Abstract
purpose. Perceptual completion can mask the presence of physiological and pathologic retinal scotomas. This psychophysical study used a spatial alignment task to examine the processes underlying this perceptual completion. Similarities between the completion of pathologic and physiological scotomas would be consistent with large-scale reorganization of the visual system in eye disease
methods. In five control subjects with no eye disease, Vernier alignment thresholds were measured over the physiological blind spot at the optic nerve head and over equally eccentric temporal retina. For nine subjects with retinal scotomas, alignment thresholds were measured over the maximum vertical extent of the larger scotoma in one eye and at an equal separation and eccentricity in the eye with a smaller or no scotoma
results. In control subjects, alignment thresholds were better over the physiological blind spot than over equally eccentric temporal retina (P < 0.05). Alignment thresholds were no better over pathologic retinal scotomas than more intact, equally eccentric retina (P = 0.9)
conclusions. These quantitative differences implicate different mechanisms for perceptual completion over pathologic and physiological retinal scotomas. Filling in across pathologic scotomas appears to involve higher level image processing-based mechanisms that operate even when their input is interrupted. Filling-in at the optic nerve head involves additional low-level processes that may be hardwired, in which receptive fields span the blind spot and support fine orientation discriminations. These results argue against low-level reorganization of the visual system in people with retinal disease.
Retinal scotomas caused by macular disease are the leading cause of visual impairment in North America and Europe.
1 People with macular disease are frequently unaware of their scotoma; less than 60% are able to discern their scotoma on an Amsler grid chart.
2 3 A principal cause of this is thought to be the phenomenon of “perceptual completion” or “filling-in” whereby the absence of visual input is not perceived.
4 Filling-in of the physiological blind spot at the optic nerve head has been known for many years,
5 but this phenomenon can also be observed over pathologic retinal scotomas,
6 7 in scotomas caused by cortical lesions,
8 9 over simulated scotomas,
10 and over featureless areas in the peripheral visual field.
11
Filling-in of retinal scotomas is important for two reasons. First, there is considerable debate in the literature as to whether retinal scotomas lead to remapping of the primary visual cortex in humans.
12 13 If the mechanism of filling-in an adult-onset retinal scotoma is similar to that supporting filling-in of the physiological blind spot, this would provide evidence for reorganization of the visual cortex. The presence of cortical reorganization has important implications for the visual rehabilitation of people with retinal disease, particularly for the development and use of visual training programs based on the principles of perceptual learning. Second, a full understanding of how retinal scotomas fill in may lead to the development of new clinical screening tests for macular disease that are not confounded by filling-in.
14
In principle, filling-in could arise from low- or high-level visual processes and probably involves both. At a low level, filling in could arise if the receptive fields of visually responsive neurons were to remap transiently or permanently, after interruption of their retinal inputs by real or artificial scotomas. Such reorganization has not been observed in the lateral geniculate nucleus,
15 but there are many reports of receptive field reorganization in primary visual cortex.
16 17 18 19 20 Behavioral evidence for such reorganization comes from shifts in the positions of contours at the boundary of an artificial scotoma during filling in.
21 22 At higher levels of visual processing, neurons with large receptive fields that are insensitive to image projection or position have been identified in the inferotemporal cortex
23 24 25 26 and medial superior temporal sulcus.
27 28 29 30 The firing rate of many neurons at these higher levels of visual processing does not change with the position, projection, or partial occlusion of the image. The presence of a scotoma in part of the receptive field may therefore produce relatively little response change in these areas and, consequently, an unawareness of the local loss of vision, in the same way that occluded and amodally completed objects do not appear to be fragmented.
31
Vernier alignment thresholds increase with the distance between the two elements
32 because sensitivity is limited by the orientation of the smallest receptive fields that respond to the bars.
33 Therefore, this task can be used to examine visual processing around the scotoma. If filling-in involves (re)organization of small receptive fields across scotomas, this would reduce the effective distance between the lines and performance would improve.
Previous work examining a similar two-dot alignment task
34 found that Vernier alignment thresholds were approximately the same across the physiological blind spot as across the corresponding visual field location in the opposite eye not containing the optic disc. Similarly, Maertens and Pollmann
35 examined completion of the induced contours of a Kaniza square using a modified orientation discrimination task.
36 These authors also found that sensitivity to the curvature of illusory contours that passed over the physiological blind spot was worse than to contours at equivalent, sighted retinal locations. These studies argue against the presence of specialized low-level receptive field organization around the optic disc. However, the stimuli used in these studies did not directly abut the boundary of the blind spot, and it is possible that the small region of the background between the edge of the scotoma and the tips of the stimuli failed to induce filling-in or failed to stimulate small receptive fields that might span the scotoma.
Here we used stimuli that could more effectively promote filling-in to reexamine alignment across the physiological blind spot in normally sighted observers and across pathologic scotomas in people with eye disease who experience filling-in. Thresholds were compared across equivalent distances of sighted and unsighted retina in each subject.
For control subjects, the stimulus was a pair of vertical black lines, each of 2° height and 0.1° width. The lines had a center-to-center separation of 7.5° (i.e., a gap of 5.5° existed between the lines), were presented against a white background for 150 ms, and were followed by a mask of isoluminant white noise. All control subjects viewed the target with the left eye occluded (other than S5, who is amblyopic in his right eye and who performed the test with his right eye occluded). Test position (15° to the left or 15° to the right of fixation, either over the physiological blind spot or over the temporal retina) was randomly interleaved across the trial.
Figure 3shows that alignment thresholds over pathologic retinal scotomas were not significantly lower than thresholds measured across equally eccentric retina in the more healthy fellow eye. This suggests that the filling-in experienced over pathologic retinal scotomas does not include low-level receptive field reorganization but instead involves higher-level processes of image completion.
Some of our subjects described the Vernier lines as tilted, distorted, or hazy during the experiment, which may indicate metamorphopsia around the scotoma boundary (as described around the boundary of macular holes by Kroyer and colleagues
41 ). Metamorphopsia in these patients could explain their elevated Vernier alignment thresholds.
It is well known that people with macular disease often demonstrate poorer performance on tasks in apparently healthy retina well beyond the boundary of the scotoma observed on funduscopy. For example, reading speed is lower
42 and temporal processing is slower
43 than at equivalent locations in normally sighted control subjects. It may be that any advantage in alignment tasks conferred by filling-in is counteracted by preclinical changes in retinal disease beyond the region of measurable scotoma. It is also known that fixation stability is significantly worse in people with macular disease.
44 This retinal motion may affect the spatial precision of position encoding in peripheral vision, ameliorating the effects of filling-in. However, in normally sighted subjects, Vernier
45 and letter acuity
46 are invariant of very large levels of random positional jitter. Neither of these explanations can account for the higher thresholds of subjects U1 and U2 across their pathologic scotomas, both of whom have normal values of fixation stability, as determined by post hoc examination of fixation data collected on the microperimeter.
An important limitation of our experimental design is that the fundus was not imaged during the alignment task. We have assumed that the retinal location used for fixating the cross target in the alignment task is the same as that used in the microperimeter. Although it is possible that patients used different retinal loci for these two tasks, a red ellipse was presented within the area of the physiological blind spot during all trials. If fixation moved by a significant amount between these conditions, the red disc would have been visible to participants during the experiment and subjects would have reported that they had seen it. This red marker also acts as a simple fixation control: if participants had extremely poor fixation stability, the dot would be visible in many trials. Note that this cannot account for the higher thresholds in subjects U1 and U2 whose scotomas were eccentric and who fixated foveally. A further limitation of our design is that the exact location of the foveal center can be difficult to determine in the eyes with central scotomas, so a small error may exist in the presentation position of the Vernier lines in the healthier eye.
No clear relationship was found between the eccentricity of the Vernier lines and alignment threshold. Although it may be that completion acts differently in the peripheral retina from across the foveal center, our sample size is not large enough to allow a detailed analysis of the effects of retinal position on perceptual completion.
The quantitative difference between Vernier alignment thresholds of the physiological blind spot and pathologic retinal scotomas are likely to reflect structural differences between the areas surrounding the optic nerve head and areas surrounding a pathologic scotoma, either in the retina or later in the visual system. If significant cortical plasticity exists, we would expect alignment thresholds over a pathologic scotoma to be improved in a manner similar to that over a physiological scotoma. Our results argue against large-scale reorganization of visual cortex in people with eye disease.
In functional imaging studies, some of the debate over whether reorganization does
12 or does not
13 occur after development of a retinal scotoma is thought to be attributed to the type of task used and whether stimulus viewing is an active or a passive process.
47 An advantage of a psychophysical paradigm such as ours is that this distinction does not apply.
It is particularly surprising that Vernier alignment thresholds across the lifelong scotoma experienced by subjects U1 and U2 were not as low as those over physiological blind spots. In both subjects, the scotomas were in peripheral visual field locations broadly similar to the physiological blind spot (30° superior retina and approximately 15° in diameter in U1 and 22° inferior retina and approximately 6.7° in diameter in U2). Both subjects were unaware of their scotomas until they were detected during routine eye examinations with an optometrist.
Although there are not thought to be significant structural changes in visual cortex around the physiological blind spot,
48 some classical and extraclassical receptive fields are known to encompass retina from opposite sides of the optic disc.
40 49 50 We suggest that the presence and organization of this class of receptive field is the basis of the improved alignment performance over the optic disc.
Quantitative differences in filling-in of pathologic and physiological scotomas may guide the future development of tests to identify retinal disease not confounded by the presence of filling-in. For example, the Vernier targets used in this experiment were perceived as filled-in across the physiological blind spot. However, although all our subjects with retinal disease experienced completion of their scotomas on an Amsler chart, not all subjects experienced completion during our Vernier task, possibly because of the tachistoscopic presentation techniques and aperiodic stimului used here. This type of stimulus presentation could be explored further as a tool to identify the presence of retinal scotomas, perhaps by enabling patients to “see” their own areas of scotoma.
We have shown that although pathologic and physiological scotomas appear filled-in, this perceptual completion confers a spatial alignment advantage only over the physiological blind spot, suggesting that receptive fields around the optic nerve head are capable of integrating spatial structure on opposite sides of the physiological blind spot. However, after retinal insult, even if lifelong, we find no evidence that receptive fields reorganize in an equivalent manner for pathologic blind spots.
Supported by the Wellcome Trust, UK.
Submitted for publication August 8, 2008; revised November 6, 2008; accepted January 22, 2009.
Disclosure:
M.D. Crossland, None;
P.J. Bex, None
The publication costs of this article were defrayed in part by page charge payment. This article must therefore be marked “
advertisement” in accordance with 18 U.S.C. §1734 solely to indicate this fact.
Corresponding author: Michael D. Crossland, University College London Institute of Ophthalmology, 11–43 Bath Street, London EC1V 9EL, UK;
[email protected].
Table 1. Diagnosis and Scotoma Characteristics of Each Participant
Table 1. Diagnosis and Scotoma Characteristics of Each Participant
Subject | Age (years) | Diagnosis | Scotoma Size (°) | |
| | | Poorer Eye | Better Eye |
A1 | 77 | AMD | 20 | 5 |
A2 | 86 | AMD | 15 | 0 |
A3 | 86 | AMD | 25 | 10 |
A4 | 70 | AMD | 25 | 10 |
A5 | 88 | AMD | 22 | 0 |
J1 | 23 | Best disease | 15 | 3 |
J2 | 53 | Stargardt disease | 25 | 15 |
U1 | 33 | Toxoplasmosis | 15 | 0 |
U2 | 22 | Chorioretinal scar | 6.7 | 0 |
KleinR, KleinB, TomanyS, MeuerS, HuangG. Ten-year incidence and progression of age-related maculopathy: the Beaver Dam eye study. Ophthalmology. 2002;109:1767–1779.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]SchuchardRA. Validity and interpretation of Amsler grid reports. Arch Ophthalmol. 1993;111:776–780.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]CrosslandM, RubinG. The Amsler chart: absence of evidence is not evidence of absence. Br J Ophthalmol. 2007;91:391–393.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]GerritsHJ, TimmermanGJ. The filling-in process in patients with retinal scotomata. Vision Res. 1969;9:439–442.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]BrewsterD. Letters on Natural Magic, Addressed to Sir Walter Scott. 1832;John Murray London.
ZurD, UllmanS. Filling-in of retinal scotomas. Vision Res. 2003;43:971–982.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]WittichW, OverburyO, KapustaMA, WatanabeDH, FaubertJ. Macular hole: perceptual filling-in across central scotomas. Vision Res. 2006;46:4064–4070.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]SergentJ. An investigation into perceptual completion in blind areas of the visual field. Brain. 1988;111:347–373.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]DilksDD, SerencesJT, RosenauBJ, YantisS, McCloskeyM. Human adult cortical reorganization and consequent visual distortion. J Neurosci. 2007;27:9585–9594.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]RamachandranVS, GregoryRL. Perceptual filling in of artificially induced scotomas in human vision. Nature. 1991;350:699–702.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]De WeerdP, DesimoneR, UngerleiderL. Perceptual filling-in: a parametric study. Vision Res. 1998;38:2721–2734.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]BakerC, PeliE, KnoufN, KanwisherN. Reorganization of visual processing in macular degeneration. J Neurosci. 2005;25:614–618.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]SunnessJ, LiuT, YantisS. Retinotopic mapping of the visual cortex using functional magnetic resonance imaging in a patient with central scotomas from atrophic macular degeneration. Ophthalmology. 2004;111:1595–1598.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]CrosslandMD, DakinSC, BexPJ. Illusory stimuli can be used to identify retinal blind spots. PLoS ONE. 2007;2:e1060.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]EyselUT. Functional reconnections without new axonal growth in a partially denervated visual relay nucleus. Nature. 1982;299:442–444.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]ChinoYM, KaasJH, SmithEL, 3rd, LangstonAL, ChengH. Rapid reorganization of cortical maps in adult cats following restricted deafferentiation in retina. Vision Res. 1992;32:789–796.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]Darian-SmithC, GilbertCD. Topographic reorganization in the striate cortex of the adult cat and monkey is cortically mediated. J Neurosci. 1995;15:1631–1647.
[PubMed]EyselUT, SchweigartG, MittmannT, et al. Reorganization in the visual cortex after retinal and cortical damage. Restor Neurol Neurosci. 1999;15:153–164.
[PubMed]GilbertCD, WieselTN. Receptive field dynamics in adult primary visual cortex. Nature. 1992;356:150–152.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]KaasJH, KrubitzerLA, ChinoYM, LangstonAL, PolleyEH, BlairN. Reorganization of retinotopic cortical maps in adult mammals after lesions of the retina. Science. 1990;248:229–231.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]KapadiaM, GilbertC, WestheimerG. A quantitative measure for short-term cortical plasticity in human vision. J Neurosci. 1994;14:451–457.
[PubMed]TailbyC, MethaA. Artificial scotoma-induced perceptual distortions are orientation dependent and short lived. Vis Neurosci. 2004;21:79–87.
[PubMed]ToveeMJ, RollsET, AzzopardiP. Translation invariance in the responses to faces of single neurons in the temporal visual cortical areas of the alert macaque. J Neurophysiol. 1994;72:1049–1060.
[PubMed]ItoM, TamuraH, FujitaI, TanakaK. Size and position invariance of neuronal responses in monkey inferotemporal cortex. J Neurophysiol. 1995;73:218–226.
[PubMed]LogothetisNK, PaulsJ, PoggioT. Shape representation in the inferior temporal cortex of monkeys. Curr Biol. 1995;5:552–563.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]WallisG, RollsET. Invariant face and object recognition in the visual system. Prog Neurobiol. 1997;51:167–194.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]TanakaK, SaitoH. Analysis of motion of the visual field by direction, expansion/contraction, and rotation cells clustered in the dorsal part of the medial superior temporal area of the macaque monkey. J Neurophysiol. 1989;62:626–641.
[PubMed]OrbanGA, LagaeL, RaiguelS, XiaoD, MaesH. The speed tuning of medial superior temporal (MST) cell responses to optic-flow components. Perception. 1995;24:269–285.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]GrazianoMS, AndersenRA, SnowdenRJ. Tuning of MST neurons to spiral motions. J Neurosci. 1994;14:54–67.
[PubMed]DuffyCJ, WurtzRH. Sensitivity of MST neurons to optic flow stimuli, I: a continuum of response selectivity to large-field stimuli. J Neurophysiol. 1991;65:1329–1345.
[PubMed]ShimojoS, NakayamaK. Amodal representation of occluded surfaces: role of invisible stimuli in apparent motion correspondence. Perception. 1990;19:285–299.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]LeviDM, KleinSA, AitsebaomoAP. Vernier acuity, crowding and cortical magnification. Vision Res. 1985;25:963–977.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]WilsonHR. Responses of spatial mechanisms can explain hyperacuity. Vision Res. 1986;26:453–469.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]TripathyS, LeviD, OgmenH, HardenC. Perceived length across the physiological blind spot. Vis Neurosci. 1995;12:385–402.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]MaertensM, PollmannS. Illusory contours do not pass through the “blind spot.”. J Cogn Neurosci. 2007;19:91–101.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]RingachDL, ShapleyR. Spatial and temporal properties of illusory contours and amodal boundary completion. Vision Res. 1996;36:3037–3050.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]PelliDG. The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. Spat Vis. 1997;10:437–442.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]TripathyS, LeviD, OgmenH. Two-dot alignment across the physiological blind spot. Vision Res. 1996;36:1585–1596.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]MatsumotoM, KomatsuH. Neural responses in the macaque v1 to bar stimuli with various lengths presented on the blind spot. J Neurophysiol. 2005;93:2374–2387.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]KroyerK, ChristensenU, LarsenM, la CourM. Quantification of metamorphopsia in patients with macular hole. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2008;49:3741–3746.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]LeggeGE, RubinGS, PelliDG, SchleskeMM. Psychophysics of reading, II: low vision. Vision Res. 1985;25:253–265.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]CheongAM, LeggeGE, LawrenceMG, CheungSH, RuffMA. Relationship between slow visual processing and reading speed in people with macular degeneration. Vision Res. 2007;47:2943–2955.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]CrosslandMD, CulhamLE, RubinGS. Fixation stability and reading speed in patients with newly developed macular disease. Ophthal Physiol Opt. 2004;24:327–333.
[CrossRef] BadcockDR, WongTL. Resistance to positional noise in human vision. Nature. 1990;343:554–555.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]FalkenbergH, RubinG, BexP. Acuity, crowding, reading and fixation stability. Vision Res. 2007;47:126–135.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]MasudaY, DumoulinSO, NakadomariS, WandellBA. V1 projection zone signals in human macular degeneration depend on task, not stimulus. Cereb Cortex. 2008;18:2483–2493.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]AwaterH, KerlinJR, EvansKK, TongF. Cortical representation of space around the blind spot. J Neurophysiol. 2005;94:3314–3324.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]FioraniM, Jr, RosaMG, GattassR, Rocha-MirandaCE. Dynamic surrounds of receptive fields in primate striate cortex: a physiological basis for perceptual completion?. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1992;89:8547–8551.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]KomatsuH, KinoshitaM, MurakamiI. Neural responses in the retinotopic representation of the blind spot in the macaque V1 to stimuli for perceptual filling-in. J Neurosci. 2000;20:9310–9319.
[PubMed]