Despite a relatively small sample size, we were able to measure significant differences in lane position between drivers with NV and those with HH that provided support for our blind-side safety-margin hypothesis. The effects varied with road segment type, emphasizing the importance of evaluating lane position and steering for straight road segments, right and left curves, and right and left turns separately (especially when investigating a population with a lateralized field loss). Our predictions and the corresponding outcomes are summarized in
Table 4.
In agreement with our main hypothesis, the drivers with RHH held a more leftward lane position (away from the blind side) than did the drivers with NV, who held a relatively central lane position (averaged across all drives and nonturn segments). This difference was particularly apparent on straight road segments. By taking a more leftward lane position, the drivers with RHH provided an apparent safety margin on the blind right side, but increased the danger of crossing into a traffic lane on the left (they were driving on the right on undivided roads). Presumably, the perceived threat was greater from objects on the blind right side than from objects on the seeing left side. This finding is further emphasized by the lack of rightward shift for the RHH group in the presence of oncoming traffic on the undivided highway.
Conversely, most drivers with LHH did not take up a lane position farther away from the blind left side than that taken by the drivers with NV. Given that the greatest threat to the LHH group would be from traffic in neighboring lanes on the blind left side, their behavior may appear inconsistent with that of the drivers with RHH. However, it is important to note that in both city and rural drives, the average lane position of the participants with LHH was to the right of the lane center (by 0.11 m on city drives and 0.37 m on rural highways). Drivers with NV took a similar rightward lane position, and thus no difference was found between these two groups. Furthermore, the entire LHH group shifted to the right (toward the edge of the road by 0.74 m) in the presence of oncoming traffic on the undivided rural highway. All the drivers with NV also moved to the right by approximately 0.50 m (on average) in response to oncoming traffic, similar in magnitude to the rightward shift in an on-road study.
20 This result strongly suggests that the oncoming traffic was perceived as a threat and the response was commensurate with the corresponding risk in the real world.
All the drivers with NV cut curves as expected. However, the curve-cutting behavior of the drivers with HH was modified to provide a safety margin when the curve opened into the blind hemifield. This result is particularly noticeable on rural highway curves for drivers with RHH. On right curves, which opened into the blind hemifield, they cut the curve to a lesser extent than the drivers with NV (i.e., took a more leftward path providing a safety margin on their blind right side). By comparison, on left curves that opened into their seeing left field, they took a path more similar to that of the drivers with NV.
In agreement with our predictions, on right turns, the drivers with RHH took a significantly more rightward path than did the drivers with NV (bringing the risk zone of the oncoming traffic or the lane boundary more quickly from the blind right field into the left seeing field), whereas the drivers with LHH took a path similar to that of the drivers with NV (keeping the risk zone in the seeing right field for as long as possible;
Fig. 5). On left turns, the drivers with RHH took a path similar to that of the drivers with NV (the risk zone was in their seeing left field for the whole turn), but the drivers with LHH took a more rightward path than did the drivers with NV, especially on left 4-to-4 turns, where there was a lane available to the right of the travel lane. This result is consistent with providing a safety margin on the blind left side, especially at the end of the turn arc when the driver must avoid entering the oncoming traffic lane on the blind side.
In terms of steering control, the HH drivers had more variable lane position than did the NV drivers, but steering wheel reversal rates were similar. This observation suggests that the steering of the drivers with HH was less stable than that of the NV drivers, but that the overall steering effort was similar. As expected, steering reversal rates of both the HH and NV drivers increased in response to greater steering demands (driving at high speed and on curves).
15 Although the overall difference in lateral lane position variability between the drivers with HH and those with NV vision appears relatively small in magnitude (average across all segments: 0.27 and 0.20 m, respectively), the greater variability combined with tendencies to hold a position away from the center of the lane, resulted in the drivers with HH being out of lane more often than the drivers with NV. Consistent with our safety margin hypothesis, a
greater proportion of the drivers with RHH were out of lane to the
left on straight segments but a
lower proportion were out of lane to the
right on right curves, whereas a greater proportion of the drivers with LHH were out of lane to the right on straight segments. As the oncoming traffic lane (or passing traffic lane) was on the left, it is lane boundary crossings to the left that may be considered more crucial. Similarly, a simulator study
4 and on-road studies
2,3 have reported more variable lane position and more lane boundary crossings for HH than for NV drivers.
Despite the wide age range, we found no effect of age on lane position or steering measures for the drivers with HH and only a small effect of age on lane position variability for the drivers with NV. This result is in stark contrast to the strong correlation between age and blind-side detection rate reported in our previous paper
1 (blind-side detection rates decreased as age increased). Taken together, these results suggest that age had little impact on adaptation to HH in overly learned tasks such as steering and lane position control, but that the older drivers compensated less well than the younger in a task that required active scanning to the blind side.
Although no simulator can fully replicate the force feedback experienced when driving a real car, the steering of our simulator did provide force feedback that varied with vehicle speed. Furthermore, the steering characteristics were found to be more realistic than those of other simulators tested by the authors. Any steering limitations would have affected all the drivers equally. Our primary interest was in
relative differences in lane position and steering between the drivers with NV and HH rather than absolute values. In a few studies, lane position and steering of NV drivers has been compared on the road and in a simulator. The findings with respect to absolute lane position varied. In one study,
17 the drivers tended to take a lane position closer to the edge of the road in real life than in the simulator, whereas in another, the opposite was reported.
21 Although
absolute lane position may vary between simulator and on-road driving
, relative differences in response to changes in road curvature, differences in driving experience, or engaging in secondary tasks, are replicated.
5,17,21 We would therefore expect that the relative differences in lateral lane position that we found between the vision groups in the simulator would also hold true for on-road driving. For example, we would predict that drivers with RHH would generally have a more leftward lane position than drivers with NV. However, that prediction is at odds with the observations from the on-road study by Tant et al.,
2 in which 4 of 13 drivers with RHH were noted to drive too close to the right side of the road. As lateral lane position was not measured by Tant et al.,
2 biases in lateral lane position that could be measured in the simulator may not have been observed by the driving examiner. Our findings relate to driving on the right side of the road. For driving on the left, we would simply expect a reversal of the roles and results between drivers with RHH and LHH.
On the basis of the steering and lane position data, most of the participants with HH in this study may appear fit to drive; however, as reported in our previous paper, most had detection rates for pedestrians on the blind side that were so low as to seem incompatible with safe driving.
1 Whereas problems with steering, such as incursions into the next travel lane, are easily detected in an on-road test, problems with detection are noted only when a detection failure puts the driver or other road-users at risk (the number of detection failures in nonrisky situations will never be known to the experimenter/evaluator). Hence, although the results of recent on-road studies
2,3 may suggest that problems with steering and lane position are the most common reasons for drivers with HH failing on-road tests, our driving simulator evaluation suggests that detection may be more of a problem than steering control.
Supported by National Institutes of Health Grants EY12890 (EP) and EY018680 (ARB).
The authors thank Kent Higgins, Laurel Bobrow, Vincent Ciaccio, Matthew Bronstad, and Amanda Albu for help in developing the lane-position analyses and Joseph Rizzo (Center for Innovative Visual Rehabilitation at the Boston VA Healthcare System, Jamaica Plain Campus) for providing access to the driving simulator.