The concept of eye dominance is well entrenched in the clinical literature. It provides the foundation for a range of clinical decisions, including monovision treatment,
1 –4 contact lens wear,
5 and cataract surgery.
6 Eye dominance has a long history, having first been discussed by Rosenbach
7 in 1903 and later by Walls
8 and Berner and Berner.
9 The concepts of motor and sensory dominance have been developed through these early works, with the former being determined by motor tests, such as the Hole-in-the-Card test,
8 and the latter by relative measures of visual sensitivity
9 –12 or the relative ability of each eye to suppress processing of an image presented to the other eye during binocular rivalry paradigms.
13 –15 At present, it is fair to say that both the importance and basis of eye dominance, be it motor or sensory, is poorly understood. What is known is that measures of motor and sensory dominance do not correlate strongly within individuals.
10,12,14,16,17 This lack of correlation is in contrast to other types of lateralized dominance, such as hand dominance,
18 and raises the question of what the relevance of eye dominance might be. Since it appears that eye dominance is not determined by a more faithful input from one eye
9,10 or more efficient cortical processing of one eye's input,
19,20 there remains the possibility that its basis lies in the nature of the interaction that occurs between the eyes when both eyes are operating together (i.e., when both eyes are contributing to a fused, stable percept) as is the case in everyday viewing. A series of recent findings
9,21 –28 regarding the role of inhibitory pathways before excitatory binocular combination may hold the key to a reinterpretation of sensory eye dominance.