Direct comparisons between the control and patient groups revealed that the amblyopes performed worse on several key measures of movement dynamics when using binocular vision, as well as under N-D eye conditions, the deficits in which covaried with existing VA loss. By contrast, there were few between-group differences in more spatial aspects of performance under these viewing conditions and in their DOM eye behavior (but see later description).
Figure 2illustrates the relationship between viewing condition and depth of amblyopia for one of the affected measures: the average movement execution time (MT, depth effect, F
(3,36)= 4.1,
P < 0.015). There were no differences between the normal subjects and those with mild amblyopia, but MT was significantly prolonged in both the moderate and severe amblyopes when viewing binocularly (by ∼125 and 210 ms, respectively;
P < 0.025) or with the N-D eye (by ∼180 and 340 ms, respectively;
P < 0.01). Plotting the average MT against the logMAR VA of the amblyopic eye in each patient revealed a positive correlation (
R 2 = 0.23) between the two for this eye, but not for binocular (
R 2 = 0.07) or DOM eye (
R 2 = 0.05) performance. An identical pattern of results (all depth effects, F
(3,36)≥ 2.8
P< 0.05), with similar correlations between N-D eye impairment and VA loss (
R 2 = 0.19–0.21), was obtained for two temporal parameters of the reach—its overall duration (RD) and LVP—and for the grip closure and application times. That is, the relatively extended movement times in the moderate and severe amblyopes under binocular and N-D eye conditions arose mainly because they prolonged their final approach to the object (increased LVP, GCT) and time in contact with it while securing their grip (increased GAT).
Depth of amblyopia also influenced the mean number of reaching (F
(3,36) = 3.3,
P < 0.05) and grasping (F
(3,36) = 12.9,
P < 0.0001) errors. For reaching errors
(Fig. 3A) , ANOVA showed that only the severe amblyopes were impaired relative to the control subjects under binocular conditions (
P = 0.006), whereas those with moderate (
P < 0.025) or severe (
P < 0.0001) VA losses were more (×1.5–2.5) error prone when using the amblyopic eye (
R 2 = 0.17). In all these cases, the increases were mainly due to the patients’ making more adjustments in the
velocity of the reach during its LVP (e.g.,
Fig. 1B ), rather than to alterations in its spatial path. Grasping deficits showed a different pattern
(Fig. 3B) . All three patient groups produced significantly more errors when using binocular vision (×1.6–3.5) and their affected eyes (×1.5–3.0), compared with the equivalent performance of the normal participants, and these increases mainly resulted from
spatial corrections in which there were additional opening/closures of the digits just before or after object contact (e.g.,
Fig. 1D ). Indeed, these increased grasping errors were the sole indicator of significant prehension deficits among the mild amblyopes (
P < 0.05 for both comparisons), and this contributed to relatively strong correlations between the number of errors committed and VA loss for both binocular (
R 2 = 0.50) and N-D eye (
R 2 = 0.36) performance.
Further analyses revealed important interactions between viewing condition and subject group affecting the parameters RT, R-GC, and dPG. For the reaction time, this (view × depth) interaction (F
(6,72) = 4.8,
P < 0.005) resulted from the severe amblyopes always taking substantially (100–400 ms) longer to initiate their movements than the other subjects. For R-GC it was due to the moderate and severe amblyopes contacting the object slightly earlier (by ∼20 ms) than normal when using binocular vision (view × depth interaction, F
(6,72) = 2.8,
P < 0.02). The interaction for distance of peak grip from the target (dPG, view × depth, F
(6,72) = 2.5,
P < 0.025) was more unusual and is shown in
Figure 4 . It can be seen that the mildly amblyopic subjects opened their grip to a maximum aperture closer to the target compared with the controls, and post hoc tests showed that this effect was significant for both conditions of monocular viewing (DOM eye,
P = 0.014; N-D eye,
P = 0.045). Moreover, dPG was also smaller across all patients under DOM eye conditions (group effect, F
(1,38) = 7.9,
P < 0.01), although these effects did not achieve significance on testing by amblyopia depth. Because opening the hand at preshaping close to the target (i.e., reduced dPG) is an element of the more confident binocular versus monocular performance in control subjects (see
Table 3 ), these findings imply that some amblyopes are superior in this regard compared with adults with normal vision.
There was just one other indication of enhanced DOM eye performance in the patients, as a group, again related to grip accuracy at preshaping. A repeated finding in normal adults is that most reach parameters (e.g., PV, RD) increase linearly with increasing target distance, whereas others related to the grasp (e.g., PGA, GOC) increase with increasing object size.
20 27 28 Figure 5illustrates these relationships for the mean peak reaching velocity to the near–far targets and for the mean peak grip aperture for the small–large objects in the control subjects and patients under each viewing condition. The kinematics were very strongly affected by these object properties with similar scaling exhibited by both subject groups (PV, distance effect, F
(3,36)> 500,
P < 0.0001; PGA, size effect, F
(3,36)> 400,
P < 0.0001). For example, the doubling of target distance from 200 (near) to 400 (far) mm from the starting position resulted in a near doubling (from ∼500 to 900 mm/s) in PV, regardless of viewing condition
(Fig. 5A) . Reaches were slightly faster (by ∼30 mm/s) to both distances under DOM eye conditions in the amblyopes compared with the control subjects, but this difference was not significant (eye × distance × group interaction,
P = 0.2). There was, however, a significant three-way (eye × size × group) interaction for PGA (
P = 0.002).
Figure 5Bshows that this was mainly due to a difference in DOM eye performance. Specifically, the normal adults opened their grip much wider compared with binocular viewing—particularly for the smaller object—but this occurred to a much lesser extent in the patients, indicating more confident (and better) grip scaling when programming the grasp via the DOM eye.