Finally, the ANOVA run on the average peak velocity did not show any effect of hand or eye dominance (
F < 1 and
F (1,41) = 2.51, NS) but revealed a main effect of target eccentricity and of saccade task (
F (2,82) = 862,
P < 0.0001;
F (3,123) = 47.47,
P < 0.0001, respectively). The peak velocity that increased with saccade size was similar for reactive saccades (Gap-0 and Gap-200: 329 ± 65°/s and 333 ± 65°/s respectively) and slower for voluntary saccades, in particular in the Antisaccade task (Overlap-600: 313 ± 60°/s and Antisaccade: 290 ± 32°/s). The effect of the saccade direction failed to reach significance (312 ± 55°/s vs. 321 ± 63°/s for leftward and rightward saccades, respectively,
F (1,41) = 2.22, NS) but interacted with eye dominance, hand dominance, and target eccentricity (
F (2,82) = 6.81,
P < 0.001). Interestingly for our purpose, the effect of saccade direction was reversed between right-eyed and left-eyed groups, irrespective of their hand dominance. For right-eyed participants, the mean peak velocity was greater for rightward saccades than for leftward saccades (RE-RH:
F (1,16) = 26.15,
P < 0.0001,
F (1,16) = 16.91,
P < 0.0008, and
F (1,16) = 20.05,
P < 0.0004, for 5°, 10°, and 15°; RE-LH:
F (1,4) = 9.46,
P < 0.03,
F (1,4) = 20.85,
P < 0.01, and
F (1,4) = 10.7,
P < 0.03, for 5°, 10°, and 15° ). For left-eyed participants, the mean peak velocity was greater for leftward saccades than for rightward saccades (LE-RH:
F (1,12) = 11.99,
P < 0.05,
F (1,11) = 11.61,
P < 0.005, and
F (1,11) = 12.60,
P < 0.004, for 5°, 10°, and 15°; LE-LH:
F (1,10) = 4.17,
P < 0.06,
F (1,10) = 13.59,
P < 0.004, and
F (1,10) = 10.94,
P < 0.007, for 5°, 10°, and 15°). Although the interaction among the five factors was not significant (
F < 1), we examined the group comparisons for each eccentricity and each saccade task (see
Fig. 3b). The effect of saccade direction was consistently found for the RE-RHs (all
P < 0.04), the LE-LHs (all
P < 0.04, except NS for 5° Gap-0; marginal for 5° and 10° Antisaccade), and the LE-RHs (all
P < 0.02, except NS for 10° Overlap-600; marginal for 5° and 15° Antisaccade). Despite the small number of subjects in the RE-LH group, the effect of saccade direction was significant in half of the conditions (all
P < 0.05) and marginal in the other half (all
P < 0.08). The examination of individual RS-LS differences clearly showed that both right-eyed groups had faster saccades toward the right than to the left (
Fig. 3b: RE-RH: Gap-200: 12/17, 12/17, 11/17 for the 5°, 10°, and 15° target; Gap-0: 14/17, 12/17, 13/17; Overlap-600: 11/17, 10/17, 11/17; Antisaccade: 11/17, 10/17, 11/17; RE-LH: Gap-200: 4/5, 5/5, 5/5; Gap-0: 4/5, 3/5, 5/5; Overlap-600: 4/5, 4/5, 5/5; Antisaccade: 3/5, 4/5, 4/5), whereas both left-eyed groups had faster saccades toward the left than the right (
Fig. 1b: LE-RH: Gap-200: 7/12, 7/12, 8/12; Gap-0: 8/12, 8/12, 6/12; Overlap-600: 5/12, 6/12, 5/12; Antisaccade: 1/12, 2/12, 2/12; LE-LH: Gap-200: 7/11, 8/11, 6/11; Gap-0: 6/11, 7/11, 7/11; Overlap-600: 5/11, 10/11, 6/11; Antisaccade: 4/11, 6/11, 6/11).