Figure 3compares the performance of the control and glaucoma groups for the steady- and pulsed-pedestal paradigms. Median log luminance increment thresholds for each group are plotted as a function of the log area of an individual test square. The median was used because the results were not normally distributed. Data are presented as a function of the log area of a single square of the array because the discrimination task involved identification of the different square.
Figures 3a and 3cshow data for the steady-pedestal paradigm, and
Figures 3b and 3dshow data for the pulsed-pedestal paradigm. The upper panels
(Figs. 3a 3b)show data for stimuli viewed foveally, and the lower panels
(Figs. 3c 3d)present peripheral data. Filled symbols represent data from the control group, and open symbols represent the glaucoma group data.
Repeated-measures ANOVA (within factors: location, pedestal-paradigm, stimulus size; between factor: experimental group) of the log data showed that the thresholds for the glaucoma group were significantly elevated compared with the control group (F (1,30) = 16.29; P < 0.001). In other words, the glaucoma participants performed significantly more poorly than the controls. As noted earlier, though every attempt was made to match participants between groups by age, the age range was slightly more restricted in the control group. To determine whether the significant elevation in threshold for the glaucoma group was an age-related artifact, Spearman correlations were conducted between age and thresholds obtained for each condition. All correlations were nonsignificant, implying that the significant elevation in threshold was not age related.
Thresholds were significantly elevated when stimuli were presented peripherally (
F (1,30) = 80.14;
P < 0.001) and were significantly different for stimulus size (
F (2. 27,68 ) = 51.78;
P < 0.001) and between pedestal-paradigms (F
(1,30) = 66.66;
P < 0.001). As expected, performance was significantly reduced in peripheral vision compared with central vision. As can be observed in
Figure 3 , thresholds were dependent on the size of the stimuli and the pedestal paradigm, such that thresholds were elevated for smaller test squares and for the pulsed-pedestal paradigm.
The two-way interaction between the testing location and the group was significant (
F (1,30) = 4.48;
P < 0.05). However, the two-way interaction between the pedestal-paradigm and group was not significant (
F (1,30) = 3.65;
P = 0.07), nor was the interaction between stimulus size and group (
F (2.27,68) = 0.85;
P = 0.44). This implies that the difference between groups depended on the testing location and not the pedestal paradigm or the size of the stimulus. Inspection of
Figure 3reveals larger differences between control and glaucoma groups in the periphery than in the fovea. Potential differences between groups in the shapes of the threshold-area functions are explored.
Data for the pulsed-pedestal paradigm showed a shallow, linear decrease in log threshold with log area. However, data for the steady-pedestal paradigm showed a steeper decrease in threshold with log area for small stimulus sizes, with an asymptote for larger stimulus sizes. This is in accordance with Smith et al.,
12 who characterized this relationship using extensive data collected on trained psychophysical observers. The pulsed-pedestal data in
Figure 3were fit with a linear function, and the steady-pedestal data were fit with an exponential decay function. We explored the curve fits. As expected, the average goodness-of-fit data were always higher for the function of choice for each experimental condition in control and glaucoma groups.
The equation for these fits to the steady-pedestal data is
\[y{=}y0{+}ae^{{-}\mathrm{bx}},\]
where
y0 is the asymptotic value,
a is the value of
y minus the value of
y0 at
x = 0,
b is the exponential constant for the rate of decay, and
x is the area of the test square.
The dashed lines fit to the pulsed-pedestal data are derived from a linear function, and the equation for these fits is
where
y0 is the
y-intercept,
a is the slope of the function, and
x is the area of the test square.
Data obtained from each participant were fit with these functions. Examples for two observers in the control group are shown in
Figure 4a , and examples from the glaucoma group are shown in
Figure 4b . To determine whether there were any differences in the nature of the threshold area functions between groups, MANOVA was performed to compare the parameters that may affect the shape of the curves fit to each participant’s results for the steady- and pulsed-pedestal conditions for both central and peripheral testing. Given that our previous ANOVA results reported that luminance increment thresholds were significantly elevated for the glaucoma group, the parameters of interest for this analysis included
a (estimate of the slope) from the linear functions fit to the pulsed-pedestal data and
b (estimate of the exponential constant for the rate of decay) from the exponential decay functions fit to the steady-pedestal data. For the pulsed-pedestal linear functions, there was no significant difference for the slope of the curve between groups (
F (2,29) = 1.83;
P = 0.18), suggesting that the underlying mechanisms resulting in differences in luminance increment thresholds with increasing stimulus size for the pulsed-pedestal paradigm were not different between the control and glaucoma groups. For the steady-pedestal paradigm, there was no significant difference for the rate of decay of the exponential decay functions between the control and glaucoma groups (
F (2,29) = 1.71;
P = 1.98). These nonsignificant differences in the shape of the curves suggest that the underlying spatial summation properties are not different between groups for the steady-pedestal paradigm.