Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science Cover Image for Volume 53, Issue 12
November 2012
Volume 53, Issue 12
Free
Eye Movements, Strabismus, Amblyopia and Neuro-ophthalmology  |   November 2012
Effects of Strabismic Amblyopia and Strabismus without Amblyopia on Visuomotor Behavior, I: Saccadic Eye Movements
Author Affiliations & Notes
  • Ewa Niechwiej-Szwedo
    Department of Ophthalmology and Vision Sciences, The Hospital for Sick Children, Toronto, Ontario, Canada; and the
  • Manokaraananthan Chandrakumar
    Department of Ophthalmology and Vision Sciences, The Hospital for Sick Children, Toronto, Ontario, Canada; and the
  • Herbert C. Goltz
    Department of Ophthalmology and Vision Sciences, The Hospital for Sick Children, Toronto, Ontario, Canada; and the
    University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada.
  • Agnes M. F. Wong
    Department of Ophthalmology and Vision Sciences, The Hospital for Sick Children, Toronto, Ontario, Canada; and the
    University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada.
  • Corresponding author: Agnes M. F. Wong, Department of Ophthalmology and Vision Sciences, The Hospital for Sick Children, 555 University Avenue, Toronto, Ontario, Canada M5G 1X8; [email protected]
Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science November 2012, Vol.53, 7458-7468. doi:https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.12-10550
  • Views
  • PDF
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Ewa Niechwiej-Szwedo, Manokaraananthan Chandrakumar, Herbert C. Goltz, Agnes M. F. Wong; Effects of Strabismic Amblyopia and Strabismus without Amblyopia on Visuomotor Behavior, I: Saccadic Eye Movements. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2012;53(12):7458-7468. https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.12-10550.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract

Purpose.: It has previously been shown that anisometropic amblyopia affects the programming and execution of saccades. The aim of the current study was to investigate the impact of strabismic amblyopia on saccade performance.

Methods.: Fourteen adults with strabismic amblyopia, 13 adults with strabismus without amblyopia, and 14 visually normal adults performed saccades and reach-to-touch movements to targets presented at ±5° and ±10° eccentricity during binocular and monocular viewing. Latency, amplitude, and peak velocity of primary and secondary saccades were measured.

Results.: In contrast to visually normal participants who had shorter primary saccade latency during binocular viewing, no binocular advantage was found in patients with strabismus with or without amblyopia. Patients with amblyopia had longer saccade latency during amblyopic eye viewing (P < 0.0001); however, there were no significant differences in saccade amplitude precision among the three groups across viewing conditions. Further analysis showed that only patients with severe amblyopia and no stereopsis (n = 4) exhibited longer latency (which was more pronounced for more central targets; P < 0.0001), and they also had reduced amplitude precision during amblyopic eye viewing. In contrast, patients with mild amblyopia (n = 5) and no stereopsis had normal latency and reduced precision during amblyopic eye viewing (P < 0.001), whereas those with gross stereopsis (n = 5) had normal latency and precision. There were no differences in peak velocity among the groups.

Conclusions.: Distinct patterns of saccade performance according to different levels of visual acuity and stereoscopic losses in strabismic amblyopia were found. These findings were in contrast to those in anisometropic amblyopia in which the altered saccade performance was independent of the extent of visual acuity or stereoscopic deficits. These results were most likely due to different long-term sensory suppression mechanisms in strabismic versus anisometropic amblyopia.

Introduction
Amblyopia is a neural disorder caused by inadequate visual stimulation during the early critical period of development. 1 Amblyopia is commonly associated with two risk factors: strabismus (eye misalignment) and anisometropia (difference in refractive errors between the eyes). 2 Patients with amblyopia have reduced visual acuity and contrast sensitivity, as well as other perceptual deficits, 310 which are most pronounced during amblyopic eye viewing; however, they are also evident during fellow eye viewing, albeit to a lesser extent. 1113  
The effects of amblyopia on visuomotor behavior have not been examined as extensively as the sensory/perceptual deficits. This is surprising because a fundamental function of the brain is to use sensory information from all modalities to make purposeful, goal-directed behaviors through the process of sensorimotor integration. Vision provides critical information about the location and properties of objects that we want to interact with or avoid. To see an object in detail, the fovea—the area of highest resolution on the retina—has to be directed toward the object, which is achieved via saccadic eye movements. Visual information is combined across eye movements to form internal spatial representations of the external world. 14,15 Thus, saccadic eye movements are an essential component of the action–perception loop, and play an important role in guiding flexible behaviors while people interact with objects in dynamic environments. 
Our group has been studying the effects of impaired spatiotemporal visual functions in amblyopia on motor behavior. In a series of detailed investigations, we have recently reported the impact of anisometropic amblyopia on saccadic eye movements, 16 reaching movements of the upper limb, 17,18 and eye–hand coordination 19 during visually guided reaching. Specifically, we have shown that patients with anisometropic amblyopia had significantly longer and more variable saccade latency during amblyopic eye viewing, 16 lower peak acceleration and a longer acceleration phase during reaching, 18 and a different temporal pattern of eye–hand coordination. 19 Importantly, the effects of amblyopia on reaching movements were evident not only during amblyopic eye viewing, but also during binocular and fellow eye viewing. 
As a next step in our systematic investigations, we examined whether different subtypes of amblyopia affect motor behaviors differentially. Previous studies have demonstrated several differences in perceptual deficits among patients with anisometropic versus strabismic amblyopia. For example, patients with anisometropic amblyopia exhibited deficits in contrast detection 8 and spatial localization across the entire visual field, 20 whereas patients with strabismic amblyopia exhibited more pronounced deficits in the central visual field than those in the peripheral visual field. A study of 427 amblyopic patients has also shown distinctive patterns of visual deficits among different amblyopia subtypes. 13 Patients with anisometropic amblyopia and moderate loss of acuity had normal/subnormal contrast sensitivity and were more likely to have gross stereopsis, whereas those with strabismic amblyopia and moderate loss of acuity had better than normal contrast sensitivity at low spatial frequencies and were more likely to have reduced/absent stereopsis. 
The effects of strabismic amblyopia on saccadic eye movements have only been investigated in two previous studies. Schor 21 examined saccades in five patients using a predictable, square-wave stimulus to elicit saccades. He reported no difference in latency and a significant increase in amplitude variability when patients viewed with their amblyopic eye. Ciuffreda and colleagues 22 tested six patients using an unpredictable stepping target and found longer saccade latencies in some patients. However, they 21,22 did not include a visually normal control group and their sample size was small, which precluded a more detailed analysis of the effects of visual acuity or stereoacuity on saccade performance. Previous studies of children with strabismus without amblyopia found no difference in saccade latency between children with and without binocular vision 23 and in comparison to children with normal vision. 24 However, adults with strabismus showed an impairment in binocular coordination of saccades that was most pronounced in patients without binocular vision. 25  
The objective of the current study was to investigate saccadic eye movements during a visually guided reaching task in patients with strabismic amblyopia, as well as the effects of visual acuity and stereoacuity deficits on saccade performance. We hypothesized that impairments in saccade performance would be largest in patients with amblyopia during amblyopic eye viewing. We also hypothesized that patients' performance would be affected by their level of visual acuity and stereoacuity deficits. Specifically, we hypothesized that saccades will be delayed and have reduced precision in patients with poorer acuity and negative stereopsis. Results of the reaching movement are the focus of our next study. 
Methods
Participants
All participants were adults and underwent a complete orthoptic assessment by a certified orthoptist, which included visual acuity (Snellen chart), prism cover test (simultaneous and alternate) of eye alignment, and measurement of stereoacuity using the Titmus test. Exclusion criteria were any ocular cause for reduced visual acuity, previous intraocular surgery, or any neurologic disease. All participants were right-handed to reduce the variability in motor performance. 26  
Fourteen patients with strabismic amblyopia (6 females; age: 31.7 ± 9.9 years; see Table 1 for clinical characteristics) were recruited. Strabismic amblyopia was defined as an interocular acuity difference ≥2 lines on a Snellen chart, and subjects with a history of childhood strabismus and manifest eye deviation. Visual acuity was tested with current refractive correction. The difference in refractive error between the two eyes was ≤1 diopter (D) of spherical or cylindrical power, to rule out a potentially amblyogenic astigmatic component (i.e., to rule out mixed-mechanism amblyopia). When stereopsis was absent, the presence or absence of sensory fusion was determined using Worth's 4-dot test and Bagolini-striated glasses. Ten patients had mild amblyopia, with acuity in the amblyopic eye ranging from 20/30 to 20/60. Five of the patients with mild amblyopia had gross stereopsis (range: 120–800 seconds of arc), whereas the other five patients had negative stereopsis. Four patients had severe amblyopia (20/200 in the amblyopic eye) and negative stereopsis. Visual acuity in the fellow eye was 20/20 or better in all patients. 
Table 1. 
 
Characteristics of Patients with Strabismic Amblyopia and Strabismus without Amblyopia
Table 1. 
 
Characteristics of Patients with Strabismic Amblyopia and Strabismus without Amblyopia
ID Age, y Sex Snellen Visual Acuity (logMAR) Refractive Error Deviation on Simultaneous Prism Cover Test (Alternate Prism Test) Stereoacuity (seconds of arc) Fusion Notes
Right Left Right Left Eye Near (PD) Distance (PD)
Patients with strabismic amblyopia
1 27 F 20/15 (−0.10) 20/40 (0.30) −1.00+0.50 −1.00+0.50 Left ET8 (ET+E10) ET2 (ET+E4) 400
2 37 F 20/40 (0.30) 20/20 (0.00) −4.00 −4.00 Right ET4 (ET+E6) ET4 800
3 19 F 20/20 (0.00) 20/40 (0.30) −2.75 −3.00 Left ET2 (ET+E14) ET2 400 Hx occlusion
4 29 M 20/30 (0.18) 20/20 (0.00) −2.25+3.00×110 −2.50+2.25×75 Right XT2 (XT+X40) XT2 (XT+X45) 200
5 28 F 20/20 (0.00) 20/30 (0.18) +0.75+0.25×45 +0.25+0.75×95 Left ET1 ET2 120 Hx occlusion
6 38 M 20/30 (0.18) 20/15 (−0.10) −3.50 −3.50+0.50×25 Right ET35, hypo4 ET35, hypo6 Negative Suppress Hx occlusion
7 19 M 20/50 (0.40) 20/20 (0.00) Plano Plano Right ET16 ET14 Negative Suppress Hx occlusion
8 30 M 20/15 (−0.10) 20/30 (0.18) Plano Plano Left ET35 ET35 Negative Suppress IET
9 51 F 20/50 (0.40) 20/20 (0.00) +1.75+2.00×74 +2.50+1.25×90 Right ET14, hypo14 ET8, hypo12 Negative Suppress IET, Hx surgery/ occlusion
10 21 F 20/20 (0.00) 20/30 (0.18) +1.50+1.50×65 +1.25+1.00×110 Left ET2, hypo5 bilateral DVD ET6, hypo5 bilateral DVD Negative W4D IET, Hx surgery/ occlusion
11 25 M 20/20 (0.00) 20/200 (1.00) −0.75+0.50×90 −0.25 Left ET12 ET10 Negative Suppress Non-central fixation, Hx occlusion
12 37 M 20/20 (0.00) 20/200 (1.00) Plano Plano Left XT 4 (XT+X8) XT 4 (XT+X6) Negative Non-central fixation
13 48 M 20/15 (−0.10) 20/200 (1.00) Plano Plano Left XT40 XT30 Negative Suppress Non-central fixation, Hx surgery/ occlusion
14 35 M 20/20 (0.00) 20/200 (1.00) −0.75 Plano Left XT59, HT12 XT40, HT10 Negative Suppress Non-central fixation, Hx surgery/ occlusion
Patients with strabismus without amblyopia
15 26 F 20/15 (−0.10) 20/20 (0.00) +1.00+0.25×35 +2.00 Left ET 2 (ET+E4) ET 2 (ET+E4) 50 Hx occlusion
16 21 M 20/15 (−0.10) 20/15 (−0.10) Plano Plano Right X4, H2 X4, H2 80 Hx surgery
17 31 F 20/20 (0.00) 20/25 (0.10) Plano Plano Right ET4 (ET+E12) ET4 (ET+E10) 3000 Hx occlusion
18 27 M 20/20 (0.00) 20/20 (0.00) Plano Plano Right ET2, HT3 (ET+E4) ET2, HT3 (ET+E4) 3000 Hx occlusion/ surgery
19 29 F 20/15 (−0.10) 20/20 (0.00) Plano −2.00+0.50×170 Right XT16, hypo2 XT14, HT3 Negative Suppress Hx surgery
20 33 F 20/15 (−0.10) 20/15 (−0.10) Plano Plano Left AET 50 ET4 (ET+E25) Negative Suppress
21 33 M 20/15 (−0.10) 20/15 (−0.10) −1.50 Plano Left ET20, bilateral HT ET20, bilateral HT Negative W4D ARC, Hx surgery
22 32 F 20/20 (0.00) 20/20 (0.00) −2.50+0.50×175 −4.00+1.25×115 Right ET20, HT3 ET18, HT3 Negative Peripheral fusion on Bagolini ARC
23 31 M 20/15 (−0.10) 20/15 (−0.10) −7.50+3.00×80 −4.25+2.00×95 Left AET45, HT18 R/AET40, HT16 Negative Suppress Hx occlusion/ surgery
24 34 M 20/15 (−0.10) 20/15 (−0.10) +1.50+0.50×94 pl+1.00×65 Right AET18 ET12 Negative Suppress Occlusion, surgery
25 34 F 20/15 (−0.10) 20/25 (0.10) +2.75 +4.00+1.00×115 Left ET20 ET25 Negative Int. fusion on W4D fusion
26 21 M 20/15 (−0.10) 20/15 (−0.10) +3.50+1.25×110 +3.00+1.00×95 Right ET25, hypo6 ET14, hypo6 Negative Suppress IET
27 25 M 20/15 (−0.10) 20/20 (0.00) −1.00+0.50×75 −0.75+0.25×130 Left XT4, HT4 XT25, hypo5 Negative Suppress Occlusion
Thirteen patients with strabismus without amblyopia (acuity 20/25 or better in both eyes) were also recruited (6 females; age: 30.1 ± 4.0 years; see Table 1 for clinical characteristics). All patients had manifest eye deviation. Nine patients were tested negative for stereopsis, two had stereoacuity of 3000 seconds of arc, and the remaining two had stereoacuity of 50 and 80 seconds of arc. 
Fourteen visually normal participants (6 females; age: 31.7 ± 9.9 years) with corrected-to-normal visual acuity (20/20 or better) in both eyes and stereoacuity ≤ 40 seconds of arc were recruited. Eye dominance in visually normal participants was determined using the Dolman “hole-in-card” test. 27  
The study was approved by the Research Ethics Board at The Hospital for Sick Children, and all protocols adhered to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki. Informed consent was obtained from each participant. 
Apparatus and Experimental Protocol
Details of the apparatus and experimental procedure have been described in a previous article. 16 Briefly, the visual target was a white square (visual angle: 0.5°) presented on a black background on a cathode ray tube computer monitor (Diamond Pro 2070SB, resolution 1600 × 1200 at 85 Hz; NEC/Mitsubishi Electric Visual Systems, Tokyo, Japan). Testing was conducted in a dimly lit room. Eye movements were recorded binocularly at 200 Hz using a video-based pupil/iris tracking system (Chronos Vision, Berlin, Germany). Reaching movements of the upper limb were also recorded simultaneously at 200 Hz using an infrared illumination-based motion-capture system (Optotrak Certus; Northern Digital, Waterloo, Canada). 
Participants were seated at a table with their heads stabilized on a chin rest. They fixated a cross at the beginning of each trial. The viewing distance was 42 cm. After a variable delay of 1.5 to 3 seconds, the fixation cross was extinguished and one visual target appeared randomly at one of four eccentricities at ±5° or ±10° along the horizontal axis. There was no temporal delay between the offset of the fixation point and the presentation of the target (simultaneous fixation offset and target onset). Participants were instructed to look at the target and to make a reaching movement to touch the target with their index finger as quickly and as accurately as possible. Details of the reaching task have been described previously. 18 In 50% of the trials, the target was switched off at the onset of hand movement. For the other 50% of the trials, the target remained on the screen. Trials with and without visual feedback of the target were randomized on a trial-by-trial basis. 
Participants performed the experiment in three viewing conditions: (1) binocular viewing; (2) monocular viewing with the dominant eye (i.e., the fellow eye for patients with amblyopia, and the nondeviating eye for patients with strabismus without amblyopia); and (3) monocular viewing with the nondominant eye (i.e., the amblyopic eye for patients with amblyopia, and the deviating eye for patients with strabismus without amblyopia). Data were collected in blocks for each viewing condition, and the order of viewing conditions was randomized across participants. All participants completed 10 trials in each combination of the experimental conditions for a total of 240 trials/session. Practice trials were completed before the experiment was begun to familiarize the participants with the experimental procedure. All data were collected in one session (1–1.5 hours), which included calibration of the apparatus, practice, and experimental trials. 
Analysis: Saccadic Eye Movements
Eye position data were low-pass filtered using a second-order dual-pass Butterworth filter with a cutoff frequency of 50 Hz. Eye velocity was obtained using a two-point differentiation method. A custom-written script (Matlab; The MathWorks, Natick, MA) was used to identify primary saccades using a velocity threshold of 20 deg/s. All trials were inspected visually to ensure that saccades were identified correctly by the computer script. We examined the recordings of the right and left eyes during the binocular viewing condition and found no difference in saccade kinematics between eyes. Therefore, only one eye was included in the statistical analysis. During monocular viewing conditions, only the viewing eye was analyzed. Outcome measures for primary saccades were mean latency, amplitude, and peak velocity. The variability (SD) of primary saccade amplitude was also calculated. 
All trials were inspected for the presence of secondary saccades that were marked manually for each trial. Secondary saccades that occurred within 250 ms of the primary saccades were defined as corrective saccades. The outcome measures for secondary saccades were frequency in each viewing condition, as well as mean saccade latency, amplitude, and peak velocity. In addition, we calculated the precision (i.e., variability) of the final amplitude after secondary saccades were executed. The final amplitude was defined as the algebraic sum of primary and secondary saccade amplitudes. The final saccade amplitude precision was used to examine whether secondary saccades were generated to correct the error in position that remained after the primary saccade. 
Statistical Analysis
All continuous dependent variables were submitted to repeated-measures ANOVA with one between-subjects factor: Group (strabismic amblyopia, strabismus without amblyopia, visually normal) and three within-subjects factors: Viewing Condition (binocular, monocular dominant eye [fellow eye for patients with amblyopia], and monocular nondominant eye [amblyopic eye for patients with amblyopia]), Target Location (5°, 10°), and Visual Feedback of Target (on, off). 
The frequency of corrective saccades was compared using Pearson's χ2 statistic. The frequency of saccades was first compared between patients and visually normal participants, then the effect of Viewing Condition was examined within each group, both using Pearson's χ2 statistic. 
All statistical analyses were performed using a commercial statistical analysis software program (SAS Software, version 9.2; SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC). Descriptive statistics were reported as the mean and corresponding SD. All main effects and interactions were analyzed further using Tukey–Kramer post hoc tests to adjust for multiple comparisons. The significance level was set at P < 0.05. Preliminary analysis of all the data showed that Visual Feedback of Target had no significant effect on any outcome measures; therefore, data with or without visual feedback were collapsed for subsequent analysis and reporting. Saccades to the left and right targets at each eccentricity were pooled together for statistical analysis. 
Effects of Severity of Amblyopia and Stereopsis.
To investigate further the effects of severity of amblyopia and stereopsis, a separate repeated-measures analysis was performed on each outcome measure. For this analysis, patients with amblyopia were stratified into three subgroups: (1) mild amblyopia (i.e., acuity ≤20/60) and gross stereopsis (mild−stereo+; Table 1: patients 1–5); (2) mild amblyopia and negative stereopsis (mild−stereo; Table 1: patients 6–10); and (3) severe amblyopia (i.e., acuity 20/200) and negative stereopsis (Table 1: patients 11–14). Since the number of subjects in each subgroup was small, a nonparametric approach was used. All data were transformed to ranks and then submitted to a repeated-measures ANOVA following the procedure developed by Conover and Iman. 28 The ANOVA had Subgroup as a between-subjects factor (i.e., mild−stereo+, mild−stereo, severe) and two within-subjects factors: Viewing Condition (binocular, monocular fellow eye, and monocular amblyopic eye) and Target Location (5°, 10°). 
Effects of Stereopsis in Patients with Strabismus without Amblyopia.
To investigate the effects of stereopsis in patients with strabismus without amblyopia, a separate repeated-measures ANOVA on the ranked data 28 was performed on each outcome measure. For this analysis, patients with strabismus without amblyopia were divided into two subgroups: stereo positive (n = 4), and stereo negative (n = 9). The ANOVA had Subgroup as a between-subjects factor (i.e., stereo+, stereo) and two within-subjects factors: Viewing Condition (binocular, monocular dominant eye, and monocular nondominant eye) and Target Location (5°, 10°). 
Effects of Strabismic Eye Deviation.
Spearman correlation analysis was performed to investigate the relation between the amount of eye deviation and primary saccade outcome measures (latency, amplitude accuracy [mean] and precision [SD], peak velocity). The correlation coefficient was calculated separately for patients with strabismic amblyopia and patients with strabismus without amblyopia for the different viewing conditions. 
Results
Primary Saccades
Figure 1 shows representative eye velocity tracings from a visually normal participant and individual patients with strabismic amblyopia (mild−stereo+, mild−stereo, severe), and strabismus without amblyopia (stereo+, stereo). The visually normal participant showed highly stereotypical saccades in all viewing conditions. The largest deviation from the stereotypical saccade behavior was seen in the patient with severe amblyopia (negative stereopsis) during amblyopic eye viewing and in the patient with strabismus without amblyopia (negative stereopsis) in all viewing conditions. Both patients showed delayed and highly variable saccades. 
Figure 1. 
 
Representative eye velocity tracings from individual trials during monocular viewing with the nondominant/amblyopic eye (left column), monocular viewing with the dominant/fellow eye (middle column), and binocular viewing (right column) when the target was shown 10° to the right. Top row: a visually normal participant; second row: a patient with mild amblyopia and gross stereopsis (200 seconds of arc) (Table 1, ID 4); third row: a patient with mild amblyopia and negative stereopsis (Table 1, ID 6); fourth row: a patient with severe amblyopia and negative stereopsis (Table 1, ID 14); fifth row: a patient with strabismus and stereopsis (80 seconds of arc) (Table 1, ID 16); last row: a patient with strabismus without amblyopia and negative stereopsis (Table 1, ID 21).
Figure 1. 
 
Representative eye velocity tracings from individual trials during monocular viewing with the nondominant/amblyopic eye (left column), monocular viewing with the dominant/fellow eye (middle column), and binocular viewing (right column) when the target was shown 10° to the right. Top row: a visually normal participant; second row: a patient with mild amblyopia and gross stereopsis (200 seconds of arc) (Table 1, ID 4); third row: a patient with mild amblyopia and negative stereopsis (Table 1, ID 6); fourth row: a patient with severe amblyopia and negative stereopsis (Table 1, ID 14); fifth row: a patient with strabismus and stereopsis (80 seconds of arc) (Table 1, ID 16); last row: a patient with strabismus without amblyopia and negative stereopsis (Table 1, ID 21).
Latency.
The main effect of Viewing Condition (F 2,76 = 16.81; P < 0.0001) and the interaction between Group and Viewing Condition were significant (F 4,76 = 6.92; P < 0.0001; Fig. 2A). Post hoc tests revealed that mean saccade latency increased significantly when patients with strabismic amblyopia viewed with their amblyopic eye (218 ± 49 ms), compared with viewing with their fellow eye (172 ± 36 ms) or binocularly (177 ± 39 ms). Patients with strabismus without amblyopia had comparable saccade latency in all viewing conditions (binocular: 191 ± 29 ms; dominant eye: 190 ± 23 ms; nondominant eye: 198 ± 32 ms). In contrast, visually normal participants had significantly shorter saccade latency when viewing binocularly (173 ± 27 ms), compared with monocular viewing with the dominant eye (190 ± 25 ms) or nondominant eye (191 ± 26 ms). 
Figure 2. 
 
Mean saccade latencies as a function of viewing condition. (A) Latencies were significantly longer for patients with strabismic amblyopia (shown in red) during amblyopic eye viewing (P < 0.0001). Visually normal participants (shown in black) had shorter saccade latencies during binocular viewing compared with monocular viewing. In contrast, all patients had comparable saccade latency during binocular and fellow (nondeviated) eye viewing (i.e., no binocular advantage). Patients with strabismus without amblyopia (shown in green) had similar saccade latency in all viewing conditions. (B) Saccade latencies were significantly longer only for patients with severe amblyopia (blue lines, visual acuity 20/200, negative stereopsis) during amblyopic eye viewing (P < 0.0001).
Figure 2. 
 
Mean saccade latencies as a function of viewing condition. (A) Latencies were significantly longer for patients with strabismic amblyopia (shown in red) during amblyopic eye viewing (P < 0.0001). Visually normal participants (shown in black) had shorter saccade latencies during binocular viewing compared with monocular viewing. In contrast, all patients had comparable saccade latency during binocular and fellow (nondeviated) eye viewing (i.e., no binocular advantage). Patients with strabismus without amblyopia (shown in green) had similar saccade latency in all viewing conditions. (B) Saccade latencies were significantly longer only for patients with severe amblyopia (blue lines, visual acuity 20/200, negative stereopsis) during amblyopic eye viewing (P < 0.0001).
The three-way interaction between Group (strabismic amblyopia, strabismus without amblyopia, visually normal), Viewing Condition, and Target Location was also significant (F 6,76 = 5.12; P = 0.0002; Fig. 3A). Post hoc testing indicated that patients with strabismic amblyopia had significantly longer latency during amblyopic eye viewing for saccades to the 5° target (227 ± 52 ms) compared with the 10° target (209 ± 44 ms). Target Location did not affect saccade latency in patients with strabismus without amblyopia and visually normal participants in any viewing condition. 
Figure 3. 
 
Mean saccade latency as a function of target eccentricity across viewing conditions. (A) Patients with strabismic amblyopia (shown in red) had longer saccade latency during amblyopic eye viewing for the 5° target compared with the 10° target (P = 0.0002). (B) Further subgroup analysis revealed that only patients with severe visual acuity deficits due to amblyopia and negative stereopsis (shown in blue) had a significant longer saccade latency (P = 0.0315) during amblyopic eye viewing to targets located closer to fixation compared with more peripheral targets.
Figure 3. 
 
Mean saccade latency as a function of target eccentricity across viewing conditions. (A) Patients with strabismic amblyopia (shown in red) had longer saccade latency during amblyopic eye viewing for the 5° target compared with the 10° target (P = 0.0002). (B) Further subgroup analysis revealed that only patients with severe visual acuity deficits due to amblyopia and negative stereopsis (shown in blue) had a significant longer saccade latency (P = 0.0315) during amblyopic eye viewing to targets located closer to fixation compared with more peripheral targets.
The comparison among patients within the amblyopia subgroup (mild−stereo+, mild−stereo, severe) showed a significant interaction between Subgroup and Viewing Condition (F 4,22 = 9.34; P < 0.0001, Fig. 2B). Post hoc tests indicated that patients with severe amblyopia had significantly longer saccade latency when viewing with the amblyopic eye (273 ± 36 ms), compared with binocular (165 ± 41 ms) or fellow eye viewing (160 ± 33 ms), and to the other subgroups (i.e., mild amblyopia with and without stereopsis). Patients with mild amblyopia and negative stereopsis also had significantly longer saccade latency during amblyopic eye viewing (187 ± 34 ms), compared with binocular (165 ± 23 ms) or fellow eye viewing (165 ± 35 ms). In contrast, patients with mild amblyopia and gross stereopsis had similar saccade latencies across all viewing conditions (binocular viewing: 200 ± 43 ms; fellow eye viewing: 190 ± 35 ms; amblyopic eye viewing: 205 ± 29 ms). 
The three-way interaction between Amblyopia Subgroup (mild−stereo+, mild−stereo, severe), Viewing Condition, and Target Location was also significant (F 6,22 = 2.88; P = 0.0315; Fig. 3B). Post hoc testing indicated that only patients with severe amblyopia had significantly longer latency during amblyopic eye viewing for saccades to the 5° targets (294 ± 7 ms) compared with the 10° targets (252 ± 21 ms). Target location did not affect saccade latency in patients with amblyopia who had mild acuity deficits with and without stereopsis. 
There was no significant difference among patients with strabismus without amblyopia with and without stereopsis for saccade latency in any viewing condition. There was no relation between the extent of eye deviation and latency in both patient groups. 
Amplitude.
There was a significant main effect of target location for saccade amplitude (F 1,38 = 1255.91; P < 0.0001). Saccades to the 10° target had higher amplitude than those to the 5° target in all experimental conditions for visually normal participants and all patients (Table 2). No other significant main effects or interactions were present for mean saccade amplitude. There were also no differences among patients with amblyopia. The distribution of primary saccade amplitude for each target location across viewing conditions for a few representative participants in each subject group is shown in Supplemental Figure S1 (see Supplementary Material and Supplementary Fig. S1). 
Table 2. 
 
Mean Primary Saccade Amplitude and Peak Velocity
Table 2. 
 
Mean Primary Saccade Amplitude and Peak Velocity
5° Target 10° Target
Binocular Dominant (Fellow) Eye Nondominant (Amblyopic) Eye Binocular Dominant (Fellow) Eye Nondominant (Amblyopic) Eye
Ampl, ° Peak Velocity, deg/s Ampl, ° Peak Velocity, deg/s Ampl, ° Peak Velocity, deg/s Ampl, ° Peak Velocity, deg/s Ampl, ° Peak Velocity, deg/s Ampl, ° Peak Velocity, deg/s
Visually normal 4.87 ± 0.29 239 ± 28 4.67 ± 0.22 229 ± 27 4.78 ± 0.25 231 ± 28 9.45 ± 0.37 349 ± 48 9.31 ± 0.67 328 ± 49 9.35 ± 0.43 338 ± 42
Patients with strabismic amblyopia
 Mild–stereo+ 4.78 ± 0.15 251 ± 29 5.34 ± 1.23 255 ± 45 4.78 ± 0.34 245 ± 28 9.54 ± 0.33 377 ± 27 8.90 ± 1.37 348 ± 29 9.67 ± 0.45 358 ± 45
 Mild–stereo 5.16 ± 0.55 263 ± 64 4.76 ± 0.78 253 ± 70 5.02 ± 0.88 256 ± 87 9.46 ± 0.46 371 ± 91 8.76 ± 1.31 344 ± 81 8.68 ± 1.09 324 ± 98
 Severe–stereo 5.13 ± 0.36 272 ± 53 5.15 ± 0.94 251 ± 34 4.83 ± 0.51 246 ± 68 9.77 ± 0.48 378 ± 74 9.14 ± 0.73 351 ± 46 9.47 ± 0.94 340 ± 65
Patients with strabismus without amblyopia
 Strabismus–stereo+ 4.68 ± 0.13 241 ± 37 4.79 ± 0.40 235 ± 25 4.60 ± 0.33 232 ± 32 9.66 ± 0.71 364 ± 48 9.21 ± 0.46 342 ± 45 9.59 ± 0.77 351 ± 50
 Strabismus–stereo 4.69 ± 0.53 242 ± 29 4.68 ± 0.21 248 ± 50 4.83 ± 0.49 248 ± 42 8.47 ± 1.69 342 ± 56 9.14 ± 0.62 355 ± 32 8.84 ± 1.15 343 ± 25
Saccade amplitude precision (i.e., variability) was not significantly different when patients with amblyopia were compared with visually normal participants and patients with strabismus without amblyopia (F 4,76 = 3.12; P = 0.064). There was a main effect of target location (F 1,38 = 114.08; P < 0.0001); however, interaction between Group and Target Location did not reach significance (F 2,38 = 2.33; P = 0.056). All subjects exhibited larger variability for the 10° target (visually normal: 1.07 ± 0.52°; patients with strabismic amblyopia: 1.32 ± 0.65°; patients with strabismus without amblyopia: 1.50 ± 0.68°) compared with the 5° target (visually normal: 0.68 ± 0.31°; patients with strabismic amblyopia: 0.88 ± 0.48°; patients with strabismus without amblyopia: 0.85 ± 0.42°). 
There was a significant interaction between Amblyopia Subgroup and Viewing Condition (F 4,22 = 6.89; P = 0.0009; Fig. 4A) for saccade amplitude precision. Post hoc testing showed that patients with severe amblyopia had significantly reduced precision of saccade amplitude during amblyopic eye viewing (2.08 ± 0.86°) compared with binocular (1.12 ± 0.52°) or fellow eye viewing (0.71 ± 0.32°). Similarly, patients with mild amblyopia and negative stereopsis had reduced saccade amplitude precision during amblyopic eye viewing (1.62 ± 0.55°) compared with binocular (1.18 ± 0.32°) or fellow eye viewing (1.07 ± 0.46°). In contrast, patients with mild amblyopia and gross stereopsis had similar saccade amplitude precision across viewing conditions (binocular viewing: 0.81 ± 0.60°; fellow eye viewing: 1.18 ± 0.64°; amblyopic eye viewing: 0.77 ± 0.25°), which was comparable to visually normal participants (binocular viewing: 0.94 ± 0.64°; dominant eye viewing: 0.83 ± 0.37°; nondominant eye viewing: 0.94 ± 0.52°) and to patients with strabismus without amblyopia (binocular viewing: 1.27 ± 0.76°; dominant eye viewing: 1.21 ± 0.73°; nondominant eye viewing: 1.16 ± 0.57°). 
Figure 4. 
 
The precision (i.e., variability) of primary saccade amplitude across viewing conditions. (A) Patients with amblyopia and negative stereopsis (both mild [shown in green] and severe [shown in blue] acuity deficits) had significantly reduced primary saccade amplitude precision (i.e., greater SD) during amblyopic eye viewing (P < 0.0001) compared with patients with mild amblyopia and gross stereopsis (shown in red). (B) Only patients with severe amblyopia and negative stereopsis had lower final saccade amplitude precision after executing a secondary saccade, whereas patients with mild acuity deficits attained precision comparable to that of visually normal participants after executing a secondary saccade (P = 0.0002).
Figure 4. 
 
The precision (i.e., variability) of primary saccade amplitude across viewing conditions. (A) Patients with amblyopia and negative stereopsis (both mild [shown in green] and severe [shown in blue] acuity deficits) had significantly reduced primary saccade amplitude precision (i.e., greater SD) during amblyopic eye viewing (P < 0.0001) compared with patients with mild amblyopia and gross stereopsis (shown in red). (B) Only patients with severe amblyopia and negative stereopsis had lower final saccade amplitude precision after executing a secondary saccade, whereas patients with mild acuity deficits attained precision comparable to that of visually normal participants after executing a secondary saccade (P = 0.0002).
There was no significant difference among patients with strabismus without amblyopia with and without stereopsis for saccade amplitude in any viewing condition. There was no relation between the extent of eye deviation and amplitude or precision in both patient groups. 
Peak Velocity.
There was a significant main effect of target location for saccade peak velocity (F 1,38 = 712.87; P < 0.0001). Saccades to the 10° target had higher peak velocity than those to the 5° target in all experimental conditions for visually normal participants and all patients. There was also a main effect of viewing condition (F 2,76 = 4.67; P = 0.012) for peak velocity. Saccades had higher peak velocity during binocular viewing compared with monocular viewing, which was most evident for saccades to the 10° target in all groups, except for patients with strabismus without amblyopia and negative stereopsis (Table 2). 
There was no significant difference among patients with strabismus without amblyopia with and without stereopsis for peak velocity in any viewing condition. There was no relation between the extent of eye deviation and peak velocity in both patient groups. 
Secondary Corrective Saccades
Frequency.
The overall frequency of corrective saccades was greater in patients with strabismic amblyopia (16.3%) and patients with strabismus without amblyopia (15.1%) compared with visually normal participants (13.5%; χ2 [df=2] = 8.61, P = 0.013). However, there was no difference in the frequency of secondary saccades among viewing conditions for patients with strabismic amblyopia (binocular viewing: 6.3%, fellow eye viewing: 5.6%, amblyopic eye viewing: 4.4%), patients with strabismus without amblyopia (binocular viewing: 5.8%; dominant eye viewing: 4.3%; nondominant eye viewing: 4.9%), and visually normal participants (binocular viewing: 4.1%; dominant eye viewing: 4.8%; nondominant eye viewing: 4.6%). 
Subgroup analysis of patients with amblyopia showed a significant frequency difference in corrective saccade frequency (χ2 [df=2] = 49.00, P < 0.001). Specifically, patients with mild amblyopia and gross stereopsis made fewer secondary saccades (3.1%), compared with patients with mild amblyopia and negative stereopsis (6.4%) and patients with severe amblyopia (6.8%). Differences among viewing conditions are shown in Table 3
Table 3. 
 
Frequency and Kinematics of Secondary Saccades for Patients with Strabismic Amblyopia
Table 3. 
 
Frequency and Kinematics of Secondary Saccades for Patients with Strabismic Amblyopia
Mild−Stereo+ (n * = 79) Mild–Stereo (n * = 163) Severe–Stereo (n * = 173)
Binocular Fellow Eye Amblyopic Eye Binocular Fellow Eye Amblyopic Eye Binocular Fellow Eye Amblyopic Eye
Frequency, % 5.0 2.0 2.4 6.5 3.9 7.3 7.5 11.4 3.0
Latency, ms 209 ± 24 225 ± 17 215 ± 19 206 ± 28 213 ± 20 202 ± 25 198 ± 40 192 ± 27 146 ± 29
Amplitude, ° 1.65 ± 0.71 1.27 ± 0.24 1.78 ± 0.59 1.90 ± 0.83 1.78 ± 1.30 2.79 ± 0.96† 1.16 ± 0.60 1.17 ± 0.49 2.64 ± 1.85†
Peak velocity, deg/s 122 ± 43 102 ± 24 131 ± 39 125 ± 43 112 ± 48 161 ± 48† 100 ± 57 95 ± 42 153 ± 97†
Latency.
The latency of corrective saccades was comparable across all viewing conditions for patients with strabismic amblyopia, patients with strabismus without amblyopia, and visually normal participants. The subanalysis of patients with amblyopia also yielded no significant differences. 
Amplitude and Peak Velocity.
There was a significant interaction between Group and Viewing Condition for corrective saccade amplitude (F 4,62 = 4.49; P = 0.003). Post hoc testing indicated that patients with strabismic amblyopia had significantly greater secondary saccade amplitudes during amblyopic eye viewing (2.44 ± 1.14°) compared with binocular (1.58 ± 0.78°) and fellow eye viewing (1.43 ± 0.90°), which were comparable to visually normal participants (binocular viewing: 1.39 ± 0.92°; dominant eye viewing: 1.34 ± 0.61°; nondominant eye viewing: 1.42 ± 0.80°) and to patients with strabismus without amblyopia (binocular viewing: 1.72 ± 0.92°; dominant eye viewing: 1.37 ± 0.66°; nondominant eye viewing: 1.47 ± 0.45°). The interaction between Group and Viewing Condition for corrective saccade peak velocity did not reach significance (F 4,62 = 1.97; P = 0.111). 
Further analysis of patients with amblyopia revealed a main effect of Viewing Condition for secondary saccade amplitude (F 2,11 = 10.03; P = 0.001) and peak velocity (F 2,11 = 4.70; P = 0.024). Data presented in Table 3 indicate that patients with negative stereopsis had significantly larger amplitude and peak velocity of secondary saccades during amblyopic eye viewing compared with binocular and fellow eye viewing. 
Amplitude Variability after Secondary Saccade.
The comparison between patients with amblyopia, patients with strabismus without amblyopia, and visually normal participants was not statistically significant. The mean precision of the final amplitude after the secondary saccade was less than 1° for all groups (range: 0.65–0.98°). In contrast, analysis of patients with amblyopia revealed a main effect of Subgroup (F 2,11 = 13.70; P = 0.001). Post hoc analysis indicated that amplitude variability after secondary saccade was larger in patients with mild amblyopia and negative stereopsis compared with the other groups in all viewing conditions, with the exception of patients with severe amblyopia during amblyopic eye viewing. There was also an interaction between Subgroup and Viewing Condition (F 4,22 = 8.91; P = 0.0002; Fig. 4B). Post hoc analysis indicated that patients with severe amblyopia had significantly lower precision (i.e., increased variability) of the final amplitude after secondary saccade during amblyopic eye viewing (1.33 ± 0.22°) compared with binocular (0.77 ± 0.28°) and fellow eye viewing (0.50 ± 0.16°), as well as with patients with mild amblyopia with and without stereopsis, as illustrated in Figure 4B. 
Discussion
This study examined saccades in patients with strabismic amblyopia and compared their performance to that of patients with strabismus without amblyopia and visually normal participants. The major findings are: (1) patients with strabismic amblyopia and patients with strabismus without amblyopia showed no binocular advantage for saccade initiation; (2) latency and amplitude of primary saccades were affected by the severity of amblyopia and the presence of stereopsis; (3) amblyopic patients without stereopsis initiated secondary saccades more frequently compared with visually normal participants. These secondary saccades improved the final precision of saccade amplitude; however, the precision remained significantly worse in patients with severe amblyopia during amblyopic eye viewing; and (4) these findings were unique to patients with strabismic amblyopia, because they were not observed in patients with strabismus without amblyopia. 
Patients Lack Binocular Advantage for Saccade Initiation
Binocular advantage refers to improvements in performance (e.g., visual acuity, contrast sensitivity, and detection of dim stimuli) during binocular viewing compared with monocular viewing in normal people. 29 It has been proposed that when sensory signals from the eyes are summated, uncorrelated signals (i.e., noise) cancel out and correlated signals are amplified (binocular summation). 29,30 This binocular summation, however, is impaired in patients with amblyopia. Levi and colleagues 31 found no improvement in contrast sensitivity during binocular viewing compared with fellow eye viewing in three patients with strabismic amblyopia and one patient with anisometropic amblyopia. Our findings provide additional evidence that patients with strabismic amblyopia lack binocular advantage in their oculomotor system; their saccade latency was comparable during binocular and fellow eye viewing. 
We also found slightly higher peak velocity during binocular viewing compared with that during monocular viewing, which was evident in all groups, except for patients with strabismus without amblyopia and negative stereopsis. The difference was statistically significant, but it was quite small (∼5% for 10° saccades). Although we cannot explain this effect at present, we will continue to investigate it in the future by examining the saccade main sequence in patients to determine whether viewing binocularly or monocularly affects the saturation velocity of saccades. 
Effects of Severity of Amblyopia and Stereopsis
Although the number of patients with strabismic amblyopia in each subgroup was small in the current study, we found significant differences in saccade latency and precision among patients with different levels of visual acuity and stereoacuity deficits. With respect to acuity deficit, our analysis yielded two important findings. First, saccade latency was prolonged only in patients with a severe acuity deficit (20/200) but not in patients with a mild deficit (≤20/60) during amblyopic eye viewing. These results are in contrast to those in patients with anisometropic amblyopia who had significantly longer saccade latencies during amblyopic eye viewing, irrespective of whether they had a mild or severe acuity deficit, using the same criteria and experimental paradigm. 16,32 Second, patients with strabismic amblyopia and severe acuity deficits experienced more difficulty orienting to targets closer to central fixation (i.e., the 5° vs. 10° target). This is in contrast to patients with anisometropic amblyopia whose saccade latencies were not affected by Target Location. 16,32 Our current and previous results 16,32 can be interpreted as the motor consequences of different long-term sensory suppression mechanisms in strabismic versus anisometropic amblyopia. Suppression of the central field helps to eliminate central diplopia arising from eye misalignment and allows some degree of peripheral fusion. In contrast, the prolonged saccade latency that is independent of Target Location 16,32 in patients with anisometropic amblyopia is consistent with sensory suppression of a blurred image across the entire visual field. 20 Interestingly, we found no correlation between the amount of strabismus and saccade latency for different target locations. A larger sample size is required for a more robust correlation analysis. 
The prolonged saccade latency for a more centrally located target is consistent with stronger sensory suppression of the central visual field in strabismic amblyopia as found in both humans 33 and cats. 34 This pattern of behavior is not likely to be due to interocular suppression because longer latency was found only when patients were viewing with the amblyopic eye and not during binocular viewing. However, it is possible that the chronic suppression of the deviated eye during binocular viewing extends to the monocular viewing conditions. 35 A previous brain imaging study 36 showed a lower level of cortical activation during foveal stimulation of the amblyopic eye, and may explain the longer saccade latency to more centrally located targets. 
A second important finding is that patients with negative stereopsis had significantly reduced precision of primary saccades during amblyopic eye viewing, regardless of the visual acuity deficit. In particular, we found that saccade amplitude precision was significantly worse in patients with strabismic amblyopia and negative stereopsis compared with patients who had gross stereopsis and a similar acuity deficit. It is possible that despite having good acuity in the amblyopic eye, the signal from the amblyopic eye in patients with negative stereopsis may remain under suppression during natural binocular viewing, which also habitually extends to monocular amblyopic eye viewing during the brief experimental period. Our findings are consistent with a recent neuroimaging study 37 on patients with strabismic amblyopia, which showed that the decrease in neural activation in V1/V2 during amblyopic eye stimulation was dependent on the suppressive effects of the fellow eye: activity was more reduced when the fellow eye was open than when it was closed. Taken together, our findings and those of others 37 suggest that the amblyopic eye is under chronic suppression during both monocular and binocular viewing, albeit to a different extent. 
Previous studies have used perceptual and psychophysical tasks to demonstrate spatial localization deficits in amblyopia. 20,38,39 Patients showed a systematic localization bias in the direction of the deviated eye, and exhibited increased spatial uncertainty that was more pronounced in central vision compared with the periphery. Using a saccade task, we provide additional evidence that patients with amblyopia have deficits in spatial localization as shown by increased variability (i.e., reduced precision) in primary saccade amplitude especially during amblyopic eye viewing. We also observed a gradation of effects of strabismic amblyopia: in patients with a severe acuity deficit (and negative stereopsis), both detection (i.e., longer saccade latency) and localization (i.e., increased saccade amplitude variability) deficits were evident. In patients with mild amblyopia and negative stereopsis, a localization deficit was evident but not a detection deficit (i.e., normal saccade latency), whereas in those with mild amblyopia and gross stereopsis, no localization or detection deficit was present. The distinct pattern found in patients with mild amblyopia and negative stereopsis could be explained by a speed–accuracy tradeoff: saccades were initiated with normal latency but with a greater than normal scatter of landing positions. This behavior might have led to a large inaccuracy; however, the errors in primary saccade amplitude were corrected by secondary saccades as discussed in the following text. 
Secondary Saccades
Primary saccades have a tendency to undershoot the target by approximately 10%. 40,41 Secondary saccades are initiated to correct the amplitude error remaining after the primary saccade. Two sources of error feedback have been proposed for the generation of secondary saccades. 40,42,43 One is based on extraretinal information derived from the efference copy of the oculomotor command. Another is based on retinal feedback derived from the position of the target image on the retina at the end of the primary saccade. 
Our data show two important findings. First, only patients with amblyopia (mild and severe) and negative stereopsis initiated secondary saccades more frequently. Second, saccades initiated by these patients with negative stereopsis during amblyopic eye viewing had higher amplitudes and peak velocities. Since we also found that these patients had lower primary saccade amplitude precision, these secondary saccades were initiated to correct the amplitude error remaining after the primary saccade. Patients with mild amblyopia were able to correct the error significantly more compared with patients with severe amblyopia, as indicated by improved final amplitude precision after the secondary saccade. These results can be interpreted by considering that the retinal feedback is more impaired in patients with severe amblyopia. Consequently, patients with less reliable retinal position error signals may need a larger visual error signal before initiating a secondary saccade, which in turn leads to higher amplitude and, thus, peak velocity of the corrective eye movement. In addition, our data indicate that the error detection process is impaired in patients with severe amblyopia because they made significantly fewer secondary saccades during amblyopic eye viewing. Our findings highlight the importance of a reliable retinal feedback signal for the initiation of secondary corrective saccades. 
In conclusion, our study adds to the growing body of literature that recognizes amblyopia as a heterogeneous disorder of both visual and visuomotor functions. 13,4447 We found a distinct pattern of deficits in patients with strabismic amblyopia that, unlike that in anisometropic amblyopia, was dependent on the level of visual acuity and stereoacuity losses. Gross stereopsis was associated with better saccade performance in terms of detection and localization, regardless of viewing condition. In addition, these deficits were unique to patients with strabismic amblyopia, because they were not found in patients with strabismus without amblyopia. Our results suggest that the evaluation of the effectiveness of treatment regimens for amblyopia should also consider motor improvement, rather than solely on visual acuity. 
Supplementary Materials
References
Daw NW. Visual Development. 2nd ed. New York: Springer; 2006:145–164.
Hensch TK. Critical period plasticity in local cortical circuits. Nat Rev Neurosci . 2005;6:877–888. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Sireteanu R Baumer CC Sarbu C Iftime A. Spatial and temporal misperceptions in amblyopic vision. Strabismus . 2007;15:45–54. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Simmers AJ Ledgeway T Mansouri B Hutchinson CV Hess RF. The extent of the dorsal extra-striate deficit in amblyopia. Vision Res . 2006;46:2571–2580. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Simmers AJ Ledgeway T Hess RF McGraw PV. Deficits to global motion processing in human amblyopia. Vision Res . 2003;43:729–738. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Holopigian K Blake R Greenwald MJ. Selective losses in binocular vision in anisometropic amblyopes. Vision Res . 1986;26:621–630. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Ho CS Giaschi DE Boden C Dougherty R Cline R Lyons C. Deficient motion perception in the fellow eye of amblyopic children. Vision Res . 2005;45:1615–1627. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Hess RF Wang YZ Demanins R Wilkinson F Wilson HR. A deficit in strabismic amblyopia for global shape detection. Vision Res . 1999;39:901–914. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Bonneh YS Sagi D Polat U. Spatial and temporal crowding in amblyopia. Vision Res . 2007;47:1950–1962. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Barrett BT Pacey IE Bradley A Thibos LN Morrill P. Nonveridical visual perception in human amblyopia. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci . 2003;44:1555–1567. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Woo GC Irving EL. The non-amblyopic eye of a unilateral amblyope: a unique entity. Clin Exp Optom . 1991;74:1–5. [CrossRef]
Mirabella G Hay S Wong AM. Deficits in perception of real-world scenes in patients with a history of amblyopia. Arch Ophthalmol . 2011;129:176–183. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
McKee SP Levi DM Movshon JA. The pattern of visual deficits in amblyopia. J Vis . 2003;3:380–405. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Irving DE. Integrating information across saccadic eye movements. Curr Direct Psychol Sci . 1996;5:94–100. [CrossRef]
Prime SL Vesia M Crawford JD. Cortical mechanisms for trans-saccadic memory and integration of multiple object features. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci . 2011;366:540–553. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Niechwiej-Szwedo E Goltz HC Chandrakumar M Hirji ZA Wong AM. Effects of anisometropic amblyopia on visuomotor behavior, I: saccadic eye movements. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci . 2010;51:6348–6354. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Niechwiej-Szwedo E Goltz HC Chandrakumar M Wong AM. The effect of sensory uncertainty due to amblyopia (lazy eye) on the planning and execution of visually-guided 3D reaching movements. PLoS ONE . 2012;7:e31075. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Niechwiej-Szwedo E Goltz HC Chandrakumar M Hirji Z Crawford JD Wong AM. Effects of anisometropic amblyopia on visuomotor behavior, part 2: visually-guided reaching. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci . 2011;52:795–803. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Niechwiej-Szwedo E Goltz HC Chandrakumar M Hirji Z Wong AM. Effects of anisometropic amblyopia on visuomotor behavior, III: temporal eye–hand coordination during reaching. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci . 2011;52:5853–5861. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Hess RF Holliday IE. The spatial localization deficit in amblyopia. Vision Res . 1992;32:1319–1339. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Schor C. A directional impairment of eye movement control in strabismus amblyopia. Invest Ophthalmol . 1975;14:692–697. [PubMed]
Ciuffreda KJ Kenyon RV Stark L. Increased saccadic latencies in amblyopic eyes. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci . 1978;17:697–702. [PubMed]
Bucci MP Kapoula Z Yang Q Bremond-Gignac D. Latency of saccades, vergence, and combined movements in children with early onset convergent or divergent strabismus. Vision Res . 2006;46:1384–1392. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Kapoula Z Bucci P. Distribution-dependent saccades in children with strabismus and in normals. Exp Brain Res . 2002;143:264–268. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Kapoula Z Bucci MP Eggert T Garraud L. Impairment of the binocular coordination of saccades in strabismus. Vision Res . 1997;37:2757–2766. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Sainburg RL. Handedness: differential specializations for control of trajectory and position. Exerc Sport Sci Rev . 2005;33:206–213. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Dolman P. Tests for determining sighting of the eye. Am J Ophthalmol . 1919;2:287. [CrossRef]
Conover WJ Iman RL. Analysis of covariance using the rank transformation. Biometrics . 1982;38:715–724. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Howard IP. Basic Mechanisms. Seeing in Depth . Toronto: University of Toronto Press; 2002:319.
Campbell FW Green DG. Monocular versus binocular visual acuity. Nature . 1965;208:191–192. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Levi DM Harwerth RS Manny RE. Suprathreshold spatial frequency detection and binocular interaction in strabismic and anisometropic amblyopia. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci . 1979;18:714–725. [PubMed]
Niechwiej-Szwedo E Goltz HC Wong AM. Deficits and adaptation of eye–hand coordination during visually-guided reaching movements in people with amblyopia. In: Steeves J Harris LR eds. Plasticity in Sensory Systems . Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; In press:3–27.
Sireteanu R Fronius M. Naso-temporal asymmetries in human amblyopia consequence of long-term interocular suppression. Vision Res . 1981;21:1055–1063. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Harrad R Sengpiel F Blakemore C. Physiology of suppression in strabismic amblyopia. Br J Ophthalmol . 1996;80:373–377. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Li J Thompson B Lam CS The role of suppression in amblyopia. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci . 2011;52:4169–4176. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Conner IP Odom JV Schwartz TL Mendola JD. Retinotopic maps and foveal suppression in the visual cortex of amblyopic adults. J Physiol . 2007;583:159–173. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Farivar R Thompson B Mansouri B Hess RF. Interocular suppression in strabismic amblyopia results in an attenuated and delayed hemodynamic response function in early visual cortex. J Vis . 2011;11:1–12. [CrossRef]
Bedell HE Flom MC Barbeito R. Spatial aberrations and acuity in strabismus and amblyopia. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci . 1985;26:909–916. [PubMed]
Fronius M Sireteanu R. Monocular geometry is selectively distorted in the central visual field of strabismic amblyopes. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci . 1989;30:2034–2044. [PubMed]
Becker W Fuchs AF. Further properties of the human saccadic system: eye movements and correction saccades with and without visual fixation points. Vision Res . 1969;9:1247–1258. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Henson DB. Corrective saccades: effects of altering visual feedback. Vision Res . 1978;18:63–67. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Prablanc C Masse D Echallier JF. Error-correcting mechanisms in large saccades. Vision Res . 1978;18:557–560. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Weber RB Daroff RB. Corrective movements following refixation saccades: type and control system analysis. Vision Res . 1972;12:467–475. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Grant S Melmoth DR Morgan MJ Finlay AL. Prehension deficits in amblyopia. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci . 2007;48:1139–1148. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Grant S Moseley MJ. Amblyopia and real-world visuomotor tasks. Strabismus . 2011;19:119–128. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Ho CS Giaschi DE. Stereopsis-dependent deficits in maximum motion displacement in strabismic and anisometropic amblyopia. Vision Res . 2007;47:2778–2785. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Agrawal R Conner IP Odom JV Schwartz TL Mendola JD. Relating binocular and monocular vision in strabismic and anisometropic amblyopia. Arch Ophthalmol . 2006;124:844–850. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Footnotes
 Supported by Canadian Institutes of Health Research Grant MOP 106663, a Leaders Opportunity Fund grant from the Canadian Foundation for Innovation, and a grant from the Department of Ophthalmology and Vision Sciences at The Hospital for Sick Children.
Footnotes
 Disclosure: E. Niechwiej-Szwedo, None; M. Chandrakumar, None; H.C. Goltz, None; A.M.F. Wong, None
Figure 1. 
 
Representative eye velocity tracings from individual trials during monocular viewing with the nondominant/amblyopic eye (left column), monocular viewing with the dominant/fellow eye (middle column), and binocular viewing (right column) when the target was shown 10° to the right. Top row: a visually normal participant; second row: a patient with mild amblyopia and gross stereopsis (200 seconds of arc) (Table 1, ID 4); third row: a patient with mild amblyopia and negative stereopsis (Table 1, ID 6); fourth row: a patient with severe amblyopia and negative stereopsis (Table 1, ID 14); fifth row: a patient with strabismus and stereopsis (80 seconds of arc) (Table 1, ID 16); last row: a patient with strabismus without amblyopia and negative stereopsis (Table 1, ID 21).
Figure 1. 
 
Representative eye velocity tracings from individual trials during monocular viewing with the nondominant/amblyopic eye (left column), monocular viewing with the dominant/fellow eye (middle column), and binocular viewing (right column) when the target was shown 10° to the right. Top row: a visually normal participant; second row: a patient with mild amblyopia and gross stereopsis (200 seconds of arc) (Table 1, ID 4); third row: a patient with mild amblyopia and negative stereopsis (Table 1, ID 6); fourth row: a patient with severe amblyopia and negative stereopsis (Table 1, ID 14); fifth row: a patient with strabismus and stereopsis (80 seconds of arc) (Table 1, ID 16); last row: a patient with strabismus without amblyopia and negative stereopsis (Table 1, ID 21).
Figure 2. 
 
Mean saccade latencies as a function of viewing condition. (A) Latencies were significantly longer for patients with strabismic amblyopia (shown in red) during amblyopic eye viewing (P < 0.0001). Visually normal participants (shown in black) had shorter saccade latencies during binocular viewing compared with monocular viewing. In contrast, all patients had comparable saccade latency during binocular and fellow (nondeviated) eye viewing (i.e., no binocular advantage). Patients with strabismus without amblyopia (shown in green) had similar saccade latency in all viewing conditions. (B) Saccade latencies were significantly longer only for patients with severe amblyopia (blue lines, visual acuity 20/200, negative stereopsis) during amblyopic eye viewing (P < 0.0001).
Figure 2. 
 
Mean saccade latencies as a function of viewing condition. (A) Latencies were significantly longer for patients with strabismic amblyopia (shown in red) during amblyopic eye viewing (P < 0.0001). Visually normal participants (shown in black) had shorter saccade latencies during binocular viewing compared with monocular viewing. In contrast, all patients had comparable saccade latency during binocular and fellow (nondeviated) eye viewing (i.e., no binocular advantage). Patients with strabismus without amblyopia (shown in green) had similar saccade latency in all viewing conditions. (B) Saccade latencies were significantly longer only for patients with severe amblyopia (blue lines, visual acuity 20/200, negative stereopsis) during amblyopic eye viewing (P < 0.0001).
Figure 3. 
 
Mean saccade latency as a function of target eccentricity across viewing conditions. (A) Patients with strabismic amblyopia (shown in red) had longer saccade latency during amblyopic eye viewing for the 5° target compared with the 10° target (P = 0.0002). (B) Further subgroup analysis revealed that only patients with severe visual acuity deficits due to amblyopia and negative stereopsis (shown in blue) had a significant longer saccade latency (P = 0.0315) during amblyopic eye viewing to targets located closer to fixation compared with more peripheral targets.
Figure 3. 
 
Mean saccade latency as a function of target eccentricity across viewing conditions. (A) Patients with strabismic amblyopia (shown in red) had longer saccade latency during amblyopic eye viewing for the 5° target compared with the 10° target (P = 0.0002). (B) Further subgroup analysis revealed that only patients with severe visual acuity deficits due to amblyopia and negative stereopsis (shown in blue) had a significant longer saccade latency (P = 0.0315) during amblyopic eye viewing to targets located closer to fixation compared with more peripheral targets.
Figure 4. 
 
The precision (i.e., variability) of primary saccade amplitude across viewing conditions. (A) Patients with amblyopia and negative stereopsis (both mild [shown in green] and severe [shown in blue] acuity deficits) had significantly reduced primary saccade amplitude precision (i.e., greater SD) during amblyopic eye viewing (P < 0.0001) compared with patients with mild amblyopia and gross stereopsis (shown in red). (B) Only patients with severe amblyopia and negative stereopsis had lower final saccade amplitude precision after executing a secondary saccade, whereas patients with mild acuity deficits attained precision comparable to that of visually normal participants after executing a secondary saccade (P = 0.0002).
Figure 4. 
 
The precision (i.e., variability) of primary saccade amplitude across viewing conditions. (A) Patients with amblyopia and negative stereopsis (both mild [shown in green] and severe [shown in blue] acuity deficits) had significantly reduced primary saccade amplitude precision (i.e., greater SD) during amblyopic eye viewing (P < 0.0001) compared with patients with mild amblyopia and gross stereopsis (shown in red). (B) Only patients with severe amblyopia and negative stereopsis had lower final saccade amplitude precision after executing a secondary saccade, whereas patients with mild acuity deficits attained precision comparable to that of visually normal participants after executing a secondary saccade (P = 0.0002).
Table 1. 
 
Characteristics of Patients with Strabismic Amblyopia and Strabismus without Amblyopia
Table 1. 
 
Characteristics of Patients with Strabismic Amblyopia and Strabismus without Amblyopia
ID Age, y Sex Snellen Visual Acuity (logMAR) Refractive Error Deviation on Simultaneous Prism Cover Test (Alternate Prism Test) Stereoacuity (seconds of arc) Fusion Notes
Right Left Right Left Eye Near (PD) Distance (PD)
Patients with strabismic amblyopia
1 27 F 20/15 (−0.10) 20/40 (0.30) −1.00+0.50 −1.00+0.50 Left ET8 (ET+E10) ET2 (ET+E4) 400
2 37 F 20/40 (0.30) 20/20 (0.00) −4.00 −4.00 Right ET4 (ET+E6) ET4 800
3 19 F 20/20 (0.00) 20/40 (0.30) −2.75 −3.00 Left ET2 (ET+E14) ET2 400 Hx occlusion
4 29 M 20/30 (0.18) 20/20 (0.00) −2.25+3.00×110 −2.50+2.25×75 Right XT2 (XT+X40) XT2 (XT+X45) 200
5 28 F 20/20 (0.00) 20/30 (0.18) +0.75+0.25×45 +0.25+0.75×95 Left ET1 ET2 120 Hx occlusion
6 38 M 20/30 (0.18) 20/15 (−0.10) −3.50 −3.50+0.50×25 Right ET35, hypo4 ET35, hypo6 Negative Suppress Hx occlusion
7 19 M 20/50 (0.40) 20/20 (0.00) Plano Plano Right ET16 ET14 Negative Suppress Hx occlusion
8 30 M 20/15 (−0.10) 20/30 (0.18) Plano Plano Left ET35 ET35 Negative Suppress IET
9 51 F 20/50 (0.40) 20/20 (0.00) +1.75+2.00×74 +2.50+1.25×90 Right ET14, hypo14 ET8, hypo12 Negative Suppress IET, Hx surgery/ occlusion
10 21 F 20/20 (0.00) 20/30 (0.18) +1.50+1.50×65 +1.25+1.00×110 Left ET2, hypo5 bilateral DVD ET6, hypo5 bilateral DVD Negative W4D IET, Hx surgery/ occlusion
11 25 M 20/20 (0.00) 20/200 (1.00) −0.75+0.50×90 −0.25 Left ET12 ET10 Negative Suppress Non-central fixation, Hx occlusion
12 37 M 20/20 (0.00) 20/200 (1.00) Plano Plano Left XT 4 (XT+X8) XT 4 (XT+X6) Negative Non-central fixation
13 48 M 20/15 (−0.10) 20/200 (1.00) Plano Plano Left XT40 XT30 Negative Suppress Non-central fixation, Hx surgery/ occlusion
14 35 M 20/20 (0.00) 20/200 (1.00) −0.75 Plano Left XT59, HT12 XT40, HT10 Negative Suppress Non-central fixation, Hx surgery/ occlusion
Patients with strabismus without amblyopia
15 26 F 20/15 (−0.10) 20/20 (0.00) +1.00+0.25×35 +2.00 Left ET 2 (ET+E4) ET 2 (ET+E4) 50 Hx occlusion
16 21 M 20/15 (−0.10) 20/15 (−0.10) Plano Plano Right X4, H2 X4, H2 80 Hx surgery
17 31 F 20/20 (0.00) 20/25 (0.10) Plano Plano Right ET4 (ET+E12) ET4 (ET+E10) 3000 Hx occlusion
18 27 M 20/20 (0.00) 20/20 (0.00) Plano Plano Right ET2, HT3 (ET+E4) ET2, HT3 (ET+E4) 3000 Hx occlusion/ surgery
19 29 F 20/15 (−0.10) 20/20 (0.00) Plano −2.00+0.50×170 Right XT16, hypo2 XT14, HT3 Negative Suppress Hx surgery
20 33 F 20/15 (−0.10) 20/15 (−0.10) Plano Plano Left AET 50 ET4 (ET+E25) Negative Suppress
21 33 M 20/15 (−0.10) 20/15 (−0.10) −1.50 Plano Left ET20, bilateral HT ET20, bilateral HT Negative W4D ARC, Hx surgery
22 32 F 20/20 (0.00) 20/20 (0.00) −2.50+0.50×175 −4.00+1.25×115 Right ET20, HT3 ET18, HT3 Negative Peripheral fusion on Bagolini ARC
23 31 M 20/15 (−0.10) 20/15 (−0.10) −7.50+3.00×80 −4.25+2.00×95 Left AET45, HT18 R/AET40, HT16 Negative Suppress Hx occlusion/ surgery
24 34 M 20/15 (−0.10) 20/15 (−0.10) +1.50+0.50×94 pl+1.00×65 Right AET18 ET12 Negative Suppress Occlusion, surgery
25 34 F 20/15 (−0.10) 20/25 (0.10) +2.75 +4.00+1.00×115 Left ET20 ET25 Negative Int. fusion on W4D fusion
26 21 M 20/15 (−0.10) 20/15 (−0.10) +3.50+1.25×110 +3.00+1.00×95 Right ET25, hypo6 ET14, hypo6 Negative Suppress IET
27 25 M 20/15 (−0.10) 20/20 (0.00) −1.00+0.50×75 −0.75+0.25×130 Left XT4, HT4 XT25, hypo5 Negative Suppress Occlusion
Table 2. 
 
Mean Primary Saccade Amplitude and Peak Velocity
Table 2. 
 
Mean Primary Saccade Amplitude and Peak Velocity
5° Target 10° Target
Binocular Dominant (Fellow) Eye Nondominant (Amblyopic) Eye Binocular Dominant (Fellow) Eye Nondominant (Amblyopic) Eye
Ampl, ° Peak Velocity, deg/s Ampl, ° Peak Velocity, deg/s Ampl, ° Peak Velocity, deg/s Ampl, ° Peak Velocity, deg/s Ampl, ° Peak Velocity, deg/s Ampl, ° Peak Velocity, deg/s
Visually normal 4.87 ± 0.29 239 ± 28 4.67 ± 0.22 229 ± 27 4.78 ± 0.25 231 ± 28 9.45 ± 0.37 349 ± 48 9.31 ± 0.67 328 ± 49 9.35 ± 0.43 338 ± 42
Patients with strabismic amblyopia
 Mild–stereo+ 4.78 ± 0.15 251 ± 29 5.34 ± 1.23 255 ± 45 4.78 ± 0.34 245 ± 28 9.54 ± 0.33 377 ± 27 8.90 ± 1.37 348 ± 29 9.67 ± 0.45 358 ± 45
 Mild–stereo 5.16 ± 0.55 263 ± 64 4.76 ± 0.78 253 ± 70 5.02 ± 0.88 256 ± 87 9.46 ± 0.46 371 ± 91 8.76 ± 1.31 344 ± 81 8.68 ± 1.09 324 ± 98
 Severe–stereo 5.13 ± 0.36 272 ± 53 5.15 ± 0.94 251 ± 34 4.83 ± 0.51 246 ± 68 9.77 ± 0.48 378 ± 74 9.14 ± 0.73 351 ± 46 9.47 ± 0.94 340 ± 65
Patients with strabismus without amblyopia
 Strabismus–stereo+ 4.68 ± 0.13 241 ± 37 4.79 ± 0.40 235 ± 25 4.60 ± 0.33 232 ± 32 9.66 ± 0.71 364 ± 48 9.21 ± 0.46 342 ± 45 9.59 ± 0.77 351 ± 50
 Strabismus–stereo 4.69 ± 0.53 242 ± 29 4.68 ± 0.21 248 ± 50 4.83 ± 0.49 248 ± 42 8.47 ± 1.69 342 ± 56 9.14 ± 0.62 355 ± 32 8.84 ± 1.15 343 ± 25
Table 3. 
 
Frequency and Kinematics of Secondary Saccades for Patients with Strabismic Amblyopia
Table 3. 
 
Frequency and Kinematics of Secondary Saccades for Patients with Strabismic Amblyopia
Mild−Stereo+ (n * = 79) Mild–Stereo (n * = 163) Severe–Stereo (n * = 173)
Binocular Fellow Eye Amblyopic Eye Binocular Fellow Eye Amblyopic Eye Binocular Fellow Eye Amblyopic Eye
Frequency, % 5.0 2.0 2.4 6.5 3.9 7.3 7.5 11.4 3.0
Latency, ms 209 ± 24 225 ± 17 215 ± 19 206 ± 28 213 ± 20 202 ± 25 198 ± 40 192 ± 27 146 ± 29
Amplitude, ° 1.65 ± 0.71 1.27 ± 0.24 1.78 ± 0.59 1.90 ± 0.83 1.78 ± 1.30 2.79 ± 0.96† 1.16 ± 0.60 1.17 ± 0.49 2.64 ± 1.85†
Peak velocity, deg/s 122 ± 43 102 ± 24 131 ± 39 125 ± 43 112 ± 48 161 ± 48† 100 ± 57 95 ± 42 153 ± 97†
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×