Spatial attention can be defined as the selection of a location for preferential processing.
25 To investigate the role of eye deviation in spatial attention, participants were asked to detect targets presented to the left or right of fixation at equal retinal eccentricity. In addition, to test whether eye deviation affected the ability to
shift attention, the task was designed with a cue that preceded the target. The cue could appear at the location of the target (50% valid trials) or at the contralateral location (50% invalid trials). Spatially uninformative cues test stimulus-driven, exogenous orienting (as opposed to endogenous, voluntary orienting),
26 the component of the spatial attention system that has been suggested to be most tightly linked to eye movements.
27 The task used here was similar to that used in the rTMS study
20 (
Fig. 1). Participants were seated 57 cm from a 36 × 29 cm cathode ray tube screen with their head stabilized using a chin rest and cheek pads. The midsagittal plane through the viewing eye was aligned to the central fixation square (black solid, 0.2° × 0.2°). Each trial started with presentation of a fixation square for a random period of between 20 and 500 ms. A spatially nonpredictive cue (red square frame, 0.4° × 0.4°) then appeared for 40 ms at one of two locations centered either 5° to the left or right of fixation. After a 100-ms delay, this was followed by a barely visible target (a gray solid square, 0.1° × 0.1°) presented for 80 ms. The target could appear either at the cued location, 5° from fixation (valid condition, 50% of the trials), or at the uncued location (invalid condition, 50% of the trials). A mask (red square frame, 0.4° × 0.4°) was then presented bilaterally at both possible target positions for 100 ms. The mask reduces the processing of the target,
28 increasing the difficulty of visual detection. The mask was presented bilaterally, and was therefore uninformative as to the location of the target. The participants had 1000 ms to respond before the start of a new trial. They held a small response box in their hands underneath the table, corresponding to their body midline and responded by pressing a button on the left of the box with their left thumb for targets on the left, and the right button with their right thumb for targets on the right. We instructed the participants to try to be as accurate as possible and to refrain from pressing any key if they were unsure. We calculated mean reaction time across correct responses. The hit rate was calculated as the percentage of correct responses relative to the total number of trials of that type, regardless of the participant's response (e.g., number of trials when the participant responded “left” divided by the total number of trials when the target was presented to the left side). The false positive rate was the percentage of responses indicating left when the target was presented to the right relative to the total number of trials where a target was presented to the right. Because the task was performed during sustained eye rotation using a suction scleral lens, we limited task duration to 4 minutes. Within this interval each participant completed 136 trials, 34 trials for each of the four conditions: valid cue, left target; invalid cue, left target; valid cue, right target; invalid cue, right target. Before the experiment, the participants practiced the task in monocular vision, with their dominant eye patched.