In this study, 2912 participants with sociodemographic, clinical, and visual functioning data were included in the analyses. Overall, the participants’ mean ± SD age was 57.5 (±10.7) years and there were marginally more men (1511; 51.9%) than women. Of the participants, 373 (12.8%) had cataract, 122 (4.2%) had diabetic retinopathy, and 26 (0.9%) age-related macular degeneration; 441 (15.1%), 213 (7.3%), 333 (11.4%), and 131 (4.5%); had corrected and uncorrected hyperopia and corrected and uncorrected myopia, respectively. There were 1792 participants considered to be emmetropic, of which 249 (8.6%) had more than a 2-line difference between presenting and corrected vision in the better eye. We labeled this group uncorrected emmetropia. The remaining 1543 (53.1%) participants had a 2-line or less difference and were labeled corrected emmetropia
(Table 1) . The mean (±SD) for spherical equivalence (SE), presenting visual acuity (PVA), corrected visual acuity (CVA), and the difference between PVA and CVA (logMAR) in the better and worse eyes are presented in
Table 1 . Those with uncorrected myopia had significantly poorer presenting and corrected VA in the better and worse eyes than participants with corrected myopia (
P < 0.001,
Table 1 ). A similar difference was found between corrected and uncorrected hyperopia, although it was only statistically significant for presenting VA (
P < 0.001).
Validation of the VF-11 in this population by Rasch analysis has been described previously.
18 34 There was evidence of disordered thresholds, which necessitated that categories 2, moderate difficulty, and 3, a little difficulty, be collapsed, and that resulted in ordered thresholds for all items. Two items—Do you have difficulty in driving during the day because of vision? and Do you have difficulty in driving at night time because of vision?—showed misfit and had to be removed. The fit statistics of the remaining nine items were found to be consistent with the Rasch model requirements. The PSR was 0.82, which indicates that the VF-11 can distinguish between several levels of person ability and has good internal reliability. There was no evidence of multidimensionality that supports the validity of the VF-11 being able to assess one underlying trait (visual functioning) that it purports to measure. Overall, the three most difficult items in the VF-9 were difficulty reading small print (1.34 logits), difficulty in filling out lottery forms (0.76 logits), and difficulty reading newspaper (0.65 logits). Conversely, the three least difficult items were associated with difficulty cooking, difficulty playing games, and difficulty seeing stairs, with logit scores of −1.56, −1.26, and −0.71, respectively. Fit of the VF-11 data to the Rasch model implies that the overall score has interval properties. Collectively, these results show that the VF-11 is a unidimensional, reliable, and valid scale to assess visual functioning in this sample.
The mean overall participants’ score on the VF-11 was 3.65 ± 0.87 logits
(Table 2) . The positive score suggests that the participants’ level of functioning was higher than the mean required level of difficulty for the items. One-way ANOVA found significant between-group effects on the overall and five individual items related to difficulty with seeing stairs, reading street signs, recognizing faces, watching TV, and filling lottery tickets
(Table 2) . Multiple comparisons showed that participants with uncorrected myopia recorded significantly worse visual functioning scores on these parameters than did those in the other five categories of refractive error (
P < 0.05).
For the overall visual functioning score, a clinically important difference was estimated at ±0.43 logit, which is approximately half the SD of the mean overall score (0.87 logit). We controlled for age, sex, educational attainment, ocular conditions, and nonocular comorbidity (stroke, heart attack, diabetes, high cholesterol, and hypertension). Considering that 8.6% of participants with emmetropia had more than a 2-line difference between presenting and corrected vision in the better eye, we considered six categories of refractive error for the regression models: 441 (15.1%), 213 (7.3%), 333 (11.4%), 131 (4.5%) 249 (8.6%), and 1545 (53.1%) for corrected hyperopia, uncorrected hyperopia, corrected myopia, uncorrected myopia, uncorrected emmetropia (>2 lines), and corrected emmetropia (≤2 lines, reference), respectively.
Only uncorrected myopia was independently associated with the overall functioning score. Compared with participants with corrected emmetropia, those with uncorrected myopia recorded significantly worse visual functioning scores overall (β regression coefficient = −0.34,
P ≤ 0.001;
Table 3 ). This result suggests that compared with persons with corrected emmetropia, those with uncorrected myopia, on average, have poorer overall QoL by 0.34 logit, and the deterioration in functioning was close to being considered clinically meaningful. Self-reported heart attack, being female, having an ocular condition, and less education were independently associated with poorer visual functioning (
P < 0.05,
Table 3 ). A similar independent association with uncorrected myopia was found for three individual items—reading street signs (β = −0.47; 95% CI: −0.62 to −0.33;
P < 0.001), recognizing friends (β = −0.52; 95% CI: −0.67 to −0.37;
P < 0.001), and watching television (β = −0.33; 95% CI: −0.44 to −0.22;
P < 0.001). These associations were all considered to be clinically meaningful. When presenting visual acuity in the better eye was introduced in the linear regression models, no category of refractive error was found to be independently associated with any aspect of visual functioning.
To validate our findings, all participants with a documented nonrefractive cause of vision impairment and nonocular comorbidities were excluded, to minimize the confounding effect of visual disability and morbidity on visual functioning. After the removal of these participants, 1112 participants remained. Overall, the participants’ mean age was 52.8 (±9.6) years, and there were marginally more women (600, 54%) than men. Of these, 119 (10.7%), 56 (5.0%), 125 (11.2%), 85 (7.8%), 641 (57.6%), and 84 (7.6%) had corrected and uncorrected myopia, corrected and uncorrected hyperopia, and corrected and uncorrected emmetropia, respectively. The independent associations found in the main sample were replicated in this subsample and were clinically meaningful for reading street signs, recognizing friends, and watching television
(Table 4) . It approached clinical significance for the overall score.