Although the number of patients with strabismic amblyopia in each subgroup was small in the current study, we found significant differences in saccade latency and precision among patients with different levels of visual acuity and stereoacuity deficits. With respect to acuity deficit, our analysis yielded two important findings. First, saccade latency was prolonged only in patients with a severe acuity deficit (20/200) but not in patients with a mild deficit (≤20/60) during amblyopic eye viewing. These results are in contrast to those in patients with anisometropic amblyopia who had significantly longer saccade latencies during amblyopic eye viewing, irrespective of whether they had a mild or severe acuity deficit, using the same criteria and experimental paradigm.
16,32 Second, patients with strabismic amblyopia and severe acuity deficits experienced more difficulty orienting to targets closer to central fixation (i.e., the 5° vs. 10° target). This is in contrast to patients with anisometropic amblyopia whose saccade latencies were not affected by Target Location.
16,32 Our current and previous results
16,32 can be interpreted as the motor consequences of different long-term sensory suppression mechanisms in strabismic versus anisometropic amblyopia. Suppression of the central field helps to eliminate central diplopia arising from eye misalignment and allows some degree of peripheral fusion. In contrast, the prolonged saccade latency that is independent of Target Location
16,32 in patients with anisometropic amblyopia is consistent with sensory suppression of a blurred image across the entire visual field.
20 Interestingly, we found no correlation between the amount of strabismus and saccade latency for different target locations. A larger sample size is required for a more robust correlation analysis.
The prolonged saccade latency for a more centrally located target is consistent with stronger sensory suppression of the central visual field in strabismic amblyopia as found in both humans
33 and cats.
34 This pattern of behavior is not likely to be due to interocular suppression because longer latency was found only when patients were viewing with the amblyopic eye and not during binocular viewing. However, it is possible that the chronic suppression of the deviated eye during binocular viewing extends to the monocular viewing conditions.
35 A previous brain imaging study
36 showed a lower level of cortical activation during foveal stimulation of the amblyopic eye, and may explain the longer saccade latency to more centrally located targets.
A second important finding is that patients with negative stereopsis had significantly reduced precision of primary saccades during amblyopic eye viewing, regardless of the visual acuity deficit. In particular, we found that saccade amplitude precision was significantly worse in patients with strabismic amblyopia and negative stereopsis compared with patients who had gross stereopsis and a similar acuity deficit. It is possible that despite having good acuity in the amblyopic eye, the signal from the amblyopic eye in patients with negative stereopsis may remain under suppression during natural binocular viewing, which also habitually extends to monocular amblyopic eye viewing during the brief experimental period. Our findings are consistent with a recent neuroimaging study
37 on patients with strabismic amblyopia, which showed that the decrease in neural activation in V1/V2 during amblyopic eye stimulation was dependent on the suppressive effects of the fellow eye: activity was more reduced when the fellow eye was open than when it was closed. Taken together, our findings and those of others
37 suggest that the amblyopic eye is under chronic suppression during both monocular and binocular viewing, albeit to a different extent.
Previous studies have used perceptual and psychophysical tasks to demonstrate spatial localization deficits in amblyopia.
20,38,39 Patients showed a systematic localization bias in the direction of the deviated eye, and exhibited increased spatial uncertainty that was more pronounced in central vision compared with the periphery. Using a saccade task, we provide additional evidence that patients with amblyopia have deficits in spatial localization as shown by increased variability (i.e., reduced precision) in primary saccade amplitude especially during amblyopic eye viewing. We also observed a gradation of effects of strabismic amblyopia: in patients with a severe acuity deficit (and negative stereopsis), both detection (i.e., longer saccade latency) and localization (i.e., increased saccade amplitude variability) deficits were evident. In patients with mild amblyopia and negative stereopsis, a localization deficit was evident but not a detection deficit (i.e., normal saccade latency), whereas in those with mild amblyopia and gross stereopsis, no localization or detection deficit was present. The distinct pattern found in patients with mild amblyopia and negative stereopsis could be explained by a speed–accuracy tradeoff: saccades were initiated with normal latency but with a greater than normal scatter of landing positions. This behavior might have led to a large inaccuracy; however, the errors in primary saccade amplitude were corrected by secondary saccades as discussed in the following text.