March 2011
Volume 52, Issue 3
Free
Visual Psychophysics and Physiological Optics  |   March 2011
Investigating Unstable Fixation in Patients with Macular Disease
Author Affiliations & Notes
  • Antonio F. Macedo
    From the UCL Institute of Ophthalmology, London, United Kingdom;
    University of Minho, Braga, Portugal; and
  • Michael D. Crossland
    From the UCL Institute of Ophthalmology, London, United Kingdom;
    NIHR Biomedical Research Centre for Ophthalmology, London, United Kingdom.
  • Gary S. Rubin
    From the UCL Institute of Ophthalmology, London, United Kingdom;
    NIHR Biomedical Research Centre for Ophthalmology, London, United Kingdom.
  • Corresponding author: Antonio F. Macedo, Department of Visual Neuroscience, UCL Institute of Ophthalmology, London EC1V 9EL, UK; filipe3305@gmail.com
Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science March 2011, Vol.52, 1275-1280. doi:https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.09-4334
  • Views
  • PDF
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Antonio F. Macedo, Michael D. Crossland, Gary S. Rubin; Investigating Unstable Fixation in Patients with Macular Disease . Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2011;52(3):1275-1280. https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.09-4334.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract

Purpose.: To assess the effect on visual acuity of compensating fixation instability by controlling retinal image motion in people with macular disease.

Methods.: Ten patients with macular disease participated in this study. Crowded and noncrowded visual acuity were measured using an eye tracking system to compensate for fixation instability. Four conditions, corresponding to four levels of retinal image motion, were tested: no compensation (normal motion), partial compensation (reduced motion), total compensation (no motion), and overcompensation (increased motion). Fixation stability and the number of preferred retinal loci were also measured.

Results.: Modulating retinal image motion had the same effect on crowded and noncrowded visual acuity (P = 0.601). When fixation instability was overcompensated, acuity worsened by 0.1 logMAR units (P < 0.001) compared with baseline (no compensation) and remained equal to baseline for all other conditions.

Conclusions.: In people with macular disease, retinal image motion caused by fixation instability does not reduce either crowded or noncrowded visual acuity. Acuity declines when fixation instability is overcompensated, showing limited tolerance to increased retinal image motion. The results provide evidence that fixation instability does not improve visual acuity and may be a consequence of poor oculomotor control.

Macular degeneration is the leading cause of legal blindness in Western countries. 1 3 In advanced macular disease patients change their fixation from the center to the periphery of the retina, in a process known as eccentric viewing. 4,5 The part of the peripheral retina most commonly used during visual tasks is known as the preferred retinal locus (PRL). 6 10 When using the peripheral retina, many visual tasks such as reading become very difficult or impossible. 
Difficulties faced by people with macular disease are partially explained by the poor resolution of the peripheral retina. Resolution declines in part because receptive fields of the visual responsive neurons in the periphery are wider than in the center of the retina. 11 17 However, in people with macular disease even when the visual task is scaled to account for the reduced resolution, visual performance is still reduced. 18 A further factor that might limit peripheral vision in people with central vision loss is fixation instability. 
In normal vision some amount of fixation instability is beneficial. In the central retina, tolerance of retinal image motion is relatively low: Resolution starts to decline for velocities above 2.5° s−1. 19,20 Despite this low tolerance, motion imposed by normal fixation instability enhances central vision. 21 23 In the peripheral retina fixation instability is of greater importance than in the central retina to prevent image fading. Linear velocities of up to 10° s−1 can improve resolution. 24 26  
Troxler fading exemplifies the difference between central and peripheral retina. Troxler fading refers to the fading of images presented in the peripheral retina during sustained fixation. This happens because image motion imposed by normal fixation is insufficient to prevent the decline of the response given by the peripheral visual-responsive neurons. 21,27 This finding has led to speculation that fixation instability might be beneficial for patients who must use eccentric viewing. In a study by Deruaz et al. 28 participants reported enhancement of peripheral presented targets when they voluntarily increased instability. The authors recommended caution when training fixation control in patients because instability would be part of a beneficial adaptation mechanism to prevent fading of the image in peripheral retina. Two recent studies have shown that when retinal image speed falls below a critical level at the fovea a dynamical triggering mechanism increases eye instability to avoid perceptual fading. 21,29 If a similar mechanism exists during eccentric viewing, eye instability would be beneficial, and instability would increase when retinal motion is reduced. In a previous study, with simulated scotomas, we found that instability reduced visual acuity for crowded letters but increased acuity for noncrowded letters. 30  
Evidence for a detrimental effect of fixation instability in eccentric viewing comes from several sources. For example, there is a strong correlation between instability and slower reading speeds in people with either newly 31 or well-established macular degeneration. 32  
Training to achieve better control of fixation is part of many rehabilitation programs for people with macular scotomas. Thus, an understanding of the implications of fixation instability is important. In this study we investigated the effect of fixation instability on visual acuity for crowded and noncrowded letters in patients with central scotomas caused by macular disease. The amount of retinal image motion was controlled by an eyetracker. We anticipated that acuity would improve by reducing the amount of retinal image motion caused by fixation instability, and the effect would be more pronounced for crowded letters. Additionally, if fixation instability is caused by a mechanism triggered by low retinal image motion, eye stability would change under different conditions. We measured fixation stability to explore that question. 
Methods
Participants were recruited from the Low Vision Clinic at Moorfields Eye Hospital in London. All subjects gave their informed consent to participate. The study was approved by the Moorfields & Whittington Research Ethics Committee and conformed to the tents of the Declaration of Helsinki. All participants had a long-term diagnosis of bilateral macular degeneration, and none had any secondary eye condition or neurologic disease. All participants had a central scotoma identified by microperimetry (MP-1 microperimeter; Nidek Technologies, Padova, Italy). The microperimeter was also used to determine the location of the preferred retinal locus. 
Visual acuity was measured in a crowded or noncrowded visual display (Landolt C) under different conditions of retinal image stabilization. The size of the optotype was under control of a Quest staircase, 33,34 written in a computing language programming environment (MATLAB; MathWorks, Natick, MA) using elements of commercially available toolboxes. 33,35,36 Stimuli were displayed as white targets on a black background on a 21-inch computer monitor (Trinitron GDM-F500R; Sony, Tokyo, Japan), with peak luminance of 98 cd/m2, resolution of 1280 × 1024 pixels, and 100 Hz refresh rate. The system had a Michelson contrast of 80% and was presented for 700 msec, without surrounding bars (noncrowded) and with surrounding bars (crowded). The separation between the boundaries of the device and the bars was equal to two strokes of the “C” (Fig. 1). 
Figure 1.
 
Sequence of stimuli in each trial. The optotype was preceded by a cue reducing spatial uncertainty and followed by a noise mask, visible until a response was given.
Figure 1.
 
Sequence of stimuli in each trial. The optotype was preceded by a cue reducing spatial uncertainty and followed by a noise mask, visible until a response was given.
Observers sat 50 cm from the monitor and viewed the display monocularly with their better eye. An infrared eyetracker (Eyelink I; SR Research Ltd., Mississauga, Ontario, Canada) using commercial software (Eyelink, v. 2.11) was used to record eye position and to change the level of retinal image motion. Control of retinal image motion was achieved by moving the stimulus with the eye. The position (x,y) of the eye was transferred, without filtering, from the eyetracker personal computer (PC) to a second PC displaying the stimulus. Eye velocity and acceleration were calculated using the time between samples. Before data collection, calibration (using a five-point grid) and drift correction were performed. 
Velocity of the optotype during fixations was modulated by a gain factor: gain = v target/v eye, where v target is target velocity and v eye is eye velocity. Four gains were used: gain 0 (no compensation of retinal image motion), gain 0.1 (partial compensation leading to reduced retinal image motion), gain 1 (total compensation resulting in no retinal image motion), and gain 10 (overcompensation resulting in increased retinal image motion). Gain 0 corresponds to the baseline condition; that is, the target was simply displayed in the center of the screen. A saccade was defined as an eye velocity greater than 30° s−1 and/or acceleration greater than 8500° s−2. These detection criteria allowed the target to be exposed during microsaccades, but during saccades the screen was blanked. The optotype position was updated continuously, and it reappeared when the next fixation started. 
There was an estimated 20 msec 35 delay from the eye movement to the update of the target position on the screen. That delay includes the time the samples took to travel from the eyetracker PC to the PC displaying the stimuli, plus the time of monitor refresh. This delay reduced the accuracy with which each gain could be achieved when the eye was moving. Further information on our experimental setup has been published previously. 30  
Visual acuity was measured in 16 blocks: 4 gains × 2 crowding conditions × 2 repetitions, in random order. Each block consisted of 40 trials. Before the first session of data collection, practice was provided by measuring crowded and noncrowded visual acuity for at least two different gains per visual acuity. In the remaining sessions, one block of practice with a randomly chosen gain was performed. 
Eye movements were analyzed offline to measure fixation stability. Periods when the optotype was visible were isolated from raw data; trials containing blinks or outliers (data collected outside the calibration area) were excluded from analysis. Eye positions from intersaccadic intervals were used to calculate the bivariate contour ellipse area (BCEA) in min arc 2 containing 68% of the eye positions. 8,37  
For the analysis of acuity thresholds, all visual acuities were normalized against noncrowded visual acuity for gain 0. Linear mixed models run in standard statistical software (SPSS v.14; SPSS, Chicago, IL) were used to analyze repeated measures. 
Results
Eleven subjects were recruited. Ten finished the study, and one withdrew because of the difficulties imposed by the task. Two subjects had been diagnosed with juvenile macular degeneration, and the remainder had age-related macular degeneration. The condition was bilateral in all participants, and all subjects had dense central scotomas on microperimetry, with the exception of S9, who had a relative central scotoma. Age ranged from 25 to 89 years old, and visual acuity in the better eye was between 0.7 and 1.2 logMAR. No patient had more than one PRL identified on either the eyetracker or the MP1 fixation data. A summary of the clinical characteristics of the participants is given in Table 1. Visual acuity and fixation stability were not different between the two subjects with juvenile macular disease and those with age-related macular degeneration (visual acuity, P = 0.06; fixation stability, P = 0.49). 
Table 1.
 
Characteristics of Participants in This Study
Table 1.
 
Characteristics of Participants in This Study
Subject Age (y) Diagnosis Eye Visual Acuity PRL Location
1 84 AMD L 0.7 logMAR L
2 87 AMD R 1.0 logMAR A
3 89 AMD R 1.0 logMAR L
4 72 AMD R 1.0 logMAR L
5 54 JMD L 0.7 logMAR B
6 73 AMD R 1.2 logMAR *
7 88 AMD R 1.1 logMAR B
8 89 AMD R 1.0 logMAR B
9 81 AMD R 0.4 logMAR Central
10 24 JMD L 0.8 logMAR B
Variation of Visual Acuity with Gain
Figure 2 shows visual acuity obtained for each condition. Figure 2A displays noncrowded acuity, and Figure 2B displays crowded acuity. Both crowding (P < 0.001) and gain (P < 0.001) had significant effects on visual acuity, but the interaction gain×crowding was not significant (P = 0.601). 
Figure 2.
 
Variation of noncrowded (A) and crowded (B) visual acuity with gain. Symbols show the mean for all participants for each gain as estimated by mixed models; error bars, 95% confidence interval. All acuities were normalized before statistical analysis against noncrowded acuity obtained with gain 0.
Figure 2.
 
Variation of noncrowded (A) and crowded (B) visual acuity with gain. Symbols show the mean for all participants for each gain as estimated by mixed models; error bars, 95% confidence interval. All acuities were normalized before statistical analysis against noncrowded acuity obtained with gain 0.
Noncrowded visual acuity was better than crowded acuity for all gains, with mean difference of 0.071 logMAR (P < 0.001). 
Acuity for the overcompensation condition (gain 10) was reduced compared with the no compensation condition by 0.10 logMAR (P < 0.001), for both crowded and noncrowded stimuli. These results show that reducing retinal image motion (gains 0.1 and 1) had no effect on patients' visual acuity; however, increased retinal image motion (gain 10) had a detrimental effect on acuity. 
Variation of Fixation Stability with Gain
Figure 3 shows the variation of fixation stability with gain during visual acuity measurements. Figure 3A shows noncrowded acuity, and Figure 3B shows crowded acuity. Gain had a significant effect on fixation stability (P < 0.001), but there was no effect of crowding (P = 0.23) or interaction of gain×crowding (P = 0.18). 
Figure 3.
 
Variation of fixation stability with gain during noncrowded (A) and crowded (B) visual acuity measurements. Length of the box, interquartile range (25th–75th percentiles); whiskers, 5th–95th percentiles. Inside the box, squares show the means, and the horizontal lines show the median. BCEA was calculated in min arc2 and log10 transformed before statistical analysis to approximate a normal distribution.
Figure 3.
 
Variation of fixation stability with gain during noncrowded (A) and crowded (B) visual acuity measurements. Length of the box, interquartile range (25th–75th percentiles); whiskers, 5th–95th percentiles. Inside the box, squares show the means, and the horizontal lines show the median. BCEA was calculated in min arc2 and log10 transformed before statistical analysis to approximate a normal distribution.
Fixation stability for crowded and noncrowded stimuli gain 0 (mean BCEA: 9795 min arc2) was significantly better than all other gains (mean BCEA: 21,230 min arc2, P = 0.001 for gain 0.1; mean BCEA: 21,750 min arc2; P < 0.001 for gain 1; mean BCEA: 17,780 min arc2; P = 0.014 for gain 10). There was no significant difference in fixation stability between gain 0.1, 1.0, and 10. BCEA values were in the same range as those found in previous studies (mean: 20,360 min arc2, 95% confidence interval: 10,160–30,560 min arc 2 ). 9,38 40  
Effect of Gain on Retinal Image Speed
Figure 4 shows the variation of the eye speed and retinal image speed calculated offline during a typical trial for gain 10. In this trial no saccades were detected by our criteria. Frames when the retinal image speed was very high correspond to large overshoots of the target on the monitor, which sometimes caused the perception of multiple targets. 
Figure 4.
 
Profile of the eye velocity (thick line) and retinal image velocity (thin line) of the target during a typical trial for gain 10.
Figure 4.
 
Profile of the eye velocity (thick line) and retinal image velocity (thin line) of the target during a typical trial for gain 10.
Retinal image speed was calculated for the intersaccadic periods (including microsaccades). The baseline retinal image speed (gain 0) was 8.7° s−1, which was significantly higher than the retinal image speed for gain of 0.1 (4.8° s−1) and gain 1 (3.0° s−1), but significantly lower than the retinal image speed for gain 10 (12.4° s−1). 
Discussion
In this study we measured crowded and noncrowded visual acuity while compensating for fixation instability by controlling retinal image motion in the better eye of patients with macular scotomas. The effects of controlling retinal image motion on visual acuity and on fixation stability are discussed separately. These effects are likely to be applicable when viewing binocularly because binocular oculomotor behavior is thought to be driven by the better eye. 41,42  
Effect of Gain on Visual Acuity
Compensating for fixation instability in people with macular disease failed to improve visual acuity on our task: no compensation (gain 0), partial compensation (gain 0.1) and total compensation (gain 1) all produced similar acuity. These results suggest that fixation instability of these patients does not reduce visual acuity for briefly exposed stimuli. 
These results are in agreement with studies showing no reduction or some improvement in normal peripheral visual acuity for moving targets. In these studies retinal image motion was caused by normal fixational eye movements or was increased by manipulating the target's velocity. 24,25,43 In people with macular scotomas we expected improvement of acuity when motion was compensated because retinal image motion is naturally increased due to their fixation instability. This was not the case. The lack of a difference in visual acuity across gain 0, gain 0.1, and gain 1 under both crowded and uncrowded conditions might indicate the following: 
  1.  
    Patients have adapted to the amount of retinal image motion caused by their “normal” fixation instability;
  2.  
    Independently of any adaptation, fixation instability in patients is within the tolerance of the part of the retina they use during eccentric viewing (and that might be part of the reason why that area is used as the PRL);
  3.  
    Limitations of our stabilization system reduced the size of any observed effect (see below).
Crowded visual acuity was worse than noncrowded visual acuity. The difference of 0.071 logMAR units between acuities is within values found in previous studies. In other studies involving patients, the reduction for crowded acuity varied from 0.06 logMAR units 44 to 0.11 logMAR units. 45 Other studies found higher differences, up to 0.15 logMAR units, in healthy peripheral retinas. 46  
Contrary to our findings for control subjects, 30 the effect of gain did not differ between crowded and noncrowded acuity. We know from previous studies that acuity measured with isolated letters would be minimally affected by very high levels of retinal image motion. 28,47 However, we expected higher variation between gains under crowded conditions because crowding would increase blur for high levels of retinal image motion. 20 In a study measuring peripheral acuity in healthy retina with jittering targets, Falkenberg et al. 24 failed to find any interaction between jitter and crowding. In the same study, reading speed for sequentially presented words reduced when jitter increased. That might happened because words caused increased crowding. 
Another contribution to the lack of interaction between gain and crowding is likely to be the characteristics of our participants. The group was small and heterogeneous regarding the PRL location. PRLs were in different directions relative to the fovea and at different eccentricities, and it is known that retinal location changes the effect of crowding. 48,49 The number of participants was not sufficient to test the interaction between gain, PRL eccentricity, and PRL direction. 
Effect of Gain on Fixation Stability
When retinal image motion was controlled by our technique, the magnitude of eye movements during fixation increased when compared with no compensation (gain 0). However, it remained constant under all compensation conditions (gains 0.1, 1.0, and 10). Reduction in eye stability under compensation conditions was probably caused by target's motion on the monitor. Atypical smooth pursuit can occur when the target is perceptually moving despite the retinal image speed being null, for example, when following an after-image. 50 52  
Under the three compensation conditions eye instability (BCEA) remained constant. If fixation instability was caused by a mechanism to increase retinal image motion to prevent perceptual fading, the three gains should yield variation in eye stability. Accordingly, reduced retinal motion (gain 0.1 and gain 1) would increase eye instability, and increased retinal motion (gain 10) would reduce eye instability. Based on their results, Deruaz et al. 28 argued that training fixation control, leading to a reduction in retinal image motion, might reduce patients' vision. They considered that fixation instability could be part of a mechanism to maintain the visual target moving between adjacent or separated loci in the retina. Our results show that eye instability was independent of the retinal image motion imposed by our three conditions. This argues against such a mechanism and against benefits of fixation instability for patients' vision. Recently Reinhard et al. 53 observed that patients with macular disease tend to use separated or adjacent loci during visual tasks, but they attributed that to patients' poor adaptation to the disease. Our results add evidence that fixation instability is a consequence of poor oculomotor control and not a strategy to enhance eccentric viewing. 
Limitations of the Study
Our stabilization system has limitations due to the delay between the real eye movement and the time its effect is visible in the screen. Because of this delay the mean retinal image speed for gain 0, gain 0.1, and gain 1 was always below 10° s−1, which might have reduced the difference between gains. Also, unstable fixation is likely to affect the overall effect of stabilization because relatively widely separated retinal areas might have been used for different eyetracker calibrations and/or drift corrections. This would lead to the optotype's being stabilized in the peripheral retina but not always in the same area. Finally, our sample size was too small to elicit differences between subjects with juvenile macular disease and those with age-related macular degeneration. 
Conclusions
Our study shows that compensating for fixation instability does not improve visual acuity in patients with macular disease. In other words, their typical fixation instability does not appear to be a cause of their reduced visual acuity. However, increasing instability reduces acuity for crowded and noncrowded targets. This study gives further evidence that fixation instability is a consequence of impaired oculomotor control rather than an adaptation made to improve visual function. 
Training oculomotor control can improve reading speed without a significant improvement in acuity. 54 Future work should investigate the effects of correcting fixation instability on more “real world” tasks such as reading or recognizing faces. Controlling retinal image motion might help to reduce positional uncertainty in the peripheral retina and consequently lead to a significant improvement of other visual abilities such as visual scanning. 
Footnotes
 Supported by a grant from Fundação para a Ciência e a Tecnologia (FCT), Portugal, POCTI 2010 & FSE, SRFD/BD/27975/2006 (AFM).
Footnotes
 Disclosure: A.F. Macedo, None; M.D. Crossland, None; G.S. Rubin, None
The authors would like to thank Hannah Dunbar for proofreading the manuscript and Mary Feely for her assistance during the recruitment of the participants. 
References
Klein R Klein EKB Tomany SC Ten-year incidence and progression of age-related maculopathy. Ophthalmology. 2002;109 (10):1767–1779 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Bjornsson OM Syrdalen P Bird AC The prevalence of age-related maculopathy (ARM) in an urban Norwegian population: the Oslo Macular Study. Acta Ophthalmol Scand. 2006;84 (5):636–641 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Friedman DS O'Colmain BJ Munoz B The Eye Diseases Prevalence Research: group prevalence of age-related macular degeneration in the United States. Arch Ophthalmol. 2004;122 (4):564–572 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Noorden GKV Mackensen G . Phenomenology of eccentric fixation. Am J Ophthalmol. 1962;53 (4):642–660 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Crossland MD Culham LE Kabanarou SA Rubin GS . Preferred retinal locus development in patients with macular disease. Ophthalmology. 2005;111 (9):1579–1585 [CrossRef]
Whittaker SG Budd J Cummings RW . Eccentric fixation with macular scotoma. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1988;29 (2):268–278 [PubMed]
Timberlake GT Peli E Essock EA Augliere RA . Reading with a macular scotoma. 2. Retinal locus for scanning text. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1987;28 (8):1268–1274 [PubMed]
Timberlake GT Mainster MA Peli E Reading with a macular scotoma. 1. Retinal location of scotoma and fixation area. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1986;27 (7):1137–1147 [PubMed]
Fletcher DC Schuchard RA . Preferred retinal loci relationship to macular scotomas in a low-vision population. Ophthalmology. 1997;104 (4):632–638 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Culham LE Fitzke FW Timberlake GT Marshall J . Assessment of fixation stability in normal subjects and patients using a scanning laser ophthalmoscope. Clin Vis Sci. 1993;8 (6):551–561
Hubel DH Wiesel TN . Receptive fields of optic nerve fibers in the spider monkey. J Physiol (Lond). 1960;154 (3):572–580 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Drasdo N . Receptive field densities of the ganglion cells of the human retina. Vis Res. 1989;29 (8):985–9888 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Curcio CA Sloan KR Packer O Distribution of cones in human and monkey retina: individual variability and radial asymmetry. Science. 1987;236 (4801):579–825 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Curcio CA Allen KA . Topography of ganglion cells in human retina. J. Comp Neurol. 1990;300 (1):5–25 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Anderson SJ Mullen KT Hess RF . Human peripheral spatial-resolution for achromatic and chromatic stimuli: limits imposed by optical and retinal factors. J Physiol (Lond). 1991;442:47–64 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Banks MS Sekuler AB Anderson SJ . Peripheral spatial vision: limits imposed by optics, photoreceptors, and receptor pooling. J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 1991;8 (11):1775–1787 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Green DG . Regional variations in visual acuity for interference fringes on retina. J Physiol. 1970;207 (2):351–356 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Rubin GS Turano K . Low-vision reading with sequential word presentation. Vis Res. 1994;34 (13):1723–1733 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Westheimer G McKee SP . Visual acuity in presence of retinal-image motion. J Opt Soc Am. 1975;65 (7):847–850 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Morgan MJ Benton S . Motion-deblurring in human vision. Nature. 1989;340 (6232):385–386 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Martinez-Conde S Macknik SL Troncoso XG Dyar TA . Microsaccades counteract visual fading during fixation. Neuron. 2006;49 (2):297–305 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Martinez-Conde S Macknik SL Hubel DH . The role of fixational eye movements in visual perception. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2004;5 (3):229–240 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Rucci M Iovin R Poletti M Santini F . Miniature eye movements enhance fine spatial detail. Nature. 2007;447 (7146):851–854 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Falkenberg HK Rubin GS Bex PJ . Acuity, crowding, reading and fixation stability. Vis Res. 2007;47 (1):126–135 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Brown B . Resolution thresholds for moving targets at the fovea and in the peripheral retina. Vis Res. 1972;12 (2):293–304 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Bex PJ Dakin SC Simmers AJ . The shape and size of crowding for moving targets. Vis Res. 2003;43 (27):2895–2904 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Clarke FJJ . Visual recovery following local adaptation of the peripheral retina (Troxler's effect). J Mod Opt. 1961;8 (2):121–135
Deruaz A Matter M Whatham AR Can fixation instability improve text perception during eccentric fixation in patients with central scotomas? Br J Ophthalmol. 2004;88 (4):461–463 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Engbert R Mergenthaler K . Microsaccades are triggered by low retinal image slip. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2006;103 (18):7192–7197 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Macedo AF Crossland MD Rubin GS . The effect of retinal image slip on peripheral visual acuity. J Vis. 2008;8 (14):1–11 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Crossland MD Culham LE Rubin GS . Fixation stability and reading speed in patients with newly developed macular disease. Ophthal Physiol Opt. 2004;24:327–333 [CrossRef]
Rubin GS Feely M . The role of eye movements during reading in patients with age-related macular degeneration (AMD). Neuro-Ophthalmol. 2009;33 (3)
Brainard DH . The Psychophysics Toolbox. Spatial Vis. 1997;10 (4):433–436 [CrossRef]
Watson AB Pelli DG . Quest—a Bayesian adaptative psychometric method. Percept Psychophys. 1983;33 (2):113–120 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Cornelissen FW Peters EM Palmer J . The Eyelink Toolbox: eye tracking with MATLAB and the Psychophysics Toolbox. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput. 2002;34 (4):613–617 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Pelli DG . The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. Spat Vis. 1997;10 (4):437–442 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Crossland MD Rubin GS . The use of an infrared eye tracker to measure fixation stability. Optom Vis Sci. 2002;79 (11):735–739 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Bellmann C Feely M Crossland MD Fixation stability using central and pericentral fixation targets in patients with age-related macular degeneration. Ophthalmology. 2004;111 (12):2265–2270 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Crossland MD Sims M Galbraith RF Rubin GS . Evaluation of a new quantitative technique to assess the number and extent of preferred retinal loci in macular disease. Vis Res. 2004;44 (13):1537–1546 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Macedo A Nascimento SM Gomes AO Puga A . Fixation in patients with juvenile macular disease. Optom Vis Sci. 2007;84:852–858 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Kabanarou SA Rubin GS . Reading with central scotomas: is there a binocular gain? Optom Vis Sci. 2006;83 (11):789–796 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Tarita-Nistor L Gonzalez EG Markowitz SN Steinbach MJ . Binocular interactions in patients with age-related macular degeneration: acuity summation and rivalry. Vis Res. 2006;46 (16):2487–2498 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Millodot M . Foveal and extra-foveal acuity with and without stabilized retinal images. Br J Physiol Opt. 1966;23 (2):75–106 [PubMed]
Pardhan S . Crowding in visually impaired patients: contour interaction and/or gaze-selection defects? Neuro-Ophthalmol. 1997;18 (2):59–65 [CrossRef]
Cacho I Dickinson CM Reeves BC Harper RA . Visual acuity and fixation characteristics in age-related macular degeneration. Optom Vis Sci. 2007;84 (6):487–495 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Leat SJ Li W Epp K . Crowding in central and eccentric vision: the effects of contour interaction and attention. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1999;40 (2):504–512 [PubMed]
Badcock DR Wong TL . Resistance to positional noise in human vision. Nature. 1990;343 (6258):554–555 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Toet A Levi DM . The two-dimensional shape of spatial interaction zones in the parafovea. Vis Res. 1992;32 (7):1349–1357 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Tripathy SP Cavanagh P . The extent of crowding in peripheral vision does not scale with target size. Vis Res. 2002;42 (20):2357–2369 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Dubois MFW Collewijn H . Optokinetic reactions in man elicited by localized retinal motion stimuli. Vis Res. 1979;19:1105–1115 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Cushman WB Tangney JF Steinman RM Ferguson JL . Characteristics of smooth eye-movements with stabilized targets. Vis Res. 1984;24 (9):1003–1109 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Wyatt HJ Pola J . Role of perceived motion in smooth pursuit eye-movements. Vis Res. 1979;19 (6):613–618 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Reinhard J Messias A Dietz K Quantifying fixation in patients with Stargardt disease. Vis Res. 2007;47 (15):2076–2085 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Seiple W Szlyk JP McMahon T Eye-movement training for reading in patients with age-related macular degeneration. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2005;46 (8):2886–2896 [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1.
 
Sequence of stimuli in each trial. The optotype was preceded by a cue reducing spatial uncertainty and followed by a noise mask, visible until a response was given.
Figure 1.
 
Sequence of stimuli in each trial. The optotype was preceded by a cue reducing spatial uncertainty and followed by a noise mask, visible until a response was given.
Figure 2.
 
Variation of noncrowded (A) and crowded (B) visual acuity with gain. Symbols show the mean for all participants for each gain as estimated by mixed models; error bars, 95% confidence interval. All acuities were normalized before statistical analysis against noncrowded acuity obtained with gain 0.
Figure 2.
 
Variation of noncrowded (A) and crowded (B) visual acuity with gain. Symbols show the mean for all participants for each gain as estimated by mixed models; error bars, 95% confidence interval. All acuities were normalized before statistical analysis against noncrowded acuity obtained with gain 0.
Figure 3.
 
Variation of fixation stability with gain during noncrowded (A) and crowded (B) visual acuity measurements. Length of the box, interquartile range (25th–75th percentiles); whiskers, 5th–95th percentiles. Inside the box, squares show the means, and the horizontal lines show the median. BCEA was calculated in min arc2 and log10 transformed before statistical analysis to approximate a normal distribution.
Figure 3.
 
Variation of fixation stability with gain during noncrowded (A) and crowded (B) visual acuity measurements. Length of the box, interquartile range (25th–75th percentiles); whiskers, 5th–95th percentiles. Inside the box, squares show the means, and the horizontal lines show the median. BCEA was calculated in min arc2 and log10 transformed before statistical analysis to approximate a normal distribution.
Figure 4.
 
Profile of the eye velocity (thick line) and retinal image velocity (thin line) of the target during a typical trial for gain 10.
Figure 4.
 
Profile of the eye velocity (thick line) and retinal image velocity (thin line) of the target during a typical trial for gain 10.
Table 1.
 
Characteristics of Participants in This Study
Table 1.
 
Characteristics of Participants in This Study
Subject Age (y) Diagnosis Eye Visual Acuity PRL Location
1 84 AMD L 0.7 logMAR L
2 87 AMD R 1.0 logMAR A
3 89 AMD R 1.0 logMAR L
4 72 AMD R 1.0 logMAR L
5 54 JMD L 0.7 logMAR B
6 73 AMD R 1.2 logMAR *
7 88 AMD R 1.1 logMAR B
8 89 AMD R 1.0 logMAR B
9 81 AMD R 0.4 logMAR Central
10 24 JMD L 0.8 logMAR B
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×