Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science Cover Image for Volume 51, Issue 11
November 2010
Volume 51, Issue 11
Free
Cornea  |   November 2010
Interaction of Corneal Nociceptive Stimulation and Lacrimal Secretion
Author Affiliations & Notes
  • Ping Situ
    From the Centre for Contact Lens Research, School of Optometry, and
  • Trefford L. Simpson
    the School of Optometry, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada.
  • Corresponding author: Trefford L. Simpson, School of Optometry, University of Waterloo, 200 University Avenue West, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada N2L 3G1; [email protected]
Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science November 2010, Vol.51, 5640-5645. doi:https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.10-5502
  • Views
  • PDF
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Ping Situ, Trefford L. Simpson; Interaction of Corneal Nociceptive Stimulation and Lacrimal Secretion. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2010;51(11):5640-5645. https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.10-5502.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract

Purpose.: To investigate the interaction between corneal stimuli at different positions and tear secretion and to establish relationships between nociceptive stimuli detection thresholds and stimulated tearing.

Methods.: Using a computerized Belmonte-esthesiometer, mechanical and chemical stimuli, from 0% to 200% of the threshold in 50% steps, were delivered (in random order) to the central and peripheral (approximately 2-mm inside the limbus) cornea during four separate sessions to 15 subjects. Immediately after each stimulus, tear meniscus height (TMH) was measured using optical coherence tomography to quantify the amount of lacrimal secretion, and subjects reported whether they felt tears starting to accumulate in their eyes. Thresholds (50% detection) for detection of tearing were estimated.

Results.: TMH increased with increasing stimulus intensity (P < 0.05), and the overall increase was higher with central stimulation than with peripheral stimulation (P < 0.05). The changes in TMH with threshold-scaled stimulus intensity depended on test location (P < 0.05) and stimulus modality (P < 0.05). The maximum intensity of mechanical stimulation of the central cornea induced the greatest TMH (all P < 0.05). For chemical stimulation, the stimulus intensity required to induce detectable tearing was higher than that required to detect a stimulus and higher in the periphery than at the center (all P < 0.05).

Conclusions.: Noxious mechanical and chemical stimuli evoked measurable tear secretion, with central corneal mechanical stimulation evoking the strongest lacrimation reflex. Central mechanical corneal stimulation is the most effective stimulus-position pairing and appears to be the major sensory driving force for reflex tear secretion by the lacrimal functional unit.

Tear secretion is controlled by the integrated lacrimal functional unit that comprises the ocular surface (cornea, conjunctiva, and meibomian glands), the lacrimal glands, and the sensory afferent and autonomic efferent nerves that connect them. 1 3 The functional unit regulates the major components of the tear film 1,4 and responds to environmental, endocrine, and central neural influences, 2,3,5,6 with overall function to protect the integrity of the tear film and the ocular surface and to maintain the quality of the principal optical component of the eye. 7  
Sensory signals arising from the ocular surface are an important aspect of the lacrimal functional unit. 7 It has been considered that waking tear production is driven by a reflex circuit consisting of the sensory afferents coming mainly from the ocular surface, the synaptic integration at the spinal trigeminal nucleus (Vsp) and a relay to the superior salivatory nucleus located in the brain stem, and the efferent fibers to parasympathetic and sympathetic ganglia and then to the lacrimal glands and other ocular surface tissue. 2,6,8,9 During the steady state, without injury or pathology, the sensory nerves of the ocular surface provide low-level input to the functional unit and operate with the efferent parasympathetic and sympathetic nerves to modulate resting tear flow. 5,7 Stimulation of sensory nerves near or above injurious levels results in tear secretion and other reflexes to protect the eye from potential damage. 10  
The cornea is richly innervated by sensory nerves that serve important sensory and reflex functions. 11,12 Corneal sensory nerves predominantly originate from neurons located in the ipsilateral trigeminal ganglion and can be classified as thin myelinated (Aδ type) and unmyelinated (C type) fibers. 10 The peripheral axons of the neurons terminate throughout the corneal epithelium as so-called free nerve endings, 12 15 though encapsulated neural terminals have been observed in the limbus and perilimbal bulbar conjunctiva. 16 Despite the lack of morphologic specialization at the endings, different functional sensory fibers, including polymodal nociceptors, mechanonociceptors, and cold receptors, have been identified based on their electrophysiological properties. 17,18 In addition, a small number of low-threshold mechanical receptors have been found in the limbus. 17 Recently, a study has proposed that the afferent pathways of basal and reflex tearing might involve a different subclass of sensory receptors. 19  
Although the peripheral and central neural pathways for the lacrimal reflex induced by corneal stimulation have been partially elucidated, the relationship between the intensity of the sensory input and the outflow of the efferent autonomic nervous system in the lacrimal functional unit has not been defined. It is unclear whether tear secretion induced by stimulation at the central and peripheral cornea is similar, though it has been suggested that the corneal distribution of a subclass of functional types of receptors might be similar. 20  
In the present study we investigated the interaction between stimulation at various corneal positions and the efferent output, as determined by tear secretion, and defined the psychophysiological function as the relationship between stimulus intensity and stimulated tearing. Systematically varied intensities of mechanical and chemical stimuli were delivered to different positions of the cornea using a Belmonte pneumatic esthesiometer. Tear volume expressed by tear meniscus height (TMH) was quantified using optical coherence tomography (OCT), 21 and perceived lacrimation was quantified after each stimulus. 
Subjects, Materials, and Methods
Subjects
Fifteen healthy subjects (nine women, six men; age range, 18–47 years; mean ± SD, 31.6 ± 7.9) who did not wear contact lenses and had no history of ocular or systemic diseases or surgery participated in the study. 
This study was conducted in accordance with the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and received clearance from the University of Waterloo, Office of Research Ethics (Waterloo, Ontario, Canada). Informed consent was obtained from each subject. 
Mechanical and Chemical Stimulation
A computer-controlled Belmonte esthesiometer, described in detail elsewhere, 20,22,23 was used to deliver mechanical and chemical stimuli to the central and temporal peripheral cornea (approximately 2 mm from limbus) of the left eye. 
Mechanical stimuli consisted of a series of air pulses with flow rate varying from 0 to 200 mL/min. Chemical stimulation was induced by increasing the concentration of CO2 in the air with the flow rate well below the mechanical threshold. The temperature of the stimulus was set at 50°C, which dropped to approximately 33°C at the ocular surface. 22 24  
At the initial assessment, subjects underwent a training session using stimulation of the central cornea of the right eye. Initial detection thresholds to mechanical and chemical stimuli at both test locations were then estimated using the ascending method of limits (the average of three first reports of stimulus presence) to define the stimulus range. Mechanical and chemical stimuli were set at five levels, ranging from 0% to 200% of the initial detection threshold in 50% steps. Measurements were made during four separate sessions at approximately the same time of day. All measurements were conducted at least 4 hours after subjects awoke to minimize the influence of diurnal effects. 25 The order of the test location and the stimulus modality was randomly assigned, and, in each session, the intensity of the stimulus was presented in random order, with a minimum interstimulus interval of 2 minutes; each stimulus was delivered three times. 
Tear Meniscus Height Measurement
Before each measurement session and immediately after each stimulus, TMH 21 at the mid-lower lid margin was measured with an optical coherence tomographer (model 2010; Zeiss Humphrey System, Dublin, CA) to quantify the amount of lacrimal secretion. Custom software was used to measure TMH of the OCT images. 
Estimation of Detectable Tearing Thresholds
Subjects reported (yes or no) the presence of detectable tearing using a button box. Tearing threshold (midpoint of intensity detectable tearing), psychometric function, and slope were estimated using nonlinear regression fits of the logistic function: Likelihood of detection = 1 − [1/(1 + (stimulus intensity/threshold)slope)]. 
Statistical Analysis
Statistical analyses were performed (Statistica 8.0; StatSoft Inc., Tulsa, OK), with P ≤ 0.05 considered statistically significant. Repeated-measures ANOVA and post hoc Tukey HSD tests were used to compare the differences in the TMH measurements between intensity levels, stimulus positions and modalities, and their interactions. The detectable tearing thresholds between locations for each modality and their interactions with the sensory thresholds were also compared. 
Results
Effects of Stimulus Intensity, Location, and Modality on TMH
Measurements of TMH at baseline (before each measurement session) and with mechanical and chemical stimulation of differing intensities stratified by modality and location are shown in Figure 1. There was a significant intensity effect regardless of stimulus modality and location, as shown in Figure 2 (ANOVA P < 0.001). TMH increased as a function of increased stimulus intensity. At 200% (2×) threshold, TMH was significantly higher than all stimulus intensities except for 150% (1.5×) threshold (Tukey HSD, all P < 0.05). TMH without stimulation (0% threshold) was significantly lower than any given stimulus intensity (Tukey HSD, all P < 0.05). 
Figure 1.
 
TMH at baseline (unstimulated condition) and with noxious stimulation. The gray dashed lines indicate the 95% confidence interval (CI) of unstimulated TMH. The horizontal axis is stimulus intensity, with the first gray value representing baseline TMH measured prior to any stimulus being present. The other numbers on the x-axis are multiples of stimulus intensity at detection threshold: 0 represents the catch trials when the esthesiometer intensity setting was zero, 0.5 was when intensity was half threshold, 1 was when intensity was the same as threshold, 1.5 was when intensity was 50% higher than threshold and 2 was when intensity was double threshold. Mech, mechanical stimulus; Chem, chemical stimulus; C, central; P, peripheral.
Figure 1.
 
TMH at baseline (unstimulated condition) and with noxious stimulation. The gray dashed lines indicate the 95% confidence interval (CI) of unstimulated TMH. The horizontal axis is stimulus intensity, with the first gray value representing baseline TMH measured prior to any stimulus being present. The other numbers on the x-axis are multiples of stimulus intensity at detection threshold: 0 represents the catch trials when the esthesiometer intensity setting was zero, 0.5 was when intensity was half threshold, 1 was when intensity was the same as threshold, 1.5 was when intensity was 50% higher than threshold and 2 was when intensity was double threshold. Mech, mechanical stimulus; Chem, chemical stimulus; C, central; P, peripheral.
Figure 2.
 
TMH (average of stimulus modalities and locations) versus stimulus intensity. Vertical bars: 95% confidence intervals. *Significant differences compared with 0 stimulus intensity (P < 0.05).
Figure 2.
 
TMH (average of stimulus modalities and locations) versus stimulus intensity. Vertical bars: 95% confidence intervals. *Significant differences compared with 0 stimulus intensity (P < 0.05).
There was a significant location effect on TMH independent of stimulus modality (ANOVA, P < 0.001), with higher TMH for central stimulation. The overall increased TMH was similar between mechanical and chemical stimulation regardless of the stimulus locations (ANOVA, P > 0.05). 
Interactions between Stimulus Intensity, Location, and Modality
Significant two-way interactions were found between intensity and location, and intensity and modality, as illustrated in Figures 3 and 4 (ANOVA, P = 0.001 and P < 0.001 for location and modality, respectively). Independent of stimulus modality, intensity at and above the threshold resulted in greater TMH for central stimulation than for peripheral stimulation (Tukey HSD, all P < 0.05). On the other hand, TMH induced by different modalities, regardless of location, were similar at most of the intensity levels except for the highest level (Tukey HSD, P = 0.001). Although the overall three-way interaction between stimulus intensity, location, and modality was not significant (ANOVA, P = 0.189), post hoc pairwise comparison showed that the significant difference between modalities was at the highest suprathreshold level for central stimulation only (Tukey HSD, P = 0.003), with the greatest TMH induced by the maximum intensity of mechanical stimulus (Fig. 5). 
Figure 3.
 
Interaction of TMH measurement and stimulus intensity regardless of stimulus modality at both positions. Vertical bars: 95% confidence intervals. *Significant differences compared with corresponding peripheral stimulation (P < 0.05).
Figure 3.
 
Interaction of TMH measurement and stimulus intensity regardless of stimulus modality at both positions. Vertical bars: 95% confidence intervals. *Significant differences compared with corresponding peripheral stimulation (P < 0.05).
Figure 4.
 
Interaction of TMH measurement and stimulus intensity regardless of stimulus position for both stimulus modalities. Vertical bars: 95% confidence intervals. *Significant difference compared with chemical stimulation (P = 0.001).
Figure 4.
 
Interaction of TMH measurement and stimulus intensity regardless of stimulus position for both stimulus modalities. Vertical bars: 95% confidence intervals. *Significant difference compared with chemical stimulation (P = 0.001).
Figure 5.
 
Interaction of TMH measurement and stimulus intensity for both stimulus modalities and position. Vertical bars: 95% confidence intervals. *Significant difference compared with the maximum intensity of chemical stimulation (P = 0.003).
Figure 5.
 
Interaction of TMH measurement and stimulus intensity for both stimulus modalities and position. Vertical bars: 95% confidence intervals. *Significant difference compared with the maximum intensity of chemical stimulation (P = 0.003).
Effect of Stimulus Location on Tearing Thresholds and Relation to Stimulus Detection
Thresholds to mechanical and chemical stimuli and the tearing thresholds at each location in response to different modalities are presented in Table 1. Generally, thresholds to detect mechanical stimulation and detectable tearing were similar across locations, and there was no significant interaction between threshold and location (ANOVA, both P > 0.05). A significant difference was found between detectable tearing and detection threshold to the chemical stimulation; tearing thresholds were higher than detection thresholds (ANOVA, P = 0.043), and this difference was dependent on the stimulus location (ANOVA, P = 0.011). Post hoc pairwise comparison showed that detectable tearing thresholds were higher at the periphery than at the center and higher than the threshold for detecting a stimulus at each corresponding location (Tukey HSD, all P ≤ 0.028). 
Table 1.
 
Thresholds of Detectable Tearing and Stimulus Detection
Table 1.
 
Thresholds of Detectable Tearing and Stimulus Detection
Threshold Mechanical (mL/min) Chemical (% CO2 added)
Central Peripheral Central Peripheral
Tearing 60.12 ± 11.09 65.25 ± 11.94 30.05 ± 6.61 38.95 ± 6.06
Stimulus detection 54.36 ± 5.34 51.60 ± 6.01 21.84 ± 2.27 20.27 ± 2.29
Discussion
The primary objective of this study was to investigate the relationship between stimulation of corneal sensory nerves and efferent output of the lacrimal functional unit determined by tear secretion. Tear secretion increased almost linearly with the increase of stimulus intensity. 
It has been suggested that the sensory effects and, hence, the lacrimal functional unit operate differently, depending on environmental conditions and pathology. 5 Without stressful stimulation, sensory nerves on the ocular surface provide subthreshold sensory input to the functional unit modulating resting tear flow. When noxious stimuli activate sensory afferents in the functional unit, a series of coordinated reflexes, including reflex tearing, are triggered to protect the eye from potential damage. The present study showed that suprathreshold nociceptive corneal stimuli evoke apparently reflex lacrimation, accompanied by the perception of increasing tear flow in the eye, whereas tear secretion after subthreshold stimuli was the same as unstimulated tear flow (Fig. 1). This physiological evidence supports the hypothesis that two states of a neural control mechanism (i.e., basal-subconscious and augmented-conscious) are involved in regulating tear secretion to protect the ocular surface from injury over a wide range of situations. 5  
Stimuli to the cornea are detected and encoded by the sensory receptors or transducers on the peripheral terminals of the corneal fibers. Corneal sensory fibers have different functional types of receptors that are preferentially activated by different types of stimulation. 17,18 In cats, most of the corneal fibers (approximately 70%) are polymodal nociceptors that are equally activated by near-noxious mechanical energy, chemical irritants, heat (higher than 39°C), and noxious cold. 17 Most of the polymodal nociceptor fibers are unmyelinated C type, but some belong to the group of thin myelinated Aδ fibers. 17 Approximately 15% to 20% of the corneal fibers are mechanonociceptors that are fast-conducting, thin myelinated Aδ fibers and are activated exclusively by intense mechanical force. 17 In the present study, mechanical and chemical stimulation generally produced similar effects on reflex tear secretion, consistent with the findings of Acosta et al., 19 although they used different methods to quantify tear volume. Studies in cats have shown that pneumatic mechanical stimuli to the cornea activated mainly the phasic mechanonociceptors and polymodal nociceptors, whereas gas mixtures of increasing CO2 primarily excited polymodal nociceptors. 26,27 This similarity between modalities suggests that both polymodal and mechanonociceptors contribute to the afferent pathways of reflex tear secretion. The highest suprathreshold mechanical stimulation in our experiment might have activated not only the polymodal nociceptors but also the high threshold mechanonociceptors, resulting in neural summation and thus producing greater tear reflex than the equivalent intensity of chemical stimulation. 
The neural activities encoded by sensory receptors are carried centripetally by trigeminal ganglion neurons to higher levels in the central nervous system. The ocular surface is represented mainly in two spatially distinct regions of the spinal trigeminal nucleus in the lower brain stem: the trigeminal nucleus interpolaris-caudalis (Vi/Vc) transition and the subnucleus caudalis-upper cervical spinal cord (Vc/C1) junction. 28 30 Because of this unique dual representation, it has been proposed that neurons at Vi/Vc and Vc/C1 transition regions mediate different aspects of corneal nociception and that their efferent projection to supraspinal areas might also be different. 31,32 The corneal neurons located at the Vi/Vc transition region that project to the superior salivatory nucleus serve ocular-specific functions such as blink and tear reflexes, whereas those located within the superficial laminae (I-II) of the Vc/C1 transition that project to the posterior thalamic nucleus may play a prominent role in the sensory-discriminative aspects of corneal nociception. 33 35 The present study showed that at and above threshold, stimulation of the central cornea produced greater reflex tearing than the equivalent stimulus to the periphery, suggesting that reflex lacrimation responses vary depending on the stimulus location. Given that suprathreshold stimulation may damage the cornea and that the ultimate goal of corneal reflexes is to protect the ocular surface and, therefore, the eye itself, it is plausible that the circuitry involved in reflex responses to noxious stimulation of the two locations may be different (e.g., efferent projection to the superior salivatory nucleus vs. the posterior thalamic nucleus) because the visual axis is situated within the central cornea. 
It has been reported that a small number of thick, fast-conducting nerve fibers innervate the perilimbal episclera. 17 These fibers possibly terminate at the limbal region with Krause-like corpuscular endings, as described by Lawrenson and Ruskell, 16 and respond to gentle mechanical stimulation of the ocular surface (they are mechanoreceptors). 10 Activation of this type of receptor has been reported to be less effective than activation by polymodal nociceptors in evoking the tearing reflex. 19 This may also contribute to the location differences in the study. 
In addition, although the density of corneal innervation in humans seems to be more uniform 36 than previously believed based on evidence from animal histology, 12 it is possible that there is a reduction in density in the periphery that is not clearly revealed by the current methods (e.g., if the periphery is not fully explored from limbus to limbus). The result in Figure 3 might simply reflect this higher sensory fiber packing in the central cornea that could lead to stronger activation of neurons with nociceptive central stimulation compared with the periphery. On the other hand, this explanation is untenable in light of the interaction between position and stimulus type (Fig. 4). This is more complex than a simple distinction between central and peripheral processing (or density differences) because the difference between center and periphery also depends on stimulus type. 
As expected, the intensity required to trigger detectable tearing was generally higher than that required to detect a noxious stimulus, though for mechanical stimulation the differences between the two thresholds did not reach statistical significance. Additionally, as in previous studies, 20,24,37 we did not find a strong positional effect on thresholds to detect mechanical and chemical stimulation. However, for suprathreshold chemical stimulation, the intensity required to induce subjectively detectable tearing was higher for the peripheral cornea than for the central cornea, consistent with the difference in amount of tear secretion between the two locations. It appears that chemical stimulation of the central and peripheral cornea is similar for mediating corneal sensation at the threshold level, but it is different for detecting stimulated tearing. This suggests that chemosensory information from the central cornea and the peripheral cornea may be processed differently at the spinal trigeminal nucleus, depending on the sensory-discriminative or tear reflex aspect. 
In conclusion, the present study demonstrates that a systematic increase in tear volume as determined by TMH is monotonically related to the intensity of the sensory input from the cornea in a dose-response manner. This provides physiological evidence that sensory innervation of the cornea (thus, the ocular surface) is the major neural driving force for lacrimal gland secretion. Acting through areas of the central nervous system, the sensory nerves and efferent parasympathetic and sympathetic nerves of the lacrimal functional unit modulate tear secretion to ensure a healthy ocular surface and to protect the eye under normal as well as environmentally challenging conditions (such as during this experiment). 
The components of the lacrimal functional unit are linked in a homeostatic loop by complex and precise sensory, parasympathetic, and sympathetic neural control. 3 Establishing the relation between activation of the sensory nerves from the ocular surface and the graded output of the lacrimal gland secretion may enable further understanding of the neural mechanisms contributing to the development of dry eye and ultimately to the development of effective treatments for ocular surface diseases in which the functioning of the innervation of the lacrimal unit may be compromised. 
Footnotes
 Supported by operating and equipment grants from NSERC Canada and CFI, respectively.
Footnotes
 Disclosure: P. Situ, None; T.L. Simpson, None
The authors thank all the subjects who participated in the study and Desmond Fonn for valuable comments on the manuscript. 
References
Dartt DA . Neural regulation of lacrimal gland secretory processes: relevance in dry eye diseases. Prog Retin Eye Res. 2009;28:155–177. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Stern ME Beuerman RW Fox RI Gao J Mircheff AK Pflugfelder SC . The pathology of dry eye: the interaction between the ocular surface and lacrimal glands. Cornea. 1998;17:584–589. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Stern ME Gao J Siemasko KF Beuerman RW Pflugfelder SC . The role of the lacrimal functional unit in the pathophysiology of dry eye. Exp Eye Res. 2004;78:409–416. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Dartt DA . Regulation of mucin and fluid secretion by conjunctival epithelial cells. Prog Retin Eye Res. 2002;21:555–576. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Beuerman RW Mircheff A Pflugfelder SC Stern ME . The lacrimal functional unit. In: Pflugfelder SC Beuerman RW Stern ME eds. Dry Eye and Ocular Surface Disorders. New York: Marcel Dekker, Inc.; 2004:11–39.
Mathers WD . Why the eye becomes dry: a cornea and lacrimal gland feedback model. CLAO J. 2000;26:159–165. [PubMed]
Lemp MA Baudouin C Baum J . The definition and classification of dry eye disease: report of the Definition and Classification Subcommittee of the International Dry Eye WorkShop. Ocul Surf. 2007;5:75–92. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Pflugfelder SC Solomon A Stern ME . The diagnosis and management of dry eye: a twenty-five-year review. Cornea. 2000;19:644–649. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Walcott B . The lacrimal gland and its veil of tears. News Physiol Sci. 1998;13:97–103. [PubMed]
Belmonte C Garcia-Hirschfeld J Gallar J . Neurobiology of ocular pain. Prog Retin Eye Res. 1997;16:117–156. [CrossRef]
Muller LJ Marfurt CF Kruse F Tervo TM . Corneal nerves: structure, contents and function. Exp Eye Res. 2003;76:521–542. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Rozsa AJ Beuerman RW . Density and organization of free nerve endings in the corneal epithelium of the rabbit. Pain. 1982;14:105–120. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Chan-Ling T . Sensitivity and neural organization of the cat cornea. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1989;30:1075–1082. [PubMed]
Marfurt CF Murphy CJ Florczak JL . Morphology and neurochemistry of canine corneal innervation. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2001;42:2242–2251. [PubMed]
Muller LJ Vrensen GF Pels L Cardozo BN Willekens B . Architecture of human corneal nerves. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1997;38:985–994. [PubMed]
Lawrenson JG Ruskell GL . The structure of corpuscular nerve endings in the limbal conjunctiva of the human eye. J Anat. 1991;177:75–84. [PubMed]
Belmonte C Acosta MC Gallar J . Neural basis of sensation in intact and injured corneas. Exp Eye Res. 2004;78:513–525. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
MacIver MB Tanelian DL . Structural and functional specialization of A delta and C fiber free nerve endings innervating rabbit corneal epithelium. J Neurosci. 1993;13:4511–4524. [PubMed]
Acosta MC Peral A Luna C Pintor J Belmonte C Gallar J . Tear secretion induced by selective stimulation of corneal and conjunctival sensory nerve fibers. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2004;45:2333–2336. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Situ P Simpson TL Fonn D . Eccentric variation of corneal sensitivity to pneumatic stimulation at different temperatures and with CO2 . Exp Eye Res. 2007;85:400–405. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Bitton E Keech A Simpson T Jones L . Variability of the analysis of the tear meniscus height by optical coherence tomography. Optom Vis Sci. 2007;84:903–908. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Belmonte C Acosta MC Schmelz M Gallar J . Measurement of corneal sensitivity to mechanical and chemical stimulation with a CO2 esthesiometer. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1999;40:513–519. [PubMed]
Feng Y Simpson TL . Nociceptive sensation and sensitivity evoked from human cornea and conjunctiva stimulated by CO2 . Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2003;44:529–532. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Stapleton F Tan M Papas E . Corneal and conjunctival sensitivity to air stimuli. Br J Ophthalmol. 2004;88:1547–1551. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
du Toit R Vega JA Fonn D Simpson T . Diurnal variation of corneal sensitivity and thickness. Cornea. 2003;22:205–209. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Acosta MC Belmonte C Gallar J . Sensory experiences in humans and single-unit activity in cats evoked by polymodal stimulation of the cornea. J Physiol. 2001;534:511–525. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Chen X Gallar J Pozo MA Baeza M Belmonte C . CO2 stimulation of the cornea: a comparison between human sensation and nerve activity in polymodal nociceptive afferents of the cat. Eur J Neurosci. 1995;7:1154–1163. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Belmonte C Tervo T . Pain in and around the eye. In: McMahon S Koltzenburg M eds. Wall and Melzack's Textbook of Pain. 5th ed. London: Elsevier Science; 2005:887–901.
Marfurt CF Del Toro DR . Corneal sensory pathway in the rat: a horseradish peroxidase tracing study. J Comp Neurol. 1987;261:450–459. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Meng ID Bereiter DA . Differential distribution of Fos-like immunoreactivity in the spinal trigeminal nucleus after noxious and innocuous thermal and chemical stimulation of rat cornea. Neuroscience. 1996;72:243–254. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Bereiter DA Hirata H Hu JW . Trigeminal subnucleus caudalis: beyond homologies with the spinal dorsal horn. Pain. 2000;88:221–224. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Hirata H Okamoto K Bereiter DA . GABA(A) receptor activation modulates corneal unit activity in rostral and caudal portions of trigeminal subnucleus caudalis. J Neurophysiol. 2003;90:2837–2849. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Hirata H Takeshita S Hu JW Bereiter DA . Cornea-responsive medullary dorsal horn neurons: modulation by local opioids and projections to thalamus and brain stem. J Neurophysiol. 2000;84:1050–1061. [PubMed]
Meng ID Hu JW Bereiter DA . Differential effects of morphine on corneal-responsive neurons in rostral versus caudal regions of spinal trigeminal nucleus in the rat. J Neurophysiol. 1998;79:2593–2602. [PubMed]
Hirata H Okamoto K Tashiro A Bereiter DA . A novel class of neurons at the trigeminal subnucleus interpolaris/caudalis transition region monitors ocular surface fluid status and modulates tear production. J Neurosci. 2004;24:4224–4232. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Marfurt CF Cox J Deek S Dvorscak L . Anatomy of the human corneal innervation. Exp Eye Res. 2010;90:478–492. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Murphy PJ Armstrong E Woods LA . A comparison of corneal and conjunctival sensitivity to a thermally cooling stimulus. Adv Exp Med Biol. 2002;506:719–722. [PubMed]
Figure 1.
 
TMH at baseline (unstimulated condition) and with noxious stimulation. The gray dashed lines indicate the 95% confidence interval (CI) of unstimulated TMH. The horizontal axis is stimulus intensity, with the first gray value representing baseline TMH measured prior to any stimulus being present. The other numbers on the x-axis are multiples of stimulus intensity at detection threshold: 0 represents the catch trials when the esthesiometer intensity setting was zero, 0.5 was when intensity was half threshold, 1 was when intensity was the same as threshold, 1.5 was when intensity was 50% higher than threshold and 2 was when intensity was double threshold. Mech, mechanical stimulus; Chem, chemical stimulus; C, central; P, peripheral.
Figure 1.
 
TMH at baseline (unstimulated condition) and with noxious stimulation. The gray dashed lines indicate the 95% confidence interval (CI) of unstimulated TMH. The horizontal axis is stimulus intensity, with the first gray value representing baseline TMH measured prior to any stimulus being present. The other numbers on the x-axis are multiples of stimulus intensity at detection threshold: 0 represents the catch trials when the esthesiometer intensity setting was zero, 0.5 was when intensity was half threshold, 1 was when intensity was the same as threshold, 1.5 was when intensity was 50% higher than threshold and 2 was when intensity was double threshold. Mech, mechanical stimulus; Chem, chemical stimulus; C, central; P, peripheral.
Figure 2.
 
TMH (average of stimulus modalities and locations) versus stimulus intensity. Vertical bars: 95% confidence intervals. *Significant differences compared with 0 stimulus intensity (P < 0.05).
Figure 2.
 
TMH (average of stimulus modalities and locations) versus stimulus intensity. Vertical bars: 95% confidence intervals. *Significant differences compared with 0 stimulus intensity (P < 0.05).
Figure 3.
 
Interaction of TMH measurement and stimulus intensity regardless of stimulus modality at both positions. Vertical bars: 95% confidence intervals. *Significant differences compared with corresponding peripheral stimulation (P < 0.05).
Figure 3.
 
Interaction of TMH measurement and stimulus intensity regardless of stimulus modality at both positions. Vertical bars: 95% confidence intervals. *Significant differences compared with corresponding peripheral stimulation (P < 0.05).
Figure 4.
 
Interaction of TMH measurement and stimulus intensity regardless of stimulus position for both stimulus modalities. Vertical bars: 95% confidence intervals. *Significant difference compared with chemical stimulation (P = 0.001).
Figure 4.
 
Interaction of TMH measurement and stimulus intensity regardless of stimulus position for both stimulus modalities. Vertical bars: 95% confidence intervals. *Significant difference compared with chemical stimulation (P = 0.001).
Figure 5.
 
Interaction of TMH measurement and stimulus intensity for both stimulus modalities and position. Vertical bars: 95% confidence intervals. *Significant difference compared with the maximum intensity of chemical stimulation (P = 0.003).
Figure 5.
 
Interaction of TMH measurement and stimulus intensity for both stimulus modalities and position. Vertical bars: 95% confidence intervals. *Significant difference compared with the maximum intensity of chemical stimulation (P = 0.003).
Table 1.
 
Thresholds of Detectable Tearing and Stimulus Detection
Table 1.
 
Thresholds of Detectable Tearing and Stimulus Detection
Threshold Mechanical (mL/min) Chemical (% CO2 added)
Central Peripheral Central Peripheral
Tearing 60.12 ± 11.09 65.25 ± 11.94 30.05 ± 6.61 38.95 ± 6.06
Stimulus detection 54.36 ± 5.34 51.60 ± 6.01 21.84 ± 2.27 20.27 ± 2.29
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×