Although often described as being mediated by a simple reflex loop, the retinal response that eventually leads to a pupillary contraction is subject to substantial modification at various locations along the pupillary pathway. The afferent signal, carried by intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells (ipRGCs),
13,14 varies in strength depending on the location of the luminance stimulus in the visual field. This pattern can be seen in the variation in contraction amplitudes between test regions in this study (
Fig. 2) and largely reflects the topography of ganglion cell densities within the retina.
13,15 In addition to the modulation of signal strength at various locations in the pathway, the distribution of signal at the optic chiasm and midbrain hemi-decussations creates further complexity in the resulting pattern of direct and consensual pupillary responses.
Assuming the same division of ipRGC axons at the optic chiasm as RGC axons within the main visual pathway, these cells project from homonymous hemiretinae to the corresponding pretectal olivary nucleus (PON); e.g., from right hemiretinae (left visual field of both eyes) to right PON. This means that signal from both the temporal field of the contralateral eye and the nasal field of the ipsilateral eye arrives at each PON. From the PON, intercalated neurons project to ipsilateral and contralateral Edinger-Westphal nuclei (EWN). The measurement of direct and consensual responses in many locations of the visual field therefore has the potential to provide information regarding the nature of the distribution of signals originating in different regions of the retina, between these midbrain projections.
The uniformity of the direct/consensual ratios within each hemifield in this study (
Fig. 2, right) implies that for given stimulus conditions, the proportional distribution of signal to each EWN is solely dependent on whether that signal originates in nasal or temporal retina. Unlike regional variations in pupillary contraction amplitudes, this distribution appears to be independent of topographic variation in retinal sensitivity or the specific location of the stimulus within a given hemifield. The larger proportional difference between direct and consensual responses in temporal field compared with nasal (
Fig. 3) is consistent with the majority of pupillary investigations of this temporo-nasal asymmetry.
4 –6
Anatomic studies in primates have provided evidence of asymmetry in the bilateral projections to the EWN, favoring the decussating pathway.
16 –18 Distribution of both temporal and nasal signals based solely on this weighting would result in equivalent but opposite ratios in each hemifield, since the more substantial projection to contralateral EWN produces the direct response when originating in nasal retina, and the consensual response when originating in temporal retina. This infers larger consensual than direct responses in the nasal field, quite different from the equivalent nasal responses we have observed. Larger consensual than direct nasal responses have been reported by others,
3,7 this being however, clearly at odds with the results presented here and therefore warranting explanation.
We have previously reported significant response saturation for stimuli having higher luminance levels, at a range of presentation rates.
19 Therefore, one explanation for reports of larger consensual than direct responses in the nasal field may arise from the use in those studies
3,7 of high-luminance, large-area stimuli that are more likely to drive the system to maximal, saturated responses. Studies reporting results like those of this current analysis (i.e., direct/consensual = 1 in the temporal field) tended to employ stimuli that would be less saturating,
4 –6 like most of the stimuli used here. This suggests that ranking the effect sizes from the linear model (
Fig. 3) using an estimate of the total retinal input to the PON may provide a rationale for these apparently discordant results.
Mean contraction amplitudes for each study were used as a starting point for this estimate of input to the PON, since when holding all other variables constant, responses to these afferent inputs increase linearly with the log of stimulus luminance.
20 Mean amplitudes were multiplied by a measure of stimulus field intensity, comprising an approximation of the total luminance delivered to the entire visual field over a given period (see Methods). These field intensity values ranged between 1.0 (protocol 4C) and 43.2 (protocol 1B). The resulting composite measure therefore provided an estimate of the afferent input to the PON during a stimulus pulse, not just from the region stimulated but also from the rest of the visual field.
Figure 4 shows the proportional differences between direct and consensual responses plotted against this measure. Also plotted are the effects for a group of 12 subjects (5 male, 7 female, aged 34.5 ± 11.3 SD) tested using a high luminance 700 cd/m
2, 200 ms stimulus duration, 24 region stimulus protocol (
Fig. 4, triangles). It can be seen from this plot that the difference between direct and consensual responses in the temporal field decreases with increasing stimulus intensity. In the nasal field, at lower intensities, there is tendency for consensual responses to be larger than direct, this tendency decreasing with increasing intensity until these responses become fairly equal. The existence of these trends within this data begs further investigation.
Martin et al. have proposed that for each PON of an individual there is a fixed ratio of the distribution of signal between the contralateral and ipsilateral midbrain projections.
7 Given the results of other studies however, this appears to be just one component of a more complex pattern. The pattern of responses observed in our present study, as well as larger consensual than direct responses at high overall luminance levels, can be explained by a model such as that presented in
Figure 5. We have shown previously that the relationship between stimulus intensity and pupillary contraction amplitudes is well represented by a saturating Naka-Rushton function:
where
R(
I) is the response at a given stimulus intensity,
R max represents the maximum attainable response,
K is the stimulus intensity at which half of the maximum attainable response is reached and z is equivalent to the slope of the function.
21 A set of Naka-Rushton curves that replicate the pattern of responses observed in this study can be produced by parsimonious adjustment of the half saturation constants, slope, and
R max of temporal and nasal projections (
Fig. 5).
This model proposes higher maximum response levels (
R max) and gain (
z), as well as earlier saturation (
K) for the contralateral projection (
Fig. 5B, upper). Within this projection, signal from temporal and nasal fields varies only in the level at which half saturation is reached (
K). Within the ipsilateral projection, signal from both hemifields behaves identically, this similarity being consistent with the pooling of both inputs within this pathway (
Fig. 5B, lower). Larger direct than consensual responses are predicted in the temporal field at all stimulus intensities (
Fig. 5C, upper). In the nasal field, larger consensual than direct responses are predicted at high stimulus intensities (
Fig. 5C, lower), the degree of this becoming more equivalent with that of temporal responses, as
R max is approached in this component of the contralateral projection. Predicted direct/consensual ratios produced using the proposed stimulus-response functions (
Fig. 5D, upper) fit the proportional effects from the linear model well (
Fig. 5D, lower).
In addition to providing an explanation for the trends within our data, these predictions are consistent with the results of the aforementioned experiments in which larger consensual than direct responses have been observed.
3,7 Two plausible locations for these types of more saturated responses have been plotted on the ratio curves (
Fig. 5D, upper). The first case indicates where some degree of difference is present in the nasal field, but not to the same extent as temporal, and the second a more extreme case where the differences are more equivalent. The occurrence of a relative afferent pupil defect (RAPD) due to a post-chiasmal lesion can also be explained, the differing dynamics of the ipsilateral and contralateral projections creating the large differences in sensitivity between the two eyes in the manner described by Kardon et al.
22
The different levels of complexity for the two projections in this model are not incongruous with primate anatomic observations of opposing density gradients of afferent PON inputs dependent on hemiretinal origin, and the wider distribution of PON efferents projecting to the contralateral EWN than to ipsilateral.
18,23 This may suggest that the different response functions proposed for the decussating contralateral projection, are not the result of separate populations of neurons, but rather the effect of recruitment of additional neurons when the signal originates in the temporal field. This idea is further supported by the existence of PON neurons with ocular dominance restricted to, or weighted toward, the contralateral eye (i.e., the temporal field).
24 Further cells recorded in that study by Clarke et al.
24 responded equally well to stimulation from either eye, and it may be that the decussating EWN projection is composed of both these and the contralaterally dominated type. A subset of these cells with no ocular preference also provide a likely source for the non-decussating projection to the ipsilateral EWN.
The idea of slightly differing response dynamics in these midbrain projections depending on retinal origin, presents some challenges and opportunities for diagnostic methods using pupil responses. The use of the mean of the direct and consensual responses obtained using whole field saturating stimuli, such as in the assessment of a RAPD, may result in inaccurate estimations of dysfunction due to the differing rates of gain and saturation within these midbrain pathways. Similarly, stimuli that target the most sensitive region of the response curves will, at the same stimulus intensities, have responses that vary at different rates in the presence of reduced input. These examples suggest that improved diagnostic accuracy may be achieved simply by using measurements of both direct and consensual responses to dimmer, hemifield, or smaller stimuli. This would therefore allow adjustments to be made for the differing dynamics of the pathways contributing to these responses.
This study has demonstrated that the distribution of signal to each EWN differs depending on the hemiretina of origin, at the same time being independent of the specific location within that hemiretina. It also provides the first evidence in humans of the summation of retinal signal at the pretectal olivary nucleus in the pupillary pathway. The proposed model suggests that signal in the projection to the ipsilateral EWN, comprised of nasal field direct and temporal field consensual responses, is likely to be mediated by a single population of neurons. The projection to the contralateral EWN, responsible for the temporal field direct and nasal field consensual responses, demonstrates differing response dynamics dependent on retinal origin. This may indicate the recruitment of additional neurons for signal originating in the nasal retina. These findings have implications for the assessment of neural dysfunction using pupillary responses, since differing rates of saturation and gain impact on the accurate comparison between these different response components. Separate assessment of direct and consensual responses at hemifield or better resolution is therefore recommended in the assessment of neural dysfunction involving pupillary pathways.
Supported by the Australian Research Council through the ARC Centre of Excellence in Vision Science (CE0561903).
Disclosures:
C.F. Carle, Seeing Machines Ltd (F);
T. Maddess, Seeing Machines Ltd (F, I, C, P, R);
A.C. James, Seeing Machines Ltd (F, I, C, P, R)
The authors thank Maria Kolic, Xin-Lin Goh, and Yik-Wen Loh for their assistance in data collection and recruitment of subjects; Maria Kolic and Rohan Essex for clinical assessments; and the participants for their time and cooperation.