April 2011
Volume 52, Issue 5
Free
Low Vision  |   April 2011
Effect of Bilateral Macular Scotomas from Age-Related Macular Degeneration on Reach-to-Grasp Hand Movement
Author Affiliations & Notes
  • George T. Timberlake
    From the Kansas City Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Kansas City, Missouri; and
    the Department of Ophthalmology and
  • Evanthia Omoscharka
    From the Kansas City Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Kansas City, Missouri; and
  • Barbara M. Quaney
    From the Kansas City Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Kansas City, Missouri; and
    the Center on Aging, University of Kansas Medical Center, Kansas City, Kansas.
  • Susan A. Grose
    From the Kansas City Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Kansas City, Missouri; and
  • Joseph H. Maino
    From the Kansas City Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Kansas City, Missouri; and
  • Corresponding author: George T. Timberlake, Department of Ophthalmology, University of Kansas Medical Center, 7400 State Line Road, Prairie Village, KS 66208; [email protected]
Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science April 2011, Vol.52, 2540-2550. doi:https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.10-6062
  • Views
  • PDF
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      George T. Timberlake, Evanthia Omoscharka, Barbara M. Quaney, Susan A. Grose, Joseph H. Maino; Effect of Bilateral Macular Scotomas from Age-Related Macular Degeneration on Reach-to-Grasp Hand Movement. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2011;52(5):2540-2550. https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.10-6062.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract

Purpose.: Vision plays a critical role in reaching and grasping objects. Consequently, bilateral macular scotomas from age-related macular degeneration (AMD) may affect reach-to-grasp movements. The purpose of this work was to investigate changes in reach-to-grasp movement dynamics and to relate those changes to the characteristics of subjects' preferred retinal loci (PRL), scotomas, and visual acuities.

Methods.: Three-dimensional positions of the index finger and thumb were recorded while subjects with bilateral scotomas and subjects with normal vision reached for and grasped blocks of three widths at two distances under binocular and monocular viewing conditions. Reach-dynamic parameters and the grip aperture (thumb-index finger distance) were calculated. Retinal locations and sizes of subjects' scotomas and PRLs were mapped with a scanning laser ophthalmoscope.

Results.: Scotoma subjects' hand trajectories had longer movement durations, lower maximum velocities, and longer visual reaction times than those of control subjects. With monocular viewing, maximum grip aperture (MGA) increased as a function of block width at a significantly higher rate for scotoma subjects than for control subjects. MGA decreased with increasing PRL bivariate normal ellipse area, and visual reaction time increased with decreasing acuity of the eye tested.

Conclusions.: Compared with normally sighted subjects, subjects with bilateral macular scotomas from AMD have reach-to-grasp movements with longer trajectories, longer visual reaction times, lower velocities, and altered MGA-block width scaling. Visual reaction time and MGA are directly related to PRL characteristics. Deficits in reach-to-grasp movement caused by macular scotomas are greater in degree than those reported by others for real or artificial peripheral scotomas.

Vision plays a crucial role in reaching and grasping objects. 1 5 Before a reach-to-grasp movement begins, the visual system provides information to the brain about the size, shape, and location of the object to be grasped. The brain then uses the information to plan motor commands to move the hand toward the object. When the motor plan is activated, the hand accelerates toward the object in what has been referred to as the “transport” or “reach” movement phase. As the hand decelerates and approaches the object in the “grasp” movement phase, vision also provides information about the object and the posture of the hand preparing to grasp it. 6 10  
Given the important role of vision in prehensile movements, it is likely that central scotomas affect reach-to-grasp movement. Persons with bilateral macular scotomas usually adopt a unique peripheral retinal area adjacent to the scotomatous retinal area for inspecting the visual environment. This area, the preferred retinal locus (PRL), 11,12 is a useful adaptation to central visual loss, but its function is poorer than that of the fovea. The PRL is most likely used to localize an object before reaching for it and to provide information about its size, shape, color, and texture. Macular scotomas may interfere with the process in several ways. First, more time to initiate a reach may be needed to place the target object image in the PRL, particularly if it initially falls in the scotoma. Second, the relatively poor visual resolution of a PRL as well as PRL fixation instability, may provide less than optimal information about object properties. Third, the scotoma may obscure the hand or object (or both) during reach-to-grasp movement. Finally, the lack of central binocular vision because of bilateral scotomas may reduce reach-to-grasp speed and accuracy. 1 3,13 15  
The effects on reach-to-grasp movements of both central 16 and peripheral artificial scotomas 4,16,17 and of scotomas from glaucoma 18 have been reported. Sivak and MacKenzie 16 investigated the effects on reach-to-grasp movements of artificial central scotomas in young subjects, but there have been no published investigations of the effects of central scotomas produced by disease or other disorders on reach-to-grasp movements. We investigated the effects of bilateral central scotomas in age-related macular degeneration (AMD) on reach-to-grasp movements. We also investigated the relationship between changes in reach-to-grasp movements and visual acuity and the characteristics of scotomas and PRLs found using the scanning laser ophthalmoscope (SLO). 
Methods
Subjects
Twenty subjects were tested, 10 with dense bilateral macular scotomas caused by AMD (scotoma group) and 10 with normal vision (control group). Ages and visual acuities of the subjects are shown in Table 1. The mean ages of the control and scotoma groups were not statistically different (t-test, P = 0.215). A physical therapist tested subjects for normal range of motion of the right arm and hand. Potential subjects were excluded if they took medications that could affect arm and hand movement, had a neurologic or musculoskeletal disorder that affected arm or hand motion, had insulin-dependent diabetes, or did not pass the Folstein Mini Mental test. All subjects were right-handed. Before testing, fixation was assessed using an SLO. None of the scotoma subjects had foveal fixation; all control subjects fixated foveally. The PRLs, scotomas, and probable position of the nonfunctional foveas were mapped using the SLO and previously described methods. 19,20 The dominant eye was determined using the Dolman hole-in-card test. 21 Binocular vision was assessed with the Titmus Fly Test 22 (Stereo Optical Co., Chicago, IL). All the control subjects were able to see the stereoscopic images in the test; none of the scotoma subjects was able to do so. Visual acuity was measured with an illuminated ETDRS chart. Before testing, all subjects provided written informed consent using a consent document approved by the Kansas City Veterans Administration Medical Center Internal Review Board. The research reported here adhered to the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki. 
Table 1.
 
Subjects' Ages and Visual Acuities
Table 1.
 
Subjects' Ages and Visual Acuities
Scotoma Subjects Control Subjects
Age (y) Duration (y) Visual Acuity (min arc) Age (y) Visual Acuity (min arc)
RE LE BE RE LE BE
88 7 20.0 20.0 20.0 73 1.0 1.0 1.0
78 4 3.16 20.0 3.16 82 1.58 1.26 1.26
84 6 10.0 10.0 10.0 77 1.0 1.0 0.79
80 7 10.0 10.0 10.0 87 1.58 1.0 1.0
81 6 7.94 7.94 7.94 89 1.58 1.26 1.26
84 6 10.0 5.01 5.01 79 1.0 1.0 1.0
87 5 2.51 5.01 3.98 70 1.0 0.79 0.79
75 8 5.02 15.89 10.02 78 1.0 0.79 0.79
75 10 7.94 10.0 5.01 74 1.0 1.58 1.0
82 10 10.0 15.89 15.89 74 1.0 0.79 0.79
81.4 6.9 8.66 11.97 9.10 78.3 1.17 1.05 0.97
Apparatus and Procedure
Subjects sat at a table and placed the thumb and index fingertip on starting position markers at the edge of the table (Fig. 1A). Small infrared LEDs were attached to the ends of the thumb and index finger. Three-dimensional positions of the infrared LEDs were recorded with a digital motion analysis system (DMAS; Spica Technology Corporation, Kihei, HI). This system uses two high-resolution digital infrared cameras placed at approximately right angles to each other approximately 3 meters from the test area. The cameras imaged the calibrated three-dimensional testing volume indicated by the vertical rods in Figure 1. The system digitally recorded the three-dimensional coordinates of the infrared LEDs on the subject's thumb and index fingertip at 120 Hz, with resolution of LED position <0.5 mm. 
Figure 1.
 
Apparatus for measuring reach-to-grasp movements. (A) Visor obscures subject's vision before trial. Block placed 40 cm from subject's hand. (B) Visor lifts and subject reaches for block. The block is shown tilted slightly off the subject's midline to show that block sides were black for high contrast.
Figure 1.
 
Apparatus for measuring reach-to-grasp movements. (A) Visor obscures subject's vision before trial. Block placed 40 cm from subject's hand. (B) Visor lifts and subject reaches for block. The block is shown tilted slightly off the subject's midline to show that block sides were black for high contrast.
Subjects wore their preferred optical correction beneath a head-mounted visor that occluded vision of the table and blocks until a trial began (Fig. 1A). Each subject reached for a wooden block placed either 20 or 40 cm from the subject's hand along the subject's midline, perpendicular to the subject. Three block sizes were used. All blocks were 3.8 cm high and of the same volume and weight, but they differed in length and width. Block widths (dimensions that were grasped) were 7.7, 5.5, and 3.6 cm. Blocks were painted white on top and black on the sides for high contrast; the blocks were placed on a light-colored table surface. The subject was instructed to reach and grasp the block as quickly and accurately as possible and to lift it slightly. When the experimenter pressed a switch, the computer began recording three-dimensional LED position data, and the visor flipped up (8 ms rise time) allowing the block to be seen (Fig. 1B). The trial ended, and the computer stopped data collection as soon as the block was lifted from the table surface. Subjects first performed 30 trials binocularly and then 30 trials with monocular vision using the dominant eye. The three sizes of block were each presented 5 times at the 20- and 40-cm distances in random order. 
Statistical Analysis
Three-dimensional coordinate data were analyzed with a technical computing program (MatLab; MathWorks, Natick, MA) after smoothing with an n = 5 boxcar average. The following parameters of the reach-to-grasp trajectories were calculated:
  1.  
    Directness ratio, the ratio of the actual trajectory length of the thumb divided by the shortest possible (straight line) distance between thumb starting and ending positions.
  2.  
    Visual reaction time, the time from first vision of the block (i.e., when the visor began to lift) until the hand began to move (Fig. 2).
  3.  
    Movement duration, the time from the start of hand movement until hand velocity was zero (Fig. 2).
  4.  
    Maximum velocity of the hand (Fig. 2).
  5.  
    Time to maximum velocity, the time from the start of hand movement until maximum hand velocity.
  6.  
    Deceleration duration, the time from maximum velocity until hand motion ceases.
Calculation of these parameters was based on the thumb infrared LED marker, which has been shown to parallel the wrist trajectory. 23 26 In addition to the parameters listed, the grip aperture (distance between the index finger and the thumb) was calculated throughout the trajectory, and the maximum grip aperture (MGA) was determined, as was the time to MGA from the start of hand movement (Fig. 2).
Figure 2.
 
Plot of thumb velocity (left axis, solid line) and grip aperture (right axis, dashed line) versus time for one 40-cm reach trial of a control subject. The visor flipped up at 0 second, allowing vision of the block.
Figure 2.
 
Plot of thumb velocity (left axis, solid line) and grip aperture (right axis, dashed line) versus time for one 40-cm reach trial of a control subject. The visor flipped up at 0 second, allowing vision of the block.
Preliminary analysis of data for all subjects and conditions indicated that the data were not normally distributed (Shapiro-Wilk tests, W > 0.308, P < 0.001), and sample data pairs of scotoma and control subjects did not satisfy equal variance conditions. Consequently, the data were analyzed using the nonparametric Friedman repeated-measures ANOVA on ranks with Student-Newman-Keuls post hoc comparisons. The Student-Newman-Keuls post hoc test calculates a q value that is then compared to a critical value found in a studentized range table. For the post hoc comparisons here, the critical value for α = 0.05, df = 36, and number of groups compared = 4 was q 0.05,36,4 = 3.85. Values of q that exceeded 3.85 indicated a statistically significant difference, whereas those <3.85 indicated no difference. The values of q for each test are given to provide some indication as to the strength of a statistical difference or the lack of one. Since the Friedman repeated-measures ANOVA on ranks tests whether there are differences in the medians among groups, results are plotted as box plots that show the median, 25th, and 75th percentiles and error bars representing the 10% to 90% percentiles. Preliminary data analysis indicated that the reach-to-grasp parameters were not statistically significantly different for different block sizes. Therefore, the data were collapsed over block size for further analysis of these parameters. 
Results
Trajectories
Three-dimensional trajectories of a control subject and a scotoma subject are shown from above (i.e., in the horizontal X, Y plane) for both binocular and monocular 20- and 40-cm reaches (Fig. 3). In Figure 3, the left-right (Y) distance between the index finger and thumb increased as the hand moved forward and then began to close as the block was approached. Overall, the trajectories of the control and scotoma subject appear similar in shape. 
Figure 3.
 
Binocular and monocular trajectories of a control subject (top plots) and a scotoma subject (bottom plots) for 40- and 20-cm reaches. Three-dimensional trajectories are shown as seen from above. Each plot shows trajectories of five reaches. Th, thumb trajectories; In, index finger trajectories.
Figure 3.
 
Binocular and monocular trajectories of a control subject (top plots) and a scotoma subject (bottom plots) for 40- and 20-cm reaches. Three-dimensional trajectories are shown as seen from above. Each plot shows trajectories of five reaches. Th, thumb trajectories; In, index finger trajectories.
Trajectory Variability
Trajectory spatial variability of control and scotoma subjects was compared by sampling the X, Y, Z thumb position of individual trajectories every 25 mm of forward movement. 23,25 The mean and SD were calculated for each 25-mm sample position in the vertical (up-down, Z axis) and in the horizontal (left-right, Y axis) directions (Fig. 4). In Figure 4, for both the control and scotoma subject, SDs were small at the start of the trajectory, greatest near the middle of the trajectory, then grew smaller again as the hand approached the object. For statistical comparison of control and scotoma trajectories, an index of variability was calculated for each subject by summing separately the horizontal SDs and vertical SDs. The index was calculated separately for the 20-cm and 40-cm reaches. Because the index was not normally distributed (Shapiro-Wilk, W ≥ 0.672, P < 0.001), statistical comparisons were carried out using repeated-measures ANOVA on ranks. Index of variability for the three block sizes were combined because preliminary repeated-measures ANOVA on ranks indicated that the index was not significantly different across block size. Control and scotoma subjects' vertical and horizontal trajectory variability indices did not differ significantly for either binocular or monocular 20-cm or 40-cm reaches (Student-Newman-Keuls tests, q ≤ 2.236, P > 0.05). 
Figure 4.
 
Averages of fifteen 40-cm binocular reach trajectories of a control and scotoma subject. Vertical lines and horizontal lines show vertical and horizontal +1 SD, respectively. The SDs have been multiplied by 5 for visibility.
Figure 4.
 
Averages of fifteen 40-cm binocular reach trajectories of a control and scotoma subject. Vertical lines and horizontal lines show vertical and horizontal +1 SD, respectively. The SDs have been multiplied by 5 for visibility.
Directness Ratio
Control and scotoma subjects' trajectories were also compared on the basis of directness ratio. 27,28 The larger the directness ratio, the longer the thumb trajectory. Median directness ratios of control and scotoma subjects were not significantly different for either the 20-cm binocular reaches (Student-Newman-Keuls, q = 0.447, P > 0.5) or monocular reaches (Student-Newman-Keuls, q = 3.130, P > 0.5; Fig. 5). For 40-cm reaches, however, scotoma subjects had significantly higher median directness ratios than control subjects for both binocular (Student-Newman-Keuls, q = 4.287, P < 0.5) and monocular (Student-Newman-Keuls, q = 3.862, P < 0.5) viewing conditions. This indicates that scotoma subjects tended to make relatively longer trajectories than control subjects when reaching for the blocks at 40 cm. Scotoma subjects' median directness ratios were 24% higher than control subjects' for 40-cm binocular reaches and 2% higher for monocular reaches. Control subjects' binocular and monocular directness ratios were not significantly different for the 20-cm reaches (Student-Newman-Keuls, q = 3.130, P > 0.05) or the 40-cm reaches (Student-Newman-Keuls, q = 1.342, P > 0.05) nor were those of scotoma subjects (20 cm: Student-Newman-Keuls, q = 3.674, P > 0.05; 40 cm: Student-Newman-Keuls, q = 2.683, P > 0.05). 
Figure 5.
 
Box plots comparing the directness ratios of control and scotoma subjects for binocular and monocular 20- and 40-cm reaches. In this and subsequent box plots, lower and upper borders of each box represent the 25th and 75th percentiles, respectively. Error bars show 10th and 90th percentiles. Solid horizontal line in boxes: medians. Asterisks: statistically significant differences (P < 0.05). Medians of control subjects' 40-cm reaches are on the lower (25%) edges of the binocular and monocular boxes.
Figure 5.
 
Box plots comparing the directness ratios of control and scotoma subjects for binocular and monocular 20- and 40-cm reaches. In this and subsequent box plots, lower and upper borders of each box represent the 25th and 75th percentiles, respectively. Error bars show 10th and 90th percentiles. Solid horizontal line in boxes: medians. Asterisks: statistically significant differences (P < 0.05). Medians of control subjects' 40-cm reaches are on the lower (25%) edges of the binocular and monocular boxes.
Visual Reaction Time
Scotoma subjects had significantly longer median visual reaction times than control subjects for 20-cm binocular reaches (Student-Newman-Keuls, q = 3.865, P < 0.05) and monocular reaches (Student-Newman-Keuls, q = 5.814, P < 0.05) and for 40-cm binocular reaches (Student-Newman-Keuls, q = 5.692, P < 0.05) and monocular reaches (Student-Newman-Keuls, q = 4.427, P < 0.05; Fig. 6). Scotoma subjects' median visual reaction times were 24% to 32% longer than those of control subjects. Visual reaction times with monocular viewing were not significantly different from those with binocular viewing for scotoma subjects or control subjects or for either reach distance. 
Figure 6.
 
Visual reaction times of control and scotoma subjects for monocular and binocular 20- and 40-cm reaches. Asterisks: statistically significant differences (P < 0.05).
Figure 6.
 
Visual reaction times of control and scotoma subjects for monocular and binocular 20- and 40-cm reaches. Asterisks: statistically significant differences (P < 0.05).
Movement Duration
Scotoma subjects had significantly longer median movement durations than control subjects for 20-cm binocular reaches (Student-Newman-Keuls, q = 4.654, P < 0.05) and monocular reaches (Student-Newman-Keuls, q = 6.708, P < 0.05) and for 40-cm binocular reaches (Student-Newman-Keuls, q = 5.144, P < 0.05) and monocular reaches (Student-Newman-Keuls, q = 6.708, P < 0.05; Fig. 7). For 20-cm reaches, scotoma subjects' median movement durations were approximately 30% longer than control subjects. For 40-cm reaches, scotoma subjects median movement durations were 28% and 59% longer than control subjects for binocular and monocular reaches, respectively. Binocular and monocular movement durations were not significantly different for control subjects for either the 20-cm reaches (Student-Newman-Keuls, q = 0.447, P > 0.05) or the 40-cm reaches (Student-Newman-Keuls, q = 0.894, P > 0.05) or for the scotoma subjects' 20-cm reaches (Student-Newman-Keuls, q = 1.642, P > 0.05) or 40-cm reaches (Student-Newman-Keuls, q = 1.789, P > 0.05). 
Figure 7.
 
Median movement duration for control and scotoma subjects for binocular and monocular 20- and 40-cm reaches. Asterisks: statistically significant differences (P < 0.05).
Figure 7.
 
Median movement duration for control and scotoma subjects for binocular and monocular 20- and 40-cm reaches. Asterisks: statistically significant differences (P < 0.05).
Maximum Velocity
Thumb velocity as a function of time is shown for a control subject's 40-cm reach in Figure 2. Velocity increased rapidly, reaching a maximum at approximately 31% of movement duration. The hand then decelerated as it approached and grasped the block. Control subjects had median maximum velocities significantly higher (20%–68%) than scotoma subjects for 20-cm binocular reaches (Student-Newman-Keuls, q = 6.369, P < 0.05) and monocular reaches (Student-Newman-Keuls, q = 6.361, P < 0.05) and for 40-cm binocular reaches (Student-Newman-Keuls, q = 5.389, P < 0.05) and monocular reaches (Student-Newman-Keuls, q = 5.367, P < 0.05; Fig. 8). Monocular and binocular maximum velocities differed significantly (Student-Newman-Keuls, q = 4.472, P < 0.05) only for control subjects for 20-cm reaches. 
Figure 8.
 
Median maximum velocities for control and scotoma subjects with binocular and monocular viewing and 20- and 40-cm reaches. Asterisks: statistically significant differences (P < 0.05).
Figure 8.
 
Median maximum velocities for control and scotoma subjects with binocular and monocular viewing and 20- and 40-cm reaches. Asterisks: statistically significant differences (P < 0.05).
Time to Maximum Velocity
Given that visual reaction times of scotoma subjects were significantly longer than those of control subjects (Fig. 6), comparing the time to achieve maximum velocity from the trial start time may bias the maximum velocity time. Consequently, time to maximum velocity was calculated by subtracting the visual reaction time. Thus, time to maximum velocity was measured from the start of thumb movement. Scotoma subjects reached maximum velocity 15% later than control subjects for 20-cm binocular reaches and 6% later for monocular reaches (Fig. 9). These differences, however, were not significant (repeated-measures ANOVA on ranks, χ2 = 3.240, P = 0.356). For 40-cm binocular reaches, scotoma subjects reached maximum velocity 3% later than control subjects, not a significant difference (Student-Newman-Keuls, q = 0.010, P > 0.05). For 40-cm binocular reaches, scotoma subjects reached maximum velocity significantly later (26%) than control subjects (Student-Newman-Keuls, q = 5.367, P < 0.05). 
Figure 9.
 
Time to maximum velocity minus visual reaction time (i.e., time to maximum velocity from the start of hand motion). Asterisks: statistically significant differences (P < 0.05).
Figure 9.
 
Time to maximum velocity minus visual reaction time (i.e., time to maximum velocity from the start of hand motion). Asterisks: statistically significant differences (P < 0.05).
Deceleration Duration
Deceleration duration, the time from maximum velocity to cessation of hand motion (see Fig. 2) is shown for all conditions in Figure 10. Deceleration durations were 54% longer for scotoma subjects' binocular 20-cm reaches (Student-Newman-Keuls, q = 4.919, P < 0.05) and 51% longer (Student-Newman-Keuls, q = 4.654, P < 0.05) for their monocular 20-cm reaches. For 40-cm reaches, scotoma subjects had deceleration times 44% longer (Student-Newman-Keuls, q = 6.261, P < 0.05) than control subjects for binocular reaches and 66% longer (Student-Newman-Keuls, q = 4.899, P < 0.05) for monocular reaches. 
Figure 10.
 
Duration (ms) of deceleration phase of reach-to-grasp movements. Asterisks: statistically significant differences (P < 0.05).
Figure 10.
 
Duration (ms) of deceleration phase of reach-to-grasp movements. Asterisks: statistically significant differences (P < 0.05).
Maximum Grip Aperture Scaling
Grip aperture increased after the beginning of hand movement and reached a maximum at approximately 64% of movement duration (Fig. 2). The MGA has been shown to increase linearly with object size. 1,13,29 32 Figure 11 shows regression lines and 95% confidence intervals for MGA versus block width for control and scotoma subjects with binocular (Fig. 11A) and monocular (Fig. 11B) viewing. Because MGAs for the 20-cm reaches did not differ significantly from those of the 40-cm reaches (Student-Newman-Keuls tests, q = 0.038–3.515l; P > 0.05), the 20- and 40-cm reach MGAs were combined for each block size within binocular and monocular conditions. The slope of the MGA versus block width regression line indicates the increase in width of MGA per unit increase in width of the object. The slope is a unitless measure (mm/mm) referred to as the grip scale factor. 33 For binocular viewing (Fig. 11A), control subjects' grip scale factor was 0.344. This slope was almost identical with that of scotoma subjects (0.367), and there was almost complete overlap of the 95% confidence intervals. The grip scale factors of control and scotoma subjects did not differ significantly (two-tailed, t = 0.044, P = 0.966), nor did the Y-intercepts (two-tailed, t = 0.333, P = 0.739). Thus, for binocular reaches, control and scotoma subjects adjusted their MGA identically as block size changed. This was not the case for monocular reaches, as shown in Figure 11B. Monocular MGAs of scotoma subjects were larger overall than those of control subjects and increased more rapidly as a function of block width. Grip scale factors for scotoma and control subjects (0.296 and 0.177, respectively) were significantly different (two-tailed, t = 2.087, P = 0.037). Thus scotoma subjects increased their MGA as a function of block width 1.7 (i.e., (0.296)/(0.177)) times that of control subjects. Regression line Y-intercepts were not significantly different (two-tailed, t = 0.521, P = 0.602). 
Figure 11.
 
Regression lines (solid lines) and 95% confidence intervals (dashed lines) for MGA versus block width for control and scotoma subjects for (A) binocular and (B) monocular viewing. Three block widths were used: 36.4, 54.8, and 77.4 mm (vertical dashed lines). Regression line slope is the grip scale factor. Data points were omitted for clarity.
Figure 11.
 
Regression lines (solid lines) and 95% confidence intervals (dashed lines) for MGA versus block width for control and scotoma subjects for (A) binocular and (B) monocular viewing. Three block widths were used: 36.4, 54.8, and 77.4 mm (vertical dashed lines). Regression line slope is the grip scale factor. Data points were omitted for clarity.
Time to MGA
As with time to maximum velocity, described previously, longer visual reaction times for scotoma subjects may bias the time to MGA toward longer times. Consequently, the time to MGA was calculated from the start of hand movement. The time required to reach MGA from the start of hand movement is shown in Figure 12 for 20- and 40-cm reaches. Time to MGA is shown for each block size, because the times to MGA were significantly different for different block sizes (Student-Newman-Keuls, q = 4.65–7.10, P < 0.05). In Figure 12, median times to MGA within each group (e.g., within the group of control subjects, 20-cm reaches and monocular viewing) are longest for the widest block (block 1, 77.4 mm) and shortest for the narrowest block (block 3, 36.4 mm). Median times to MGA were significantly longer for scotoma subjects than for control subjects for each block size. For example, within binocular 20-cm reaches, scotoma subjects had a longer median time to MGA for block 1 than control subjects had for block 1, a longer median time for block 2 than control subjects, and a longer median time for block 3. These differences were significant for all monocular and binocular 20-cm reaches (Student-Newman-Keuls, q = 5.14–8.22, P < 0.05) and all monocular and binocular 40-cm reaches (Student-Newman-Keuls, q = 5.14–7.84, P < 0.05). Overall, time to MGA of scotoma subjects increased 18% to 39% compared with control subjects with binocular viewing and 27% to 64% with monocular viewing. 
Figure 12.
 
Time to MGA from initiation of hand movement. 1, widest width block, 77.4 mm; 2, medium width block, 54.8 mm; 3, narrowest width block, 36.4 mm.
Figure 12.
 
Time to MGA from initiation of hand movement. 1, widest width block, 77.4 mm; 2, medium width block, 54.8 mm; 3, narrowest width block, 36.4 mm.
Retinal Correlations
SLO retinal maps were obtained for 7 of the 10 scotoma subjects. An example of an SLO retinal map showing the subject's PRL, the densest aspect of the macular scotoma, and the normal estimated foveal fixation area 20 is shown in Figure 13. From such images the PRL bivariate normal ellipse area (P = 0.68), 20 the PRL retinal eccentricity relative to the normal estimated fixation ellipse centroid, and the scotoma area were calculated. Correlations among these retinal parameters, visual acuity, and the reach-to-grasp parameters were explored. Significant correlations were found between scotoma subjects' monocular MGA and their PRL bivariate ellipse area for each of the three size blocks (Fig. 14A). In Figure 14A, MGA minus block width is plotted versus PRL bivariate normal ellipse area (P = 0.68) for the three block widths to show how much larger the grip aperture was than the block to be grasped as a function of PRL bivariate area. Regression line correlation coefficients (Fig. 14A legend) were all statistically significantly different from zero (t-tests: block 1, P = 0.023; block 2, P = 0.001; block 3, P = 0.002). Figure 13A shows that scotoma subjects tended to increase their MGA approximately 70 to 90 mm larger than the narrowest (36.4 mm) block (Fig. 14A, squares) but by less (∼48–58 mm) when grasping the largest (77.4 mm) block. For all three block sizes, MGA minus block width decreased as PRL bivariate ellipse area increased. Visual reaction time for monocular reaches was also significantly correlated with visual acuity of the dominant eye used for the task (r = 0.614. t-test, P = 0.004; Fig. 14B). As visual acuity decreased (i.e., minimum angular resolution in min arc increased), visual reaction time increased. 
Figure 13.
 
SLO retinal map of a scotoma subject's right retina. Irregular black line encloses area of dense scotoma. Large ellipse represents the normal foveal fixation area (P = 0.90 bivariate normal ellipse). Normal foveal fixation area shows the estimated position of the subject's nonfunctional fovea with a 90% probability. Small ellipse represents the subject's PRL (P = 0.68 bivariate normal ellipse). Dots in centers of ellipses are ellipse centroids. The image has been rotated so that the top of the image is visually up and the left of the image is visually left.
Figure 13.
 
SLO retinal map of a scotoma subject's right retina. Irregular black line encloses area of dense scotoma. Large ellipse represents the normal foveal fixation area (P = 0.90 bivariate normal ellipse). Normal foveal fixation area shows the estimated position of the subject's nonfunctional fovea with a 90% probability. Small ellipse represents the subject's PRL (P = 0.68 bivariate normal ellipse). Dots in centers of ellipses are ellipse centroids. The image has been rotated so that the top of the image is visually up and the left of the image is visually left.
Figure 14.
 
(A) Regression plot of MGA minus block width versus PRL bivariate normal ellipse area (P = 0.68) for scotoma subjects with monocular viewing. (B) Regression plot of visual reaction time versus visual acuity of the tested dominant eye for scotoma subjects with monocular viewing. Regression lines for 20- and 40-cm reaches were not significantly different (t-tests, P > 0.05) and were combined.
Figure 14.
 
(A) Regression plot of MGA minus block width versus PRL bivariate normal ellipse area (P = 0.68) for scotoma subjects with monocular viewing. (B) Regression plot of visual reaction time versus visual acuity of the tested dominant eye for scotoma subjects with monocular viewing. Regression lines for 20- and 40-cm reaches were not significantly different (t-tests, P > 0.05) and were combined.
Discussion
The goal of the present study was to determine how bilateral macular scotomas affect reach-to-grasp hand movements and to relate reach-to-grasp changes to PRL and scotoma characteristics. Overall, hand trajectory length and precision were not greatly affected by bilateral scotomas. However, trajectory dynamics (e.g., visual reaction time, movement duration, maximum velocity) were uniformly poorer for subjects with scotomas than for control subjects. Although reach-to-grasp dynamic parameters were generally not strongly correlated with PRL and scotoma characteristics, visual reaction time was found to be significantly correlated with visual acuity (presumably PRL acuity), and MGA was correlated with decreasing PRL size. Detailed discussion of these results follows. 
Trajectories
The trajectory variability index was not significantly different for control and scotoma subjects indicating that, despite reduced visual capacity from macular scotomas, scotoma subjects obtained sufficient visual information to execute trajectories toward the target with as little variability as control subjects. Although trajectory variability of scotoma subjects was the same as of control subjects, scotoma subjects did have larger directness ratios for binocular and monocular 40-cm reaches. This means that for the longer reaches, scotoma subjects moved their hand along a longer path toward the object than did control subjects. The reason for such longer trajectories is unclear but may result from moving the hand so it is not obscured by the scotoma when the PRL is viewing the block. Longer trajectories also may result from the longer deceleration times of scotoma subjects. 
Reach Dynamics
The percentage increases or decreases in reach dynamics parameters relative to controls for artificial and real scotomas from previously published studies and the present study are summarized in Table 2. 16 18,34 The range of percentage changes in reach parameters for peripheral and for central scotomas is given in the last two rows of Table 2. The increases or decreases in reach parameters found in the present study are generally in the same direction (i.e., positive or negative) as those found by others for peripheral scotomas and artificial central scotomas. However, the percentage changes produced by artificial 16 or real (present study) central scotomas are generally larger than those from artificial 16,34 or real peripheral scotomas 18 (Table 2, last two rows). A single exception is the 82% increase in deceleration duration with artificial binocular peripheral scotomas found by Sivak and MacKenzie. 16 During the deceleration phase, visual feedback is used to make final corrections as the hand approaches the object. 17,34 More deceleration time may be needed for this correction with central scotomas than with peripheral scotomas (with the exception mentioned) because central scotomas may obscure the hand or object or both, making online changes more difficult. In summary, central scotomas generally appear to produce larger deficits in reach-to-grasp dynamics than do peripheral scotomas. This seems reasonable in view of the potential obscuration of the target object and hand by the scotoma and the difficulty of using a PRL to gather visual information. 
Table 2.
 
Percentage Increase (Positive Values) or Decrease (Negative Values) of Reach and Grasp Parameters of Scotoma Subjects Relative to Control Subjects
Table 2.
 
Percentage Increase (Positive Values) or Decrease (Negative Values) of Reach and Grasp Parameters of Scotoma Subjects Relative to Control Subjects
Scotoma Type Reach Component Grasp Component
Reach Distance (cm) Reaction Time Movement Duration Maximum Velocity Time to Maximum Velocity Deceleration Duration Object Width (cm) MGA Time to MGA
Peripheral
    Artificial (binocular) 16 30 41 −20 −4 82 2.5 4 28
    Artificial (binocular) 17 * 30 9 −8 −3 32 3.0 0 2
    Glaucoma (binocular) 18 20 18 16 −2 13 17
40 0 14 17 4.8 2 8
2.4 6 10
    Artificial (monocular) 34 (36, 68) 10§ −10 3 12 3 5
4‖ −11 0 3 2
    Glaucoma (monocular) 18 20 26 25 −2 10 30
40 0 12 30 4.8 0 8
2.4 0 14
Central
    Artificial (binocular) 16 30 44 −21 30 53 2.5 36 29
    AMD (binocular)‡ 20 31 29 −16 15 54 7.7 0 39
5.5 0 24
3.6 0 18
40 36 31 −18 3 44 7.7 0 30
5.5 0 29
3.6 0 25
    AMD (monocular)‡ 20 26 37 −26 6 51 7.7 9 36
5.5 5 36
3.6 8 27
40 26 54 −28 26 66 7.7 7 64
5.5 5 51
3.6 9 44
Range, all studies
    Peripheral 18–26 4–41 0–(−20) −4–14 12–82 2–6 2–28
    Central 26–36 29–54 −16–(−28) 26–30 44–66 5–36 18–64
In the present study, for both control and scotoma subjects, reach dynamic parameters with monocular viewing were not significantly different from those with binocular viewing with the exception of a significant difference in maximum velocity for control subjects' 20-cm reaches (Fig. 8). Previous studies of young, visually normal subjects have reported that monocular viewing results in increased movement duration, decreased peak velocities, 4,13 15,41 and increased time to maximum velocity. 4,14,15,35 It is possible that the general lack of change in reach dynamics with monocular viewing is related to aging because subjects in the present study were considerably older (mean age, 79.9 years) than those in the previous studies cited (mean reported age, ∼26 years). The lack of a difference in binocular and monocular dynamics parameters for scotoma subjects is not surprising because these subjects usually strongly preferred using one eye and showed no evidence of binocular vision. 
Maximum Grip Aperture
There are conflicting reports concerning the effects of peripheral and central scotomas on MGA (Table 2). For peripheral scotomas, two previous studies reported an increase in MGA for scotoma subjects relative to control subjects with binocular viewing, 16,18 whereas another study 17 found no change. With monocular viewing, one study 34 reported increased MGA with scotomas, whereas another 18 found no difference in MGA between scotoma and control subjects. For central scotomas, the present study found no significant difference between MGA for scotoma and control subjects with binocular viewing. In contrast, Sivak and MacKenzie 16 using binocular, artificial central scotomas, found a 36% increase in MGA with binocular viewing. However, the subjects in the present study differed from those of Sivak and McKenzie 16 in several ways. First, the subjects were considerably older, and there is a documented effect of age on prehension. 36 Second, the subjects' scotomas resulted from disease and were long term, providing time for visuomotor adaptation. Third, scotoma subjects had all developed unique PRLs, and it is not known whether the artificial scotoma subjects in the Sivak and MacKenzie study 16 developed short-term PRLs. These factors may account for the large increase in MGA for binocular viewing found by Sivak and MacKenzie 16 compared with the finding of no increase in the present study. 
Scotoma subjects' lack of an increase in binocular MGA relative to control subjects as well as only a modest increase relative to control subjects with monocular viewing is consistent with the hypothesis that long-term experience with macular scotomas results in more complete visuomotor adaptation for prehensile movements than do artificial, temporary macular scotomas. Part of this adaptation may be due to improvement over time in the ability to judge the size and position of objects to be grasped using PRLs. Experiments comparing judgments of object size by normally sighted and scotoma subjects 37 would help to address this. The time course of the visuomotor adaptation to central scotomas is unknown, but it is possible that subjects improve in grasping accuracy with increasing experience. Longitudinal measurements of PRL reach-to-grasp movements from the onset of macular scotomas may be useful in understanding the adaptation process and, perhaps, in developing rehabilitation procedures. 
Previous studies by others of young, visually normal subjects found increased, 2,3,15,35 decreased, 13 or no change 4 in MGA with monocular viewing compared with binocular viewing. In the present study with monocular viewing, scotoma subjects' MGAs increased relative to control subjects (Table 2). Scotoma subjects' MGAs were not different from those of control subjects in binocular viewing. It is possible that the observed increased MGA with monocular viewing represents a “safety margin” 34 (i.e., making the grip aperture larger than necessary to ensure grasping) such as that described when all visual feedback is eliminated during the reach. 5,26  
The range of increases in time to MGA of subjects with central scotomas relative to control subjects for both binocular and monocular viewing was 18% to 64% compared with 2% to 28% for peripheral scotomas (Table 2). Central scotomas, therefore, generally resulted in longer times to reach MGA than did peripheral scotomas, suggesting that central scotomas interfere more with gathering initial information about object position than do peripheral scotomas. This is consistent with the finding that positional uncertainty increases markedly with eccentricity in near-peripheral retinas (2.5°-10°), 38 similar to the range of eccentricities of subjects' PRLs. Previous studies also reported decreased, 2 increased, 30 or no change 3,4 in the time to MGA with monocular viewing, whereas the present work found increases relative to control subjects in the time to MGA for both monocular and binocular viewing (Fig. 12). 
Grip Scale Factor
Grip scale factors (i.e., slopes of regression lines of MGA versus object size) are shown in Table 3 for the present work and for five previous studies by others. 1,13,29 31 Binocular grip scale factors for control subjects in the present study are 25% to 60% lower than the scale factors found by others and 76% to 80% lower for monocular viewing (Table 3). Binocular grip scale factors of scotoma subjects are 20% to 58% lower than those found by others and 59% to 66% lower for monocular viewing. These differences in grip aperture scaling between subjects in the present work and in work by others may be due to age differences. The age range of subjects in work by others (Table 3) was 19 to 28 years compared with 70 to 91 years in the present study. A variety of hand functions decrease with age, 39 41 and it is possible that these changes produced the observed changes in grip aperture scaling. 
Table 3.
 
Slope of Regression Line Fit to MGA versus Object Width (mm/mm)
Table 3.
 
Slope of Regression Line Fit to MGA versus Object Width (mm/mm)
Authors Subject Mean Age (y) n Object Width (cm) Binocular Slope Monocular Slope
Goodale et al. 29 1 2.5–5 0.460
1 2.5–5 0.810
Servos et al. 13 22.6 9 2–5 0.756 0.892
Servos and Goodale 1 23.5 8 2–5 0.878 0.721
Kudoh et al. 31 19–28 12 2–8 0.855
Jakobson and Goodale 30 25.3 15 2–5 0.664
Present study (controls) 78.3 10 3.4–7.7 0.345 0.177
Present study (scotoma) 81.4 10 3.4–7.7 0.367 0.296
The present study also found that grip scale factor decreased with monocular viewing for both control and scotoma subjects (Table 3). The relative decrease of monocular to binocular grip scale factor was 49% for control subjects and 19% for scotoma subjects (Fig. 11). Scotoma subjects may have a monocular grip scale factor that is larger than that of control subjects and close to their binocular scale factor because scotoma subjects primarily use their dominant eye in both monocular and binocular viewing. Given that the retinal lesions that cause central scotomas are rarely the same size, shape, and position in both eyes, PRLs in both eyes are usually located on different retinal areas compared with the fovea. As a result, there is no retinal correspondence of PRLs, and there may be little or no central binocular vision. 42  
Retinal Correlations
The present work demonstrates that some changes in reach-to-grasp dynamics and MGA are directly related to retinal and visual changes produced by AMD. MGA decreased as a function of increasing PRL bivariate area (i.e., decreasing fixation stability) for each of the three block widths (Fig. 14A), and visual reaction time increased with decreasing visual acuity (Fig. 14B). Longer visual reaction times with reduced acuity may be attributed to poor visual resolution resulting in uncertainty about object shape, width, or position. 
The finding that MGA decreases with increasing PRL bivariate area is puzzling because, intuitively, one would think that greater target image movement associated with a larger PRL bivariate ellipse area would increase uncertainty about target size and, hence, a larger MGA would be used. It has been shown previously that PRL bivariate area is positively correlated with PRL eccentricity, 20 so it is possible that increasing PRL eccentricity is correlated with underestimation of object size and smaller MGA. However, others have found that the MGA of normally sighted subjects increased with increasing target object eccentricity, although perceptual estimation of target size did not. 43 There are marked differences in that study 43 and the present study with scotoma subjects. First, in the other study, 43 young visually normal subjects fixated (foveally) a stimulus while the target object was presented at an eccentric position. Consequently, it is reasonable to assume that the stability of the object image on the retina was typical of normal fixation (≤0.1 deg2), which is an order of magnitude, or more, smaller than for PRL fixation by scotoma subjects in the present study. Second, the eccentricities in the other study 43 were 10° to 40°, larger than the 3° to 12° range of PRL eccentricities in the present study. Thus, differences in target object image stability and eccentricity may account for the different findings in the two studies. It may be informative to perform the MGA and perceived object size measurements described by Brown, Halpert, and Goodale 43 with scotoma subjects and with normally sighted subjects at eccentricities similar to those of the scotoma subjects' PRLs. 
Another factor that may affect MGA size is metamorphopsia associated with macular scotomas. Pincushion metamorphopsia has been found with macular holes 44,45 and perceived line shortening with homonymous paracentral scotomas. 46 In these studies, the amount of distortion is related to eccentricity. With pincushion distortion, block width would be perceived as smaller than normal, possibly resulting in a smaller MGA. If perceptual distortion at the PRL increases with increasing PRL eccentricity, then a smaller MGA might be expected with more eccentric PRLs that also have larger bivariate ellipse areas. Measurement of perceptual distortion at the PRL would be useful in elucidating the role of distortion in setting the MGA. Finally, it is possible that the correlations of MGA and PRL bivariate ellipse area are spurious given the relatively small number of subjects. Further testing will help answer this question 
Reaching and grasping a solitary block is not representative of everyday reaching and grasping situations, but investigating it may provide some insight into problems faced by persons with bilateral macular scotomas. A more realistic reach-to-grasp situation might be one in which the target object is in an environment with multiple objects of different sizes, shapes, and positions. Such cluttered visual environments undoubtedly make reaching and grasping a target object more difficult, 35,47,48 and it would be important to describe reach-to-grasp movements with such cluttered conditions. We believe that it will also be important to investigate further the relationship between deficits in eye-hand coordination and retinal functional geography (e.g., retinal position, size of the PRL, scotoma). Ultimately, documenting real-time scotoma obscurations of the hand and objects and establishing PRL location during eye-hand coordination could be useful in developing effective rehabilitation measures. 
Footnotes
 Presented at the annual meeting of the Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology, Fort Lauderdale, Florida, April 2009.
Footnotes
 Supported by Department of Veterans Affairs, Rehabilitation Research and Development Service Grant C6218R.
Footnotes
 Disclosure: G.T. Timberlake, None; E. Omoscharka, None; B.M. Quaney, None; S.A. Grose, None; J.H. Maino, None
References
Servos P Goodale MA . Binocular vision and online control of human prehension. Exp Brain Res. 1994;54:121–132.
Watt SJ Bradshaw MF . Binocular cues are important in controlling the grasp but not the reach in natural prehension movements. Neuropsychologia. 2000;38:1473–1481. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Bradshaw M Elliott K . The role of binocular information in the ‘on-line’ control of prehension. Spatial Vision. 2003;16:295–309. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Loftus A Servos P Goodale MA Mendarozqueta N Mon-Williams M . When two eyes are better than one in prehension: monocular viewing and end-point variance. Exp Brain Res. 2004;158:317–327. [PubMed]
Rand M Lemay M Squire L Shimansky Y Stelmach G . Role of vision in aperture closure control during reach-to-grasp movements. Exp Brain Res. 2007;181:447–460. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Paulignan Y MacKenzie C Marteniuk R Jeannerod M . The coupling of arm and finger movements during prehension. Exp Brain Res. 1990;79:431–435. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Saunders JA Knill DC . Humans use continuous visual feedback from the hand to control fast reaching movements. Exp Brain Res. 2003;152:341–352. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
MA-Wyatt A McKee S . Visual information throughout a reach determines endpoint precision. Exp Brain Res. 2007;179:55–64. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Lee Y-L Crabtree C Norman J Bingham G . Poor shape perception is the reason reaches-to-grasp are visually guided online. Percept Psychophys. 2008;70:1032–1046. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Connolly J Goodale M . The role of visual feedback of hand position in the control of manual prehension. Exp Brain Res. 1999;125:281–286. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Timberlake GT Peli E Essock EA Augliere RA . Reading with a macular scotoma II: Retinal locus for scanning text. Invest Ophthalmo Vis Sci. 1987;28:1268–1274.
Schuchard RA . Preferred retinal loci and macular scotoma characteristics in patients with age-related macular degeneration. Can J Ophthalmol. 2005;40:303–312. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Servos P Goodale MA Jakobson LS . The role of binocular vision in prehension: a kinematic analysis. Vision Res. 1992;32:1513–1521. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Marotta J Perrot T Nicolle D Servos P Goodale M . Adapting to monocular vision: grasping with one eye. Exp Brain Res. 1995;104:107–114. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Jackson S Newport R Shaw A . Monocular vision leads to a dissociation between grip force and grip aperture scaling during reach-to-grasp movements. Curr Biol. 2002;12:237–240. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Sivak B MacKenzie C . Integration of visual information and motor output in reaching and grasping: the contributions of peripheral and central vision. Neuropsychologia. 1990;28:1095–1116. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Loftus A Murphy S McKenna I Mon-Williams M . Reduced fields of view are neither necessary nor sufficient for distance underestimation but reduce precision and may cause calibration problems. Exp Brain Res. 2004;158:328–335. [PubMed]
Kotecha A O'Leary N Melmoth D Grant S Crabb D . The functional consequences of glaucoma for eye-hand coordination. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2009;50:203–213. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Timberlake GT Mainster MA Webb RH Hughes GW Trempe CL . Retinal localization of scotomata by scanning laser ophthalmoscopy. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1982;22:91–97. [PubMed]
Timberlake GT Sharma MK Grose SA . Retinal location of the preferred retinal locus (PRL) relative to the fovea in scanning laser ophthalmoscope (SLO) images. Optom Vis Sci. 2005;82:177–186. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Dolman P . Tests for determining sighting of the eye. Am J Ophthalmol. 1919;2:287. [CrossRef]
Somers WW Hamilton MJ . Estimation of the stereoscopic threshold utilizing perceived depth. Ophthal Physiol Opt. 1984;4:245–250. [CrossRef]
Paulignan Y Jeannerod M MacKenzie C Marteniuk R . Selective perturbation of visual input during prehension movements. Exp Brain Res. 1991;87:407–420. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Wing A Fraser C . The contribution of the thumb to reaching movements. Q J Exp Psychol. 1983;35A:297–309. [CrossRef]
Haggard P Wing A . On the hand transport component of prehensile movements. J Motor Behav. 1997;29:282–287. [CrossRef]
Wing A Turton A Fraser C . Grasp size and accuracy of approach in reaching. J Mot Behav. 1986;18:245–260. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Bastian A Martin T Keating J Thach W . Cerebellar ataxia: abnormal control of interaction torques across multiple joints. J Neuophysiol. 1996;76:492–509.
McCrea PH Eng JJ . Consequences of increased neuromotor noise for reaching movements in persons with stroke. Exp Brain Res. 2005;162:70–77. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Goodale M Milner A Jakobson L . A neurological dissociation between perceiving objects and grasping them. Nature. 1991;349:154–156. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Jakobson LS Goodale MA . Factors affecting higher-order movement planning: a kinematic analysis of human prehension. Exp Brain Res. 1991;86:199–208. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Kudoh N Hattori M Numata N Maruyama K . An analysis of spatiotemporal variability during prehension movements: effects of object size and distance. Exp Brain Res. 1997;117:457–464. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Jeannerod M . The timing of natural prehension movements. J Mot Behav. 1984;16:235–254. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Marotta J Behrmann M Goodale M . The removal of binocular cues disrupts the calibration of grasping in patients with visual form agnosia. Exp Brain Res. 1997;116:113–121. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
González-Alvarez C Subramanian A Pardhan S . Reaching and grasping with restricted peripheral vision. Ophthal Physiol Opt. 2007;27:265–274. [CrossRef]
Mon-Williams M Dijkerman H . The use of vergence information in the programming of prehension. Exp Brain Res. 1999;128:578–582. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Ketcham C Seidler RD Van Gemmert WA . Age-related kinematic differences as influenced by task difficulty, target size, and movement amplitude. J Gerontol. 2002;57B:P54–P64. [CrossRef]
Subramanian A Dickinson C . Size constancy in visual impairment: a comparison with normally sighted individuals. Ophthal Physiol Opt. 2004;24:350–354. [CrossRef]
Levi DM Klein SA Yap YL . Positional uncertainty in peripheral and amblyopic vision. Vis Res. 1987;27:581–597. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Carmeli E Patish H Coleman R . The aging hand. J Gerontol. 2003;58A:146–152. [CrossRef]
Walker N Philbin D Fisk A . Age-related differences in movement control: adjusting submovement structure to optimize performance. J Gerontol. 1997;52B:P40–P52. [CrossRef]
Oliver I Hay L Bard C Fleury M . Age-related differences in the reaching and grasping coordination in children: unimanual and bimanual tasks. Exp Brain Res. 2007;179:17–27. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Schuchard RA . Retinal correspondence and binocular perception characteristics in people with biocular central scotomas. Washington, DC: Second National Department of Veterans Affairs Rehabilitation Research and Development; 2000:90. Abstract C-68.
Brown LE Halpert BA Goodale MA . Peripheral vision for perception and action. Exp Brain Res. 2005;165:97–106. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Arimura E Matsumoto C Okuyama S . Quantification of metamorphopsia in a macular hole patient using M-CHARTS. Acta Ophthalmol Scand. 2007;85:55–59. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Kroyer K Christensen U Larsen M la Cour M . Quantification of metamorphopsia in patients with macular hole. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2008;49:3741–3746. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Mavrakanas NA Dang-Burgener NPL Lorincz EN Landis T Safran AB . Perceptual distortion in homonymous paracentral scotomas. J Neuro-Ophthalmol. 2009;29:37–42. [CrossRef]
Bulakowski P Post R Whitney D . Visuomotor crowding: the resolution of grasping in cluttered scenes. Front Behav Neurosci. 2009;3:1–7. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Mon Williams M Tresilian J Coppard V Carson R . The effect of obstacle position on reach-to-grasp movements. Exp Brain Res. 2001;137:497–501. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1.
 
Apparatus for measuring reach-to-grasp movements. (A) Visor obscures subject's vision before trial. Block placed 40 cm from subject's hand. (B) Visor lifts and subject reaches for block. The block is shown tilted slightly off the subject's midline to show that block sides were black for high contrast.
Figure 1.
 
Apparatus for measuring reach-to-grasp movements. (A) Visor obscures subject's vision before trial. Block placed 40 cm from subject's hand. (B) Visor lifts and subject reaches for block. The block is shown tilted slightly off the subject's midline to show that block sides were black for high contrast.
Figure 2.
 
Plot of thumb velocity (left axis, solid line) and grip aperture (right axis, dashed line) versus time for one 40-cm reach trial of a control subject. The visor flipped up at 0 second, allowing vision of the block.
Figure 2.
 
Plot of thumb velocity (left axis, solid line) and grip aperture (right axis, dashed line) versus time for one 40-cm reach trial of a control subject. The visor flipped up at 0 second, allowing vision of the block.
Figure 3.
 
Binocular and monocular trajectories of a control subject (top plots) and a scotoma subject (bottom plots) for 40- and 20-cm reaches. Three-dimensional trajectories are shown as seen from above. Each plot shows trajectories of five reaches. Th, thumb trajectories; In, index finger trajectories.
Figure 3.
 
Binocular and monocular trajectories of a control subject (top plots) and a scotoma subject (bottom plots) for 40- and 20-cm reaches. Three-dimensional trajectories are shown as seen from above. Each plot shows trajectories of five reaches. Th, thumb trajectories; In, index finger trajectories.
Figure 4.
 
Averages of fifteen 40-cm binocular reach trajectories of a control and scotoma subject. Vertical lines and horizontal lines show vertical and horizontal +1 SD, respectively. The SDs have been multiplied by 5 for visibility.
Figure 4.
 
Averages of fifteen 40-cm binocular reach trajectories of a control and scotoma subject. Vertical lines and horizontal lines show vertical and horizontal +1 SD, respectively. The SDs have been multiplied by 5 for visibility.
Figure 5.
 
Box plots comparing the directness ratios of control and scotoma subjects for binocular and monocular 20- and 40-cm reaches. In this and subsequent box plots, lower and upper borders of each box represent the 25th and 75th percentiles, respectively. Error bars show 10th and 90th percentiles. Solid horizontal line in boxes: medians. Asterisks: statistically significant differences (P < 0.05). Medians of control subjects' 40-cm reaches are on the lower (25%) edges of the binocular and monocular boxes.
Figure 5.
 
Box plots comparing the directness ratios of control and scotoma subjects for binocular and monocular 20- and 40-cm reaches. In this and subsequent box plots, lower and upper borders of each box represent the 25th and 75th percentiles, respectively. Error bars show 10th and 90th percentiles. Solid horizontal line in boxes: medians. Asterisks: statistically significant differences (P < 0.05). Medians of control subjects' 40-cm reaches are on the lower (25%) edges of the binocular and monocular boxes.
Figure 6.
 
Visual reaction times of control and scotoma subjects for monocular and binocular 20- and 40-cm reaches. Asterisks: statistically significant differences (P < 0.05).
Figure 6.
 
Visual reaction times of control and scotoma subjects for monocular and binocular 20- and 40-cm reaches. Asterisks: statistically significant differences (P < 0.05).
Figure 7.
 
Median movement duration for control and scotoma subjects for binocular and monocular 20- and 40-cm reaches. Asterisks: statistically significant differences (P < 0.05).
Figure 7.
 
Median movement duration for control and scotoma subjects for binocular and monocular 20- and 40-cm reaches. Asterisks: statistically significant differences (P < 0.05).
Figure 8.
 
Median maximum velocities for control and scotoma subjects with binocular and monocular viewing and 20- and 40-cm reaches. Asterisks: statistically significant differences (P < 0.05).
Figure 8.
 
Median maximum velocities for control and scotoma subjects with binocular and monocular viewing and 20- and 40-cm reaches. Asterisks: statistically significant differences (P < 0.05).
Figure 9.
 
Time to maximum velocity minus visual reaction time (i.e., time to maximum velocity from the start of hand motion). Asterisks: statistically significant differences (P < 0.05).
Figure 9.
 
Time to maximum velocity minus visual reaction time (i.e., time to maximum velocity from the start of hand motion). Asterisks: statistically significant differences (P < 0.05).
Figure 10.
 
Duration (ms) of deceleration phase of reach-to-grasp movements. Asterisks: statistically significant differences (P < 0.05).
Figure 10.
 
Duration (ms) of deceleration phase of reach-to-grasp movements. Asterisks: statistically significant differences (P < 0.05).
Figure 11.
 
Regression lines (solid lines) and 95% confidence intervals (dashed lines) for MGA versus block width for control and scotoma subjects for (A) binocular and (B) monocular viewing. Three block widths were used: 36.4, 54.8, and 77.4 mm (vertical dashed lines). Regression line slope is the grip scale factor. Data points were omitted for clarity.
Figure 11.
 
Regression lines (solid lines) and 95% confidence intervals (dashed lines) for MGA versus block width for control and scotoma subjects for (A) binocular and (B) monocular viewing. Three block widths were used: 36.4, 54.8, and 77.4 mm (vertical dashed lines). Regression line slope is the grip scale factor. Data points were omitted for clarity.
Figure 12.
 
Time to MGA from initiation of hand movement. 1, widest width block, 77.4 mm; 2, medium width block, 54.8 mm; 3, narrowest width block, 36.4 mm.
Figure 12.
 
Time to MGA from initiation of hand movement. 1, widest width block, 77.4 mm; 2, medium width block, 54.8 mm; 3, narrowest width block, 36.4 mm.
Figure 13.
 
SLO retinal map of a scotoma subject's right retina. Irregular black line encloses area of dense scotoma. Large ellipse represents the normal foveal fixation area (P = 0.90 bivariate normal ellipse). Normal foveal fixation area shows the estimated position of the subject's nonfunctional fovea with a 90% probability. Small ellipse represents the subject's PRL (P = 0.68 bivariate normal ellipse). Dots in centers of ellipses are ellipse centroids. The image has been rotated so that the top of the image is visually up and the left of the image is visually left.
Figure 13.
 
SLO retinal map of a scotoma subject's right retina. Irregular black line encloses area of dense scotoma. Large ellipse represents the normal foveal fixation area (P = 0.90 bivariate normal ellipse). Normal foveal fixation area shows the estimated position of the subject's nonfunctional fovea with a 90% probability. Small ellipse represents the subject's PRL (P = 0.68 bivariate normal ellipse). Dots in centers of ellipses are ellipse centroids. The image has been rotated so that the top of the image is visually up and the left of the image is visually left.
Figure 14.
 
(A) Regression plot of MGA minus block width versus PRL bivariate normal ellipse area (P = 0.68) for scotoma subjects with monocular viewing. (B) Regression plot of visual reaction time versus visual acuity of the tested dominant eye for scotoma subjects with monocular viewing. Regression lines for 20- and 40-cm reaches were not significantly different (t-tests, P > 0.05) and were combined.
Figure 14.
 
(A) Regression plot of MGA minus block width versus PRL bivariate normal ellipse area (P = 0.68) for scotoma subjects with monocular viewing. (B) Regression plot of visual reaction time versus visual acuity of the tested dominant eye for scotoma subjects with monocular viewing. Regression lines for 20- and 40-cm reaches were not significantly different (t-tests, P > 0.05) and were combined.
Table 1.
 
Subjects' Ages and Visual Acuities
Table 1.
 
Subjects' Ages and Visual Acuities
Scotoma Subjects Control Subjects
Age (y) Duration (y) Visual Acuity (min arc) Age (y) Visual Acuity (min arc)
RE LE BE RE LE BE
88 7 20.0 20.0 20.0 73 1.0 1.0 1.0
78 4 3.16 20.0 3.16 82 1.58 1.26 1.26
84 6 10.0 10.0 10.0 77 1.0 1.0 0.79
80 7 10.0 10.0 10.0 87 1.58 1.0 1.0
81 6 7.94 7.94 7.94 89 1.58 1.26 1.26
84 6 10.0 5.01 5.01 79 1.0 1.0 1.0
87 5 2.51 5.01 3.98 70 1.0 0.79 0.79
75 8 5.02 15.89 10.02 78 1.0 0.79 0.79
75 10 7.94 10.0 5.01 74 1.0 1.58 1.0
82 10 10.0 15.89 15.89 74 1.0 0.79 0.79
81.4 6.9 8.66 11.97 9.10 78.3 1.17 1.05 0.97
Table 2.
 
Percentage Increase (Positive Values) or Decrease (Negative Values) of Reach and Grasp Parameters of Scotoma Subjects Relative to Control Subjects
Table 2.
 
Percentage Increase (Positive Values) or Decrease (Negative Values) of Reach and Grasp Parameters of Scotoma Subjects Relative to Control Subjects
Scotoma Type Reach Component Grasp Component
Reach Distance (cm) Reaction Time Movement Duration Maximum Velocity Time to Maximum Velocity Deceleration Duration Object Width (cm) MGA Time to MGA
Peripheral
    Artificial (binocular) 16 30 41 −20 −4 82 2.5 4 28
    Artificial (binocular) 17 * 30 9 −8 −3 32 3.0 0 2
    Glaucoma (binocular) 18 20 18 16 −2 13 17
40 0 14 17 4.8 2 8
2.4 6 10
    Artificial (monocular) 34 (36, 68) 10§ −10 3 12 3 5
4‖ −11 0 3 2
    Glaucoma (monocular) 18 20 26 25 −2 10 30
40 0 12 30 4.8 0 8
2.4 0 14
Central
    Artificial (binocular) 16 30 44 −21 30 53 2.5 36 29
    AMD (binocular)‡ 20 31 29 −16 15 54 7.7 0 39
5.5 0 24
3.6 0 18
40 36 31 −18 3 44 7.7 0 30
5.5 0 29
3.6 0 25
    AMD (monocular)‡ 20 26 37 −26 6 51 7.7 9 36
5.5 5 36
3.6 8 27
40 26 54 −28 26 66 7.7 7 64
5.5 5 51
3.6 9 44
Range, all studies
    Peripheral 18–26 4–41 0–(−20) −4–14 12–82 2–6 2–28
    Central 26–36 29–54 −16–(−28) 26–30 44–66 5–36 18–64
Table 3.
 
Slope of Regression Line Fit to MGA versus Object Width (mm/mm)
Table 3.
 
Slope of Regression Line Fit to MGA versus Object Width (mm/mm)
Authors Subject Mean Age (y) n Object Width (cm) Binocular Slope Monocular Slope
Goodale et al. 29 1 2.5–5 0.460
1 2.5–5 0.810
Servos et al. 13 22.6 9 2–5 0.756 0.892
Servos and Goodale 1 23.5 8 2–5 0.878 0.721
Kudoh et al. 31 19–28 12 2–8 0.855
Jakobson and Goodale 30 25.3 15 2–5 0.664
Present study (controls) 78.3 10 3.4–7.7 0.345 0.177
Present study (scotoma) 81.4 10 3.4–7.7 0.367 0.296
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×