccade but rotated clockwise (from the subject’s view) in the vertical-horizontal plane. Mean angular velocity vector (bold arrow) of the saccade, however, pointed horizontally to the left, as expected if the eye were finally to reach the vertically displaced target. The initial angular velocity vector from saccade onset to peak acceleration (empty arrow) pointed down and to the left, indicating a rightward deviation at the beginning of the saccade. In the torsional direction (Fig. 4E) , angular velocity vectors were closely aligned during the entire saccade, indicating no torsional curvature in the upright position. 
Roll tilt toward the left ear (Figs. 4A 4D)increased the loop of angular velocity vectors in the vertical-horizontal plane, corresponding to an increase of horizontal curvature of the downward saccade. Roll tilt toward the right ear (Figs. 4C 4F)decreased the loop of angular velocity vectors in the vertical-horizontal plane. In the torsional direction, however, angular velocity vectors remained almost aligned in both ear-down positions. 
Saccade curvature was quantified by the angle between the initial angular velocity vector and the mean angular velocity vector in the respective plane. The sign of the angle was determined by the sign of the direction of the saccade multiplied by the sign of the direction of the deviation according to the right-hand rule. For example, a downward saccade (positive) with a rightward curvature (negative) resulted in a negative deviation angle. 
Figure 5summarizes the modulation of horizontal curvature of saccades made along the vertical meridian by different roll positions for all subjects. Except for one extreme value, mean horizontal curvature in downward saccades of viewing left eyes scattered around zero in the upright position (Fig. 5A , up). In the right-ear-down position, curvature systematically increased toward the left (upper) ear and vice versa. Upward saccades of viewing left eyes showed a similar pattern: Curvature scattered around zero in the upright position (Fig. 5C , up) but systematically increased toward the lower ear in the ear-down position. The right eye followed the same rule when viewing (Figs. 5B 5D) : downward saccades curved toward the upper ear, and upward saccades curved toward the lower ear in the ear-down position. 
Figure 6depicts torsional curvature of vertical saccades of all subjects using the same definition as in Figure 5 . Downward saccades of viewing left eyes predominantly showed a small extorsional curvature when subjects were in the upright position (Fig. 6A) . In contrast to saccade curvature in the vertical-horizontal plane (Fig. 5A) , there was no consistent modulation pattern by roll position in the torsional direction. Viewing right eyes showed a mirrored pattern with extorsional saccade curvature in the upright position (Fig. 6B) . Upward saccades exhibited a small extorsional curvature in the upright position (Figs. 6C 6D) . In contrast to downward saccades, upward saccades showed a small but systematic modulation by roll position with intorsion of the lower eye. 
For statistical analysis, right eyes were mirrored as left eyes, and all eyes were pooled (n = 16 eyes of eight subjects). To quantify the modulation of saccade curvature by roll position, linear regression was performed for each eye through curvature of every saccade in left-ear-down, upright, and right-ear-down positions. Figure 7summarizes the results for saccades made along the vertical meridian. The positive slope for downward saccades denotes a modulation of horizontal curvature by roll position toward the upper ear (Fig. 7A , first column). In the t-test, slopes were significantly different from zero (*P < 0.01). Mean offset (Fig. 7C , first column, open square), however, was close to zero, which indicates no directional preponderance of horizontal curvature in the upright position. The slope of torsional curvature scattered mostly around zero, suggesting no systematic modulation of torsional curvature of downward saccades by roll position (Fig. 7A , second column). The offset of torsional curvature was significantly different from zero, indicating an extorsional curvature in downward saccades (Fig. 7C , second column). Upward saccades exhibited a similar pattern of horizontal curvature as downward saccades. On average, downward saccades were straight in the upright position (Fig. 7D , first column) and were significantly curved toward the lower ear in the ear-down position (Fig. 7B , first column). Torsional curvature of upward saccades showed a small but significant modulation by roll position with intorsion of the lower eye but no significant predominance of torsional curvature in the upright position (Figs. 7B 7D , second column). 
Analogous to the previous figure for vertical saccades, Figure 8summarizes the analysis of saccades along the horizontal meridian. In abducting saccades, vertical curvature and torsional curvature were modulated by the whole-body roll position (Fig. 8A) . Adducting saccades showed only a small modulation of vertical curvature and no consistent modulation of torsional curvature (Fig. 8B) . In ear-down positions, saccadic trajectories of both eyes toward the lower ear tended to bend upward, whereas trajectories toward the upper ear bended downward (Figs. 8A 8B , first column). Torsional curvature was modulated only in abducting saccades with increasing intorsion at contralateral ear-down position (Fig. 8A , second column). In the upright position, abducting saccades showed a significant downward and intorsional deviation (Fig. 8C) . Adducting saccades exhibited extorsional curvature (Fig. 8D , second column) in the same direction as the intorsional curvature of the abducting fellow eye. 
In a next step, we asked whether the magnitude of saccade curvature was related to the amount of OCR. If so, subjects with higher OCR amplitudes should show greater modulation of saccade curvature as a function of whole-body roll. OCR amplitudes from the 45° left-ear-down to the 45° right-ear-down position (mean 12.0° ± 4.2° SD, both eyes pooled) were correlated against the slopes from Figures 7A 7B and 8A 8Bas a measure of modulation of saccade curvature. Vertical saccades showed significant correlation between horizontal curvature and OCR (Pearson correlation coefficient for downward saccades, r = 0.73, P = 0.0012; for upward saccades, r = 0.51, P = 0.043). In contrast, the correlation between vertical curvatures of horizontal saccades and OCR was not significant. Torsional curvatures of horizontal and vertical saccades did not significantly correlate with OCR, except for abducting saccades (r = 0.62, P = 0.01). 
For a visual validation of our results, we compared the dual-search coil recordings with 3D video-oculography. Figures 9A 9B 9C 9D 9E 9Fshow 3D video-oculographic data collected from the same subject and during the same paradigm as in Figure 1 . Downward saccades showed the same modulation pattern of horizontal curvature, with a small offset between both recording methods (Fig. 9G) . Torsional curvature of downward saccades was comparable and showed a predominantly extorsional offset and little modulation compared with the horizontal curvature with both methods (Fig. 9H) . The same correspondence between dual-search coil recordings and 3D video-oculography was found in all four subjects tested with both recording methods. 
Discussion
The purpose of this study was to explore the influence of OCR on the kinematics of saccades. We found a specific modulation pattern of saccade curvature by OCR, depending on saccade direction (qualitative summary, Fig. 10 ). OCR predominantly influenced horizontal curvatures of vertical saccades. Downward saccades curved to the upper ear, whereas upward saccades curved to the lower ear in roll positions. Vertical curvature of horizontal saccades was also slightly modulated by ear-down positions (Figs. 8A 8B) , but the direct correlation with OCR did not reach significance. Modulation of saccade curvature by OCR was also found in the torsional direction, but this effect was significant only in abducting horizontal saccades. 
To explain the complex modulation patterns of saccade curvatures by OCR, we considered different mechanisms along the path of signal transformation from retinal input to ocular motor output. Saccade curvatures could arise as a result of a mismatch between retinal and ocular motor coordinates, an imperfection of the saccadic burst generator to observe the noncommutative properties of 3D eye rotations, and characteristics of the ocular motor plant including transient force imbalances between agonist eye muscles (vertical rectus and oblique muscles) and eye muscle pulley shifts during vertical saccades. 
Mechanism 1: Mismatch between Retinal and Ocular Motor Coordinates
A mismatch between the sensed oculocentric target directions and head-fixed ocular motor commands during OCR could explain the horizontal curvatures of vertical saccades in ear-down positions. 15 As in Schworm et al., 20 45° head-roll induced approximately 6° OCR in our subjects. Hence, head-fixed targets appear rotated clockwise by 6° in the right-ear-down position. Therefore, the target for a downward saccade appears down and shifted to the left, and the target for an upward saccade appears up and shifted to the right. The initial motor command would aim toward the perceived, not the actual, target location if OCR were not considered by the visual-motor transformation mechanism. To prevent a directional mismatch between the end point of the saccade and the target, an adaptation-driven mechanism ensures that during the latter part of the saccade, the initial directional error is corrected and the saccade trajectory curves toward the actual target. This mechanism would hold not only for vertical but also for horizontal saccades. The horizontal curvature of vertical saccades is compatible with this hypothesis, but the pattern of vertical curvature during horizontal saccades is not. 
Although OCR reduces the roll-tilt of the visual input in ear-down positions, perceptional studies demonstrated that subjects tend to overestimate the angle of the perceived earth vertical in body roll positions less than 60°. 28 This observation, called the e-effect, was also found if subjects had to indicate earth vertical (not body vertical) directly with saccadic eye movements. 29 Contrary to the coordinate transformation hypothesis described, such a perceptual hypothesis would explain results only for vertical curvature of horizontal saccades but not for horizontal curvature of vertical saccades. 
Mechanism 2: Kinematic Imperfections of the Saccadic Burst Generator
There has been a long debate about whether the explicit implementation of Listing’s law for saccades occurs at the level of premotor neurons, the ocular plant, or both. Tweed and Vilis 2 described a model of neural controller that takes into account the noncommutativity of rotations to encode a 3D eye position signal in Listing’s plane. The existence of a saccadic burst generator that takes into account current eye position has been questioned by several authors, though a commutative controller invariably produces torsional deviations during and after saccades. 30 The amplitudes of those so-called blips, however, is considerably smaller than predicted in the computer model. 11 This observation led to the idea that Listing’s law is implemented at the level of eye muscle pulleys. 31 These structures, consisting of connective tissue and smooth muscles, change extraocular muscle pulling directions as a function of eye position. Such a mechanical implementation of Listing’s law is suitable to simplify the neural control of saccades. Recent experiments in monkeys confirmed that horizontal eye movements still obey Listing’s law when they are elicited by microelectric stimulation of the abducens nerve. 32 The preservation of Listing’s law during such nonphysiological activation of the lateral rectus muscle further supports the notion that this law is implemented at the level of the ocular motor plant. 
Indeed, our data show no universal modulation pattern as predicted by a kinematic mechanism but do show a pattern that is different depending on saccade direction. In addition, there is a striking disparity between the modulation of saccade curvature in the horizontal-vertical plane compared with the torsional direction. For example, clear horizontal modulation of vertical saccades is scarcely reflected in ocular torsion (Fig. 7A) . This dissociation further argues against an explicit 3D saccadic burst generator and in favor of a mechanical implementation of Listing’s law. 
Mechanism 3: Characteristics of the Ocular Motor Plant
The dynamics and kinematics of saccades are determined by force changes in agonist eye muscles. Although agonist activity during horizontal eye movements is restricted to one muscle, agonist activity during vertical eye movements involves two muscles. 33 Unless their secondary actions in the torsional and horizontal directions cancel, the eyes will deviate from a straight vertical trajectory. 34 35 For example, both the superior oblique and the inferior rectus muscle contract during downward saccades. Secondary actions of these muscles in the torsional (superior oblique, intorsion; inferior rectus, extorsion) and horizontal (superior oblique, abduction; inferior rectus, adduction) directions must cancel each other for a saccade to be perfectly downward. 
OCR leads to an imbalance between the two coagonist eye muscles for downward saccades. During intorsion, the superior oblique muscle contracts and stretches the inferior rectus muscle. 36 Considering the length-tension curve of eye muscles, 37 38 39 40 the shorter (precontracted) superior oblique muscle will develop less traction than the longer (stretched) inferior rectus muscle. Hence, OCR reciprocally changes the dynamic characteristics of the two coagonist eye muscles. This transient force imbalance between the two vertically pulling coagonist muscles can lead to a curved saccade trajectory. During the initial part of a downward saccade, the eye would deviate toward the pulling direction of the inferior rectus muscle—that is, medially and extorsionally—similar to the pattern found in patients with trochlear nerve palsy. 41 42 During the final part of the saccade, the eye would deviate toward the pulling direction of the superior oblique muscle—that is, intorsionally and laterally. In our data, the observed horizontal curvatures of vertical saccades are consistent with such transient force imbalances during saccades with two coagonist muscles. 
Transient force imbalances between coagonist muscles during vertical saccades should not cause only horizontal but also torsional curvatures that are modulated by OCR. Such a modulation of torsional curvature, however, was not evident in our data. Possibly, the configuration of rectus muscle pulleys, which shift in the presence of OCR, cancels torsional curvature to preserve Listing’s law even in roll positions. 14  
Similarly, shifts of eye muscle pulleys could also provide an explanation for direction-dependent saccade curvature in the horizontal-vertical plane. Clark et al. 43 44 have shown that lateral rectus muscle pulleys shift with vertical gaze, whereas there is no commensurate pulley shift with horizontal gaze. Such distinct characteristics of individual eye muscle pulleys would help to explain different curvature patterns, depending on saccade direction. 
Conclusion
Neither of the proposed universal mechanisms, visual-motor coordinate transformations or kinematic characteristics of the saccadic burst generator, can explain the entire modulation pattern of saccade curvature by OCR that we found. Consideration of characteristics of the ocular motor plant allows for individual deviation patterns, depending on saccade direction. Dynamic interactions of agonist eye muscles in vertical saccades and shifting eye muscle pulleys are both suitable to explain such a direction-dependent modulation pattern by OCR. Additional insights can be expected from studies of saccade curvature in patients with individual eye muscle palsies. 42  
 
Figure 1.
 
Modulation of horizontal (AC) and torsional (DF) curvature during vertical saccades at different whole-body roll positions. Example of 10° downward saccades of a left eye at −10°, 0°, and 10° horizontal eccentricities in upright, left-ear-down, and right-ear-down positions. (AC) Eye position in the vertical-horizontal plane (projection from the subject’s perspective). Starting positions are aligned for clarity. (DF) Vertical-torsional plane. Separate saccade trajectories represent different horizontal eccentricities corresponding to panels (A) to (C). (A, D) 45° left-ear-down whole-body roll position (LED). (B, E) Upright position (up). (C, F) 45° right-ear-down whole-body roll position (RED).
Figure 1.
 
Modulation of horizontal (AC) and torsional (DF) curvature during vertical saccades at different whole-body roll positions. Example of 10° downward saccades of a left eye at −10°, 0°, and 10° horizontal eccentricities in upright, left-ear-down, and right-ear-down positions. (AC) Eye position in the vertical-horizontal plane (projection from the subject’s perspective). Starting positions are aligned for clarity. (DF) Vertical-torsional plane. Separate saccade trajectories represent different horizontal eccentricities corresponding to panels (A) to (C). (A, D) 45° left-ear-down whole-body roll position (LED). (B, E) Upright position (up). (C, F) 45° right-ear-down whole-body roll position (RED).
Figure 2.
 
Modulation of vertical (AC) and torsional (DF) curvature during horizontal saccades at different whole-body roll positions. 10° adducting rightward saccades of a viewing left eye at −10°, 0°, and 10° vertical eccentricity in upright, left-ear-down, and right-ear-down positions. (AC) Eye position in the horizontal-vertical plane (projection from the subject’s perspective). Starting positions are aligned for clarity. (DF) Horizontal-torsional plane. Separate saccade trajectories represent different vertical eccentricities corresponding to panels (A) to (C). (A, D) 45° left-ear-down whole-body roll position (LED). (B, E) Upright position (up). (C, F) 45° right-ear-down whole-body roll position (RED).
Figure 2.
 
Modulation of vertical (AC) and torsional (DF) curvature during horizontal saccades at different whole-body roll positions. 10° adducting rightward saccades of a viewing left eye at −10°, 0°, and 10° vertical eccentricity in upright, left-ear-down, and right-ear-down positions. (AC) Eye position in the horizontal-vertical plane (projection from the subject’s perspective). Starting positions are aligned for clarity. (DF) Horizontal-torsional plane. Separate saccade trajectories represent different vertical eccentricities corresponding to panels (A) to (C). (A, D) 45° left-ear-down whole-body roll position (LED). (B, E) Upright position (up). (C, F) 45° right-ear-down whole-body roll position (RED).
Figure 3.
 
Analysis of the curvature of a typical 10° downward saccade in a left eye. (A) Vertical-horizontal plane (projection from the subject’s perspective). Bold trace: trajectory from saccade onset to saccade offset. Thin trace: subsequent catch-up saccade. Dashed line: straight connection between onset and offset. Arrow: eye position at peak acceleration. (B) Traces of torsional (light gray), vertical (black), and horizontal (dark gray) rotation vector components (converted to degrees). Dashed vertical lines: saccade onset and offset. Solid vertical line: peak acceleration. (C) Traces of angular velocity vector components. (D) Trace of the derivative of angular velocity vector length.
Figure 3.
 
Analysis of the curvature of a typical 10° downward saccade in a left eye. (A) Vertical-horizontal plane (projection from the subject’s perspective). Bold trace: trajectory from saccade onset to saccade offset. Thin trace: subsequent catch-up saccade. Dashed line: straight connection between onset and offset. Arrow: eye position at peak acceleration. (B) Traces of torsional (light gray), vertical (black), and horizontal (dark gray) rotation vector components (converted to degrees). Dashed vertical lines: saccade onset and offset. Solid vertical line: peak acceleration. (C) Traces of angular velocity vector components. (D) Trace of the derivative of angular velocity vector length.
Figure 4.
 
Example of downward saccades in different whole-body roll positions. Trajectories of angular velocity vectors represent ocular rotation axes. Bold arrow: mean angular velocity vector of the entire saccade. Empty arrow: initial angular velocity vector from saccade onset to peak acceleration. Lengths of arrows are multiplied by 1.5 for clarity. (AC) Vertical-horizontal plane. (DF) Vertical-torsional plane. (A, D) 45° left-ear-down whole-body roll position (LED). (B, E) Upright position (up). (C, F) 45° right-ear-down whole-body roll position (RED).
Figure 4.
 
Example of downward saccades in different whole-body roll positions. Trajectories of angular velocity vectors represent ocular rotation axes. Bold arrow: mean angular velocity vector of the entire saccade. Empty arrow: initial angular velocity vector from saccade onset to peak acceleration. Lengths of arrows are multiplied by 1.5 for clarity. (AC) Vertical-horizontal plane. (DF) Vertical-torsional plane. (A, D) 45° left-ear-down whole-body roll position (LED). (B, E) Upright position (up). (C, F) 45° right-ear-down whole-body roll position (RED).
Figure 5.
 
Modulation of horizontal curvature of saccades made along the vertical meridian as a function of whole-body roll position. Connected symbols: mean saccade curvature in the horizontal direction of individual subjects. (A, B) 10° downward saccades. (C, D) 10° upward saccades. (A, C) Viewing left eye. (B, D) Viewing right eye. Ordinate: horizontal saccade curvature (direction indicated by the respective curved arrows). Dashed line: level of straight saccade. Abscissa: 45° left-ear-down (LED), upright (up), and 45° right-ear-down whole-body roll position (RED).
Figure 5.
 
Modulation of horizontal curvature of saccades made along the vertical meridian as a function of whole-body roll position. Connected symbols: mean saccade curvature in the horizontal direction of individual subjects. (A, B) 10° downward saccades. (C, D) 10° upward saccades. (A, C) Viewing left eye. (B, D) Viewing right eye. Ordinate: horizontal saccade curvature (direction indicated by the respective curved arrows). Dashed line: level of straight saccade. Abscissa: 45° left-ear-down (LED), upright (up), and 45° right-ear-down whole-body roll position (RED).
Figure 6.
 
Modulation of torsional curvature in saccades made along the vertical meridian as a function of whole-body roll position. Connected symbols: mean saccade curvature in the torsional direction of individual subjects. (A, B) 10° downward saccades. (C, D) 10° upward saccades. (A, C) Viewing left eye. (B, D) Viewing right eye. Ordinate: torsional saccade curvature (intor, extor: torsion of the respective eye). Dashed line: level of straight saccade. Abscissa: 45° left-ear-down (LED), upright (up), and 45° right-ear-down whole-body roll position (RED).
Figure 6.
 
Modulation of torsional curvature in saccades made along the vertical meridian as a function of whole-body roll position. Connected symbols: mean saccade curvature in the torsional direction of individual subjects. (A, B) 10° downward saccades. (C, D) 10° upward saccades. (A, C) Viewing left eye. (B, D) Viewing right eye. Ordinate: torsional saccade curvature (intor, extor: torsion of the respective eye). Dashed line: level of straight saccade. Abscissa: 45° left-ear-down (LED), upright (up), and 45° right-ear-down whole-body roll position (RED).
Figure 7.
 
Horizontal and torsional curvatures during vertical saccades as a function of whole-body roll position. (A, C) Downward saccades. (B, D) Upward saccades. (A, B) Slopes indicating modulation of horizontal curvature (first column) and torsional curvature (second column) as a function of roll position. (C, D) Offsets in horizontal curvature (first column) and torsional curvature (second column) in upright position. Open circles: average values of each eye (n = 16 eyes of 8 subjects). Open squares with error bars: mean ± 1 SD. *Mean is significantly different from zero (t-test; P < 0.01).
Figure 7.
 
Horizontal and torsional curvatures during vertical saccades as a function of whole-body roll position. (A, C) Downward saccades. (B, D) Upward saccades. (A, B) Slopes indicating modulation of horizontal curvature (first column) and torsional curvature (second column) as a function of roll position. (C, D) Offsets in horizontal curvature (first column) and torsional curvature (second column) in upright position. Open circles: average values of each eye (n = 16 eyes of 8 subjects). Open squares with error bars: mean ± 1 SD. *Mean is significantly different from zero (t-test; P < 0.01).
Figure 8.
 
Vertical and torsional curvatures during horizontal saccades as a function of whole-body roll position. (A, C) Abducting saccades. (B, D) Adducting saccades. (A, B) Slopes indicating modulation of vertical curvature (first column) and torsional curvature (second column) as a function of roll position. (C, D) Offsets in vertical curvature (first column) and torsional curvature (second column) in upright position. Open circles: average values of each eye (n = 16 eyes of 8 subjects). Open squares with error bars: mean ± 1 SD. *Mean is significantly different from zero (t-test, P < 0.01).
Figure 8.
 
Vertical and torsional curvatures during horizontal saccades as a function of whole-body roll position. (A, C) Abducting saccades. (B, D) Adducting saccades. (A, B) Slopes indicating modulation of vertical curvature (first column) and torsional curvature (second column) as a function of roll position. (C, D) Offsets in vertical curvature (first column) and torsional curvature (second column) in upright position. Open circles: average values of each eye (n = 16 eyes of 8 subjects). Open squares with error bars: mean ± 1 SD. *Mean is significantly different from zero (t-test, P < 0.01).
Figure 9.
 
Comparison of 3D video-oculography (AF) with dual-search coil recordings (same paradigm and same subject as in Fig. 1 ). (AF) 3D video-oculography of the left eye during downward saccades. (G) Horizontal curvature during downward saccades recorded with 3D video-oculography (empty triangles; data from Figs. 9A 9B 9C ) compared with dual-search coils (filled triangles; data from Figs. 1A 1B 1C ). (H) Torsional curvature in downward saccades recorded with 3D video-oculography (empty triangles; data from Figs. 9D 9E 9F ) compared with dual-search coils (filled triangles; data from Fig. 1D 1E 1F ).
Figure 9.
 
Comparison of 3D video-oculography (AF) with dual-search coil recordings (same paradigm and same subject as in Fig. 1 ). (AF) 3D video-oculography of the left eye during downward saccades. (G) Horizontal curvature during downward saccades recorded with 3D video-oculography (empty triangles; data from Figs. 9A 9B 9C ) compared with dual-search coils (filled triangles; data from Figs. 1A 1B 1C ). (H) Torsional curvature in downward saccades recorded with 3D video-oculography (empty triangles; data from Figs. 9D 9E 9F ) compared with dual-search coils (filled triangles; data from Fig. 1D 1E 1F ).
Figure 10.
 
Modulation of saccade curvature by whole-body roll position in a left eye (qualitative summary plot). Horizontal and vertical saccade curvature as seen from the subject’s perspective. Shaded gray: significant correlation between modulation of saccade curvature and OCR amplitude. ab, abduction; ad, adduction; LED, left-ear-down position; RED, right-ear-down position.
Figure 10.
 
Modulation of saccade curvature by whole-body roll position in a left eye (qualitative summary plot). Horizontal and vertical saccade curvature as seen from the subject’s perspective. Shaded gray: significant correlation between modulation of saccade curvature and OCR amplitude. ab, abduction; ad, adduction; LED, left-ear-down position; RED, right-ear-down position.
The authors thank Oliver Bergamin, Sarah Marti, Antonella Palla, Alexander Tarnutzer, and Albert Züger for assistance and the reviewers for their helpful comments. 
HausteinW. Considerations on Listing’s law and the primary position by means of a matrix description of eye position control. Biol Cybern. 1989;60:411–420. [PubMed]
TweedD, VilisT. Implications of rotational kinematics for the oculomotor system in three dimensions. J Neurophysiol. 1987;58:832–849. [PubMed]
WestheimerG. Kinematics of the eye. J Opt Soc Am. 1957;47:967–974. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
HelmholtzHV. Handbuch der Physiologischen Optik. 1867;Voss Hamburg und Leibzig.
TweedD, VilisT. Geometric relations of eye position and velocity vectors during saccades. Vision Res. 1990;30:111–127. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
BeckerW, JürgensR. An analysis of the saccadic system by means of double step stimuli. Vision Res. 1979;19:967–983. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
MinkenAW, Van OpstalAJ, Van GisbergenJA. Three-dimensional analysis of strongly curved saccades elicited by double-step stimuli. Exp Brain Res. 1993;93:521–533. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
BahillAT, StarkL. The trajectories of saccadic eye movements. Sci Am. 1979;240:108–117. [PubMed]
SmitAC, Van GisbergenJA. An analysis of curvature in fast and slow human saccades. Exp Brain Res. 1990;81:335–345. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
TweedD, MisslischH, FetterM. Testing models of the oculomotor velocity-to-position transformation. J Neurophysiol. 1994;72:1425–1429. [PubMed]
StraumannD, ZeeDS, SolomonD, et al. Transient torsion during and after saccades. Vision Res. 1995;35:3321–3334. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
BockischCJ, HaslwanterT. Three-dimensional eye position during static roll and pitch in humans. Vision Res. 2001;41:2127–2137. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
HaslwanterT, StraumannD, HessBJ, et al. Static roll and pitch in the monkey: shift and rotation of Listing’s plane. Vision Res. 1992;32:1341–1348. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
DemerJL, ClarkRA. Magnetic resonance imaging of human extraocular muscles during static ocular counter-rolling. J Neurophysiol. 2005;94:3292–3302. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
KlierEM, CrawfordJD. Human oculomotor system accounts for 3-D eye orientation in the visual-motor transformation for saccades. J Neurophysiol. 1998;80:2274–2294. [PubMed]
CollewijnH, Van der MarkF, JansenTC. Precise recording of human eye movements. Vision Res. 1975;15:447–450. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
RobinsonDA. A method of measuring eye movement using a scleral-search coil in a magnetic field. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng. 1963;10:137–145. [PubMed]
StraumannD, ZeeDS, SolomonD, et al. Validity of Listing’s law during fixations, saccades, smooth pursuit eye movements, and blinks. Exp Brain Res. 1996;112:135–146. [PubMed]
BergaminO, RamatS, StraumannD, et al. Influence of orientation of exiting wire of search coil annulus on torsion after saccades. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2004;45:131–137. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
SchwormHD, YggeJ, PansellT, et al. Assessment of ocular counterroll during head tilt using binocular video oculography. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2002;43:662–667. [PubMed]
SavitzkyA, GolayMJE. Smoothing + differentiation of data by simplified least squares procedures. Anal Chem. 1964;36:1627. [CrossRef]
HaslwanterT. Mathematics of three-dimensional eye rotations. Vision Res. 1995;35:1727–1739. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
HeppK. On Listing’s law. Commun Math Phys. 1990;132:285–292. [CrossRef]
ClarkeAH, DitterichJ, DruenK, et al. Using high frame rate CMOS sensors for three-dimensional eye tracking. Behav Res Methods Instr Comput. 2002;34:549–560. [CrossRef]
PeterkaRJ, MerfeldDM. Calibration techniques for video-oculography. Vision Res. 1996;6:S75.
HatamianM, AndersonDJ. Design considerations for a real-time ocular counterroll instrument. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng. 1983;30:278–288. [PubMed]
Van OpstalAJ, Van GisbergenJA. Skewness of saccadic velocity profiles: a unifying parameter for normal and slow saccades. Vision Res. 1987;27:731–745. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
HowardIP. Human Visual Orientation. 1982;Wiley New York.
Van BeuzekomAD, Van GisbergenJA. Properties of the internal representation of gravity inferred from spatial-direction and body-tilt estimates. J Neurophysiol. 2000;84:11–27. [PubMed]
SchnabolkC, RaphanT. Modeling three-dimensional velocity-to-position transformation in oculomotor control. J Neurophysiol. 1994;71:623–638. [PubMed]
RaphanT. Modeling control of eye orientation in three dimensions, I: role of muscle pulleys in determining saccadic trajectory. J Neurophysiol. 1998;79:2653–2667. [PubMed]
KlierEM, MengH, AngelakiDE. Three-dimensional kinematics at the level of the oculomotor plant. J Neurosci. 2006;26:2732–2737. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
HeringE. Die Lehre vom binocularen Sehen. 1868;Verlag von Wilhelm Engelmann Leipzig.
BoederP. The co-operation of extraocular muscles. Am J Ophthalmol. 1961;51:469–481. [CrossRef]
VolkmannAW. Zur Mechanik der Augenmuskeln. Ber Verh Sachs Ges Wsch. 1869;21:28–69.
BielschowskyA, HofmannFB. Die Verwerthung der Kopfneigung zur Diagnostik von Augenmuskellähmungen aus der Heber- und Senkergruppe. [The evaluation of head inclination for the diagnosis of pareses of ocular muscles from the group of elevators and depressors.]. Graefes Arch Opthalmol. 1900;51:174–185. [CrossRef]
CollinsCC. Orbital mechanics. The Control of Eye Movements. 1971;283–325.Academic Press New York.
CollinsCC, O'MearaD, ScottAB. Muscle tension during unrestrained human eye movements. J Physiol. 1975;245:351–369. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
GordonAM, HuxleyAF, JulianFJ. The variation in isometric tension with sarcomere length in vertebrate muscle fibres. J Physiol. 1966;184:170–192. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
SimonszHJ, KollingGH, van DijkB, et al. Length-tension curves of human eye muscles during succinylcholine-induced contraction. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1988;29:1320–1330. [PubMed]
SteffenH, StraumannDS, WalkerMF, et al. Torsion in patients with superior oblique palsies: dynamic torsion during saccades and changes in Listing’s plane. Graefes Arch Clin Exp Ophthalmol. 2008;246:771–778. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
StraumannD, BockischCJ, WeberKP. Dynamic aspects of trochlear nerve palsy. Prog Brain Res. 2008;171:53–58. [PubMed]
ClarkRA, MillerJM, DemerJL. Location and stability of rectus muscle pulleys: muscle paths as a function of gaze. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1997;38:227–240. [PubMed]
ClarkRA, MillerJM, DemerJL. Three-dimensional location of human rectus pulleys by path inflections in secondary gaze positions. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2000;41:3787–3797. [PubMed]
Figure 1.
 
Modulation of horizontal (AC) and torsional (DF) curvature during vertical saccades at different whole-body roll positions. Example of 10° downward saccades of a left eye at −10°, 0°, and 10° horizontal eccentricities in upright, left-ear-down, and right-ear-down positions. (AC) Eye position in the vertical-horizontal plane (projection from the subject’s perspective). Starting positions are aligned for clarity. (DF) Vertical-torsional plane. Separate saccade trajectories represent different horizontal eccentricities corresponding to panels (A) to (C). (A, D) 45° left-ear-down whole-body roll position (LED). (B, E) Upright position (up). (C, F) 45° right-ear-down whole-body roll position (RED).
Figure 1.
 
Modulation of horizontal (AC) and torsional (DF) curvature during vertical saccades at different whole-body roll positions. Example of 10° downward saccades of a left eye at −10°, 0°, and 10° horizontal eccentricities in upright, left-ear-down, and right-ear-down positions. (AC) Eye position in the vertical-horizontal plane (projection from the subject’s perspective). Starting positions are aligned for clarity. (DF) Vertical-torsional plane. Separate saccade trajectories represent different horizontal eccentricities corresponding to panels (A) to (C). (A, D) 45° left-ear-down whole-body roll position (LED). (B, E) Upright position (up). (C, F) 45° right-ear-down whole-body roll position (RED).
Figure 2.
 
Modulation of vertical (AC) and torsional (DF) curvature during horizontal saccades at different whole-body roll positions. 10° adducting rightward saccades of a viewing left eye at −10°, 0°, and 10° vertical eccentricity in upright, left-ear-down, and right-ear-down positions. (AC) Eye position in the horizontal-vertical plane (projection from the subject’s perspective). Starting positions are aligned for clarity. (DF) Horizontal-torsional plane. Separate saccade trajectories represent different vertical eccentricities corresponding to panels (A) to (C). (A, D) 45° left-ear-down whole-body roll position (LED). (B, E) Upright position (up). (C, F) 45° right-ear-down whole-body roll position (RED).
Figure 2.
 
Modulation of vertical (AC) and torsional (DF) curvature during horizontal saccades at different whole-body roll positions. 10° adducting rightward saccades of a viewing left eye at −10°, 0°, and 10° vertical eccentricity in upright, left-ear-down, and right-ear-down positions. (AC) Eye position in the horizontal-vertical plane (projection from the subject’s perspective). Starting positions are aligned for clarity. (DF) Horizontal-torsional plane. Separate saccade trajectories represent different vertical eccentricities corresponding to panels (A) to (C). (A, D) 45° left-ear-down whole-body roll position (LED). (B, E) Upright position (up). (C, F) 45° right-ear-down whole-body roll position (RED).
Figure 3.
 
Analysis of the curvature of a typical 10° downward saccade in a left eye. (A) Vertical-horizontal plane (projection from the subject’s perspective). Bold trace: trajectory from saccade onset to saccade offset. Thin trace: subsequent catch-up saccade. Dashed line: straight connection between onset and offset. Arrow: eye position at peak acceleration. (B) Traces of torsional (light gray), vertical (black), and horizontal (dark gray) rotation vector components (converted to degrees). Dashed vertical lines: saccade onset and offset. Solid vertical line: peak acceleration. (C) Traces of angular velocity vector components. (D) Trace of the derivative of angular velocity vector length.
Figure 3.
 
Analysis of the curvature of a typical 10° downward saccade in a left eye. (A) Vertical-horizontal plane (projection from the subject’s perspective). Bold trace: trajectory from saccade onset to saccade offset. Thin trace: subsequent catch-up saccade. Dashed line: straight connection between onset and offset. Arrow: eye position at peak acceleration. (B) Traces of torsional (light gray), vertical (black), and horizontal (dark gray) rotation vector components (converted to degrees). Dashed vertical lines: saccade onset and offset. Solid vertical line: peak acceleration. (C) Traces of angular velocity vector components. (D) Trace of the derivative of angular velocity vector length.
Figure 4.
 
Example of downward saccades in different whole-body roll positions. Trajectories of angular velocity vectors represent ocular rotation axes. Bold arrow: mean angular velocity vector of the entire saccade. Empty arrow: initial angular velocity vector from saccade onset to peak acceleration. Lengths of arrows are multiplied by 1.5 for clarity. (AC) Vertical-horizontal plane. (DF) Vertical-torsional plane. (A, D) 45° left-ear-down whole-body roll position (LED). (B, E) Upright position (up). (C, F) 45° right-ear-down whole-body roll position (RED).
Figure 4.
 
Example of downward saccades in different whole-body roll positions. Trajectories of angular velocity vectors represent ocular rotation axes. Bold arrow: mean angular velocity vector of the entire saccade. Empty arrow: initial angular velocity vector from saccade onset to peak acceleration. Lengths of arrows are multiplied by 1.5 for clarity. (AC) Vertical-horizontal plane. (DF) Vertical-torsional plane. (A, D) 45° left-ear-down whole-body roll position (LED). (B, E) Upright position (up). (C, F) 45° right-ear-down whole-body roll position (RED).
Figure 5.
 
Modulation of horizontal curvature of saccades made along the vertical meridian as a function of whole-body roll position. Connected symbols: mean saccade curvature in the horizontal direction of individual subjects. (A, B) 10° downward saccades. (C, D) 10° upward saccades. (A, C) Viewing left eye. (B, D) Viewing right eye. Ordinate: horizontal saccade curvature (direction indicated by the respective curved arrows). Dashed line: level of straight saccade. Abscissa: 45° left-ear-down (LED), upright (up), and 45° right-ear-down whole-body roll position (RED).
Figure 5.
 
Modulation of horizontal curvature of saccades made along the vertical meridian as a function of whole-body roll position. Connected symbols: mean saccade curvature in the horizontal direction of individual subjects. (A, B) 10° downward saccades. (C, D) 10° upward saccades. (A, C) Viewing left eye. (B, D) Viewing right eye. Ordinate: horizontal saccade curvature (direction indicated by the respective curved arrows). Dashed line: level of straight saccade. Abscissa: 45° left-ear-down (LED), upright (up), and 45° right-ear-down whole-body roll position (RED).
Figure 6.
 
Modulation of torsional curvature in saccades made along the vertical meridian as a function of whole-body roll position. Connected symbols: mean saccade curvature in the torsional direction of individual subjects. (A, B) 10° downward saccades. (C, D) 10° upward saccades. (A, C) Viewing left eye. (B, D) Viewing right eye. Ordinate: torsional saccade curvature (intor, extor: torsion of the respective eye). Dashed line: level of straight saccade. Abscissa: 45° left-ear-down (LED), upright (up), and 45° right-ear-down whole-body roll position (RED).
Figure 6.
 
Modulation of torsional curvature in saccades made along the vertical meridian as a function of whole-body roll position. Connected symbols: mean saccade curvature in the torsional direction of individual subjects. (A, B) 10° downward saccades. (C, D) 10° upward saccades. (A, C) Viewing left eye. (B, D) Viewing right eye. Ordinate: torsional saccade curvature (intor, extor: torsion of the respective eye). Dashed line: level of straight saccade. Abscissa: 45° left-ear-down (LED), upright (up), and 45° right-ear-down whole-body roll position (RED).
Figure 7.
 
Horizontal and torsional curvatures during vertical saccades as a function of whole-body roll position. (A, C) Downward saccades. (B, D) Upward saccades. (A, B) Slopes indicating modulation of horizontal curvature (first column) and torsional curvature (second column) as a function of roll position. (C, D) Offsets in horizontal curvature (first column) and torsional curvature (second column) in upright position. Open circles: average values of each eye (n = 16 eyes of 8 subjects). Open squares with error bars: mean ± 1 SD. *Mean is significantly different from zero (t-test; P < 0.01).
Figure 7.
 
Horizontal and torsional curvatures during vertical saccades as a function of whole-body roll position. (A, C) Downward saccades. (B, D) Upward saccades. (A, B) Slopes indicating modulation of horizontal curvature (first column) and torsional curvature (second column) as a function of roll position. (C, D) Offsets in horizontal curvature (first column) and torsional curvature (second column) in upright position. Open circles: average values of each eye (n = 16 eyes of 8 subjects). Open squares with error bars: mean ± 1 SD. *Mean is significantly different from zero (t-test; P < 0.01).
Figure 8.
 
Vertical and torsional curvatures during horizontal saccades as a function of whole-body roll position. (A, C) Abducting saccades. (B, D) Adducting saccades. (A, B) Slopes indicating modulation of vertical curvature (first column) and torsional curvature (second column) as a function of roll position. (C, D) Offsets in vertical curvature (first column) and torsional curvature (second column) in upright position. Open circles: average values of each eye (n = 16 eyes of 8 subjects). Open squares with error bars: mean ± 1 SD. *Mean is significantly different from zero (t-test, P < 0.01).
Figure 8.
 
Vertical and torsional curvatures during horizontal saccades as a function of whole-body roll position. (A, C) Abducting saccades. (B, D) Adducting saccades. (A, B) Slopes indicating modulation of vertical curvature (first column) and torsional curvature (second column) as a function of roll position. (C, D) Offsets in vertical curvature (first column) and torsional curvature (second column) in upright position. Open circles: average values of each eye (n = 16 eyes of 8 subjects). Open squares with error bars: mean ± 1 SD. *Mean is significantly different from zero (t-test, P < 0.01).
Figure 9.
 
Comparison of 3D video-oculography (AF) with dual-search coil recordings (same paradigm and same subject as in Fig. 1 ). (AF) 3D video-oculography of the left eye during downward saccades. (G) Horizontal curvature during downward saccades recorded with 3D video-oculography (empty triangles; data from Figs. 9A 9B 9C ) compared with dual-search coils (filled triangles; data from Figs. 1A 1B 1C ). (H) Torsional curvature in downward saccades recorded with 3D video-oculography (empty triangles; data from Figs. 9D 9E 9F ) compared with dual-search coils (filled triangles; data from Fig. 1D 1E 1F ).
Figure 9.
 
Comparison of 3D video-oculography (AF) with dual-search coil recordings (same paradigm and same subject as in Fig. 1 ). (AF) 3D video-oculography of the left eye during downward saccades. (G) Horizontal curvature during downward saccades recorded with 3D video-oculography (empty triangles; data from Figs. 9A 9B 9C ) compared with dual-search coils (filled triangles; data from Figs. 1A 1B 1C ). (H) Torsional curvature in downward saccades recorded with 3D video-oculography (empty triangles; data from Figs. 9D 9E 9F ) compared with dual-search coils (filled triangles; data from Fig. 1D 1E 1F ).
Figure 10.
 
Modulation of saccade curvature by whole-body roll position in a left eye (qualitative summary plot). Horizontal and vertical saccade curvature as seen from the subject’s perspective. Shaded gray: significant correlation between modulation of saccade curvature and OCR amplitude. ab, abduction; ad, adduction; LED, left-ear-down position; RED, right-ear-down position.
Figure 10.
 
Modulation of saccade curvature by whole-body roll position in a left eye (qualitative summary plot). Horizontal and vertical saccade curvature as seen from the subject’s perspective. Shaded gray: significant correlation between modulation of saccade curvature and OCR amplitude. ab, abduction; ad, adduction; LED, left-ear-down position; RED, right-ear-down position.
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×