February 2010
Volume 51, Issue 2
Free
Cornea  |   February 2010
Human Corneal Adaptation to Mechanical, Cooling, and Chemical Stimuli
Author Affiliations & Notes
  • Jiangtao Chen
    From the School of Optometry, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada.
  • Yunwei Feng
    From the School of Optometry, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada.
  • Trefford L. Simpson
    From the School of Optometry, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada.
  • Corresponding author: Jiangtao Chen, School of Optometry, University of Waterloo, 200 University Avenue West, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada, N2L 3G1; [email protected]
Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science February 2010, Vol.51, 876-881. doi:https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.08-3072
  • Views
  • PDF
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Jiangtao Chen, Yunwei Feng, Trefford L. Simpson; Human Corneal Adaptation to Mechanical, Cooling, and Chemical Stimuli. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2010;51(2):876-881. https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.08-3072.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract

Purpose.: To psychophysically investigate adaptation in human corneas using the Belmonte pneumatic esthesiometer.

Methods.: Twenty, 8, and 20 healthy subjects were enrolled in the mechanical, cool, and chemical experiments, respectively. Thresholds were estimated using an ascending method of limits and three intensities (subthreshold, threshold, and suprathreshold, in random order) were each presented 10 or 20 times, and subjects scaled the intensity of the stimuli (0–4 [no stimulus to very intense stimulus]). Friedman nonparametric ANOVA was used to analyze the rating data.

Results.: There was measurable adaptation with both mechanical and cool stimuli. For both suprathreshold mechanical and cool stimuli, the earlier stimuli were rated more intensely than subsequent stimuli (both P < 0.05). However, this was not the case for subthreshold and threshold mechanical and cool stimuli (all P > 0.05). Paradoxically, for the chemical stimuli, there was adaptation to threshold stimuli (P = 0.03) but no adaptation for subthreshold and suprathreshold stimuli (P = 0.19 and 0.11, respectively).

Conclusions.: Both mechanical (mechanosensory or polymodal) and cold receptors on human corneas show adaptation to repeated suprathreshold stimuli with a reduction in perceived intensity after multiple exposures to the same physical stimulus intensity. This is in accord with the results found in electrophysiological and psychophysical experiments of somatosensation elsewhere in the body (and in other animals). The response to chemical stimuli was different, and this might reflect proximal and distal neural or stimulus-specific effects.

Adaptation is “the decrease in the magnitude of sensation or in neural response during a sustained stimulation at a constant intensity” 1 that reflects function of both the peripheral and the central neural pathways. Adaptation has been demonstrated in sensory systems, such as olfaction, 2,3 audition, 4 and the somatosensory systems in the tooth pulp 5,6 and skin. 1,7 It is believed to maintain a sensory system's sensitivity over a wide dynamic range. 8 On the other hand, sensitization—the increase of magnitude of sensation to a sustained stimulus—has been observed after repeated high-intensity mechanical 9,10 or thermal 1,7,11 stimulation. It is hypothesized to initiate protective activities in the nervous system to limit further injury. 12 Therefore, under certain conditions, either adaptation or sensitization could occur to repeated noxious stimulation. 7,11,13,14 In the eye, adaptation to repeated mechanical stimulation and sensitization to noxious thermal stimulation have been demonstrated. 1517  
Corneal sensory processing is a subset of somatosensation. 18 The cornea has the densest innervation in the body, 19 and free nerve endings are the only terminals in the human corneal epithelium, with evenly distributed dense innervation in the central two-thirds of the cornea. 20 The major nerve fiber types, Aδ- and C-, related to nociception, 2022 are the primary innervators of the cornea. Perhaps for this reason the cornea has, until recently, been considered an organ experiencing only nociceptive sensations. 22 Recently, there have been demonstrations that stimulation of the cornea can produce innocuous sensations, such as cooling. 17,2326  
Despite the strong nociceptive corneal innervation pattern, there have been no systematic studies of adaptation and sensitization of human corneas. Therefore, we chose to systemically examine psychophysical adaptation or sensitization patterns to mechanical, thermal (cooling), and chemical stimuli on the human cornea using a Belmonte pneumatic esthesiometer. Stimuli were chosen that we hypothesized would have little effect (subthreshold and threshold intensities) or would produce adaptation (suprathreshold intensities). This stimulus selection is similar to that used in adaptation experiments in vision. 27  
Subjects and Methods
Subjects
Subjects were selected according to the following criteria: None had a history of eye disease, systemic disease, or dry eye symptoms. Contact lens wear was not an exclusion criterion, but lens wearers had to be asymptomatic, have noninvasive tear breakup time of at least 10 seconds, and use only silicon hydrogel lenses. In mechanical, chemical, and cooling stimulus phases, we enrolled 20, 20, and 8 healthy subjects (age range, 21–46 years) respectively. There were four contact lens wearers in the mechanical, four in the chemical, and two in the cooling phases, respectively. Most subjects were the same in the three stimulus phases except that eight subjects in the chemical experiment did not participate in the mechanical or cooling experiments. When subjects participated in multiple phases, intervals between participation lasted at least 1 week, and the order of participation was randomized. This study adhered to the Declaration of Helsinki for research involving human subjects and received clearance from the University of Waterloo Office of Research Ethics. Informed consent was signed before enrollment. 
Instruments
Stimuli were delivered using a computer-controlled Belmonte pneumatic esthesiometer, with temperature, flow, and CO2 proportion monitored and automatically regulated. In addition, the computer collected subject responses and calculated the stimuli based on these inputs. A more detailed description of this instrument can be found in previous reports. 28,29  
Psychophysical Methods
Thresholds to mechanical, chemical, and thermal (cooling) stimuli were measured using an ascending method of limits. The final threshold was the average of 6 “yes” responses. In order for subjects to unambiguously detect the chemical stimulus at detection threshold, chemical thresholds were measured using a flow rate of half the mechanical threshold. 
Subjects then participated in three stimulus intensity sessions in random order (subthreshold, threshold, and suprathreshold). In the mechanical and cooling experiments, subthreshold and suprathreshold were 25% below and above threshold, respectively, whereas in the chemical experiments, they were 50% below and above threshold, respectively (these intensities were based on pilot data). 
In each stimulus intensity session, 20 equal-intensity, 2-second trials were presented, with 10-second interstimulus interval (ISI) for mechanical and cooling stimuli 29,30 and 30-second ISI in the chemical experiment. 30,31 Subjects were asked to use a 5-point intensity rating scale after each stimulus: 0, no stimulus; 1, very mild stimulus; 2, mild stimulus; 3, moderately strong stimulus; and 4, strong stimulus. Subjects reported responses by using a computer button box. Pilot experimentation showed that this scale produced a spread of ratings, with “1” being the modal response to threshold stimulation. Given that detectability is a statistical concept, 32 subthreshold stimuli, perceived occasionally, could still be rated lower than 1, if necessary, minimizing biased results because of a floor effect. The experimental stimulation and response sequence are illustrated in Figure 1  
Figure 1.
 
The experimental sequence. Interstimulus intervals were 10 seconds for cooling and mechanical stimulation and 30 seconds for chemical stimulation.
Figure 1.
 
The experimental sequence. Interstimulus intervals were 10 seconds for cooling and mechanical stimulation and 30 seconds for chemical stimulation.
Each subject received up to three training sessions, but 80% received only one or two sessions because during the training, their responses to the threshold stimuli were repeatable. If subjects were unable to give stable results after three training sessions, they were discontinued from the experiment and replaced. One subject from the mechanical and three subjects from the chemical experiments were excluded; no subjects were excluded during the cooling experiments. There was a 5-minute break between sessions. Half the subjects in the mechanical experiment also received an additional suprathreshold (mechanical) session with a 20-second ISI. 
Statistical Analysis
Sample sizes were chosen using data derived during the first part of each experiment that served as a pilot phase. Power calculations were based on the effect size of the difference between the “unadapted” and “adapted” intervals (and assuming α = 0.05 and β = 0.2 [power = 80%]). In two experiments (mechanical and cooling), this occurred during suprathreshold stimulation. For the third (chemical) experiment, this occurred with threshold stimulation. 
Five sequential stimulus ratings were averaged; hence, there were four periods in each session. Because these data were derived from an ordinal rating scale, Friedman nonparametric ANOVA was used to evaluate the statistical significance of time after each experimental session began (set at P ≤ 0.05). 
Results
Mechanical Stimulation
Figure 2a shows the average ratings during the four time periods in the mechanical, subthreshold stimulus intensity session. Subjects' average intensity ratings were below 1 in each period, with no significant change during this session (Friedman nonparametric ANOVA; P = 0.86). When the stimulus intensity was at threshold, as illustrated in Figure 2b, there was also no change in intensity ratings (Friedman nonparametric ANOVA; P = 0.15). However, as shown in Figure 2c, when the mechanical stimulus intensity was suprathreshold, the subjects' ratings gradually decreased in the later periods (Friedman nonparametric ANOVA; P = 0.004). Figure 2d shows that there was no effect of time on subjects' ratings of multiple suprathreshold stimulation when the ISI was 20 seconds (Friedman nonparametric ANOVA; P = 0.42). 
Figure 2.
 
(a) Subthreshold mechanical stimulation with 10-second ISI, Friedman nonparametric ANOVA; P = 0.86. (b) Threshold mechanical stimulation with 10-second ISI; P = 0.15. (c) Suprathreshold mechanical stimulation with 10-second ISI; P = 0.004. (d) Suprathreshold mechanical stimulation with 20-second ISI; P = 0.42.
Figure 2.
 
(a) Subthreshold mechanical stimulation with 10-second ISI, Friedman nonparametric ANOVA; P = 0.86. (b) Threshold mechanical stimulation with 10-second ISI; P = 0.15. (c) Suprathreshold mechanical stimulation with 10-second ISI; P = 0.004. (d) Suprathreshold mechanical stimulation with 20-second ISI; P = 0.42.
Cooling Stimulation
Figures 3a and b show that there was no significant change in intensity ratings during sequential cooling stimulus periods with subthreshold and threshold stimulation (Friedman nonparametric ANOVA; P = 0.88 and 0.27, respectively). Figure 3c shows that there was significant reduction in ratings during four sequential periods with suprathreshold cooling stimuli (Friedman nonparametric ANOVA; P = 0.004). 
Figure 3.
 
(a) Subthreshold cooling stimulation with 10-second ISI, Friedman nonparametric ANOVA; P = 0.88. (b) Threshold cooling stimulation with 10-seconds ISI; P = 0.27. (c) Suprathreshold cooling stimulation with 10-second ISI; P = 0.004.
Figure 3.
 
(a) Subthreshold cooling stimulation with 10-second ISI, Friedman nonparametric ANOVA; P = 0.88. (b) Threshold cooling stimulation with 10-seconds ISI; P = 0.27. (c) Suprathreshold cooling stimulation with 10-second ISI; P = 0.004.
Chemical Stimulation
Figure 4a shows no change in the average intensity ratings during repeated subthreshold chemical stimulation (ISI 30 seconds, Friedman nonparametric ANOVA P = 0.19). Figure 4b shows that there was a significant decrease in intensity ratings with repeated threshold stimulation (Friedman nonparametric ANOVA; P = 0.03). Figure 4c shows that the average ratings in the sequential periods of repeated suprathreshold stimulation did not change (Friedman nonparametric ANOVA; P = 0.11). 
Figure 4.
 
(a) Subthreshold chemical stimulation with 30-second ISI; Friedman nonparametric ANOVA; P = 0.19. (b) Threshold chemical stimulation with 30-second ISI; P = 0.03. (c) Suprathreshold chemical stimulation with 30-second ISI; P = 0.11.
Figure 4.
 
(a) Subthreshold chemical stimulation with 30-second ISI; Friedman nonparametric ANOVA; P = 0.19. (b) Threshold chemical stimulation with 30-second ISI; P = 0.03. (c) Suprathreshold chemical stimulation with 30-second ISI; P = 0.11.
Discussion
To our knowledge, this is the first report of human corneal surface adaptation to mechanical, cold, and chemical stimuli, delivered with a modified Belmonte aesthesiometer. 
The result demonstrated that subjective ratings of intensity changed over time, with more intense stimuli (suprathreshold) often producing significant reductions in perceived intensity; this appears to be adaptation in the classic sense. 1,37,33 However, the ratings of suprathreshold mechanical, chemical, and thermally cold stimulation were not consistent. The response to chemical stimuli appeared to differ from the other two modes of stimulation. As hypothesized, there was adaptation to the suprathreshold mechanical and cooling stimuli or in the threshold session in the chemical experiment. This difference might reflect a difference in the pneumatic stimuli and how they interact with the tear film and ocular surface, differences between receptor types and underlying sensory channels, functional differences in the central nervous system, or combinations of these. 
In the cornea, two types of neurons—mechanosensory and polymodal—are sensitive to mechanical stimulation 17 ; both show adaptation to repeated mechanical stimulation. 1517 The mechanosensory neurons have slightly higher mechanical thresholds with more apparent adaptation to repeated stimulation than polymodal neurons. 15,17 In our study, suprathreshold mechanical stimuli would have been expected to activate both of these nociceptive fiber types and perhaps caused adaptation. However, in the subthreshold and threshold stimulus intensity sessions, it is likely that only the more sensitive polymodal neurons would have been activated (if at all), inducing no adaptation. 
In addition to adaptation to mechanical stimulation, subjects showed adaptation to room temperature (cooling) pneumatic stimuli; the ratings decreased dramatically after repeated suprathreshold stimuli. A similar phenomenon has also been observed in the skin, 34,35 and it has been proposed that the unmyelinated C-fibers more readily develop adaptation, 1,7,11,36 perhaps because of the peculiar membrane processes of the very thin nerve fibers and endings. 7,37 In the human cornea, all cold and 70% of polymodal neurons are C-fibers. 17,20,38 If similar mechanisms operate in the skin as in the cornea, it seems reasonable to conclude that peripheral neural adaptation may play a major role in the adaptation we demonstrated in mechanical and cooling experiments. 
Putative central adaptation mechanism could also play a role in our experiment. Subjects' arousal has been shown to decrease with decreased pain intensity when repeated stimuli caused “bearable” experimental dental pain. 5 Our suprathreshold mechanical stimuli caused bearable sensations (Fig. 2); if a similar central arousal reduction occurred, this might have resulted in the decreased intensity reported by subjects. 3941 It has also been reported that cooling stimulation can also activate nociceptive pathways. 42 Therefore, a similar reduction in arousal status during the cooling experiment might also be invoked to account for the cooling results. 3941  
The effect of the corneal chemical stimulation is presumably different from mechanical and cooling stimuli. Chemical stimulation arises because CO2 in the pneumatic stimulus dissolves in the tears to generate local tear film (therefore, presumably corneal) areas that are acidic. 1517,31 Repeated CO2 stimulation causes even more irritation of the ocular surface and often leads to unpleasant sensations indicative of polymodal nociceptor sensitization after stimulation. 43,44 In the skin, acidosis causes sensitization. 45,46 On the other hand, taste receptors adapt to acid. 47,48 According to a demonstration of adaptation to acidic stimulation in a cat's corneal polymodal nociceptors, 16 we speculated that subjects would show the same adaptation to suprathreshold stimuli shown with mechanical and cooling stimulation, but this was not found. There was significant reduction in reported intensity with repeated threshold stimulation. There are a number of possible indirect explanations for this lack of adaptation. The first had little directly to do with sensory processing of the actual stimulus. Because subjects responded to suprathreshold chemical stimulation with blinking, blepharospasm, and increased reflex tearing, 49 it was possible that these would minimize the neural effect of protracted stimulation of the nociceptors that, presumably, were partly responsible for the adaptation effects with other stimuli. A second possibility giving rise to an apparent lack of adaptation to suprathreshold stimuli is that sensitization could follow suppression after a recovery period. 50 Hence, in the chemical experiment, the longer ISI might have allowed the C-fibers to operate normally and might have caused less adaptation. Similar results have been found in a human cutaneous heat pain perception study with shorter ISI causing adaptation. 7,10 Third, a number of possible neurophysiological changes might have occurred. These include the activation of deep (“silent”) additional C-nociceptors after repeated suprathreshold stimulation, 16,5153 the interaction between chemical and mechanical channels (similar to that demonstrated in the skin of rats 54 and psychophysically in humans 44 ), and corneal nociceptive receptive field in the spinal trigeminal complex increasing in size after noxious chemical corneal stimulation. 24 Each of these might effectively result in sensitization that might counteract any reduction in sensitivity because of adaptation. Finally, a central mechanism related to the anticipation of pain could activate the cortical nociceptive system. 5558 After experiencing a distinct uncomfortable sensation from chemical stimulation and, perhaps, the peculiarity of the stimulus, a top-down mechanism was triggered to facilitate nociception 59 that could counteract any reduction in the effectiveness of chemical stimuli because of adaptation. Except for the reduction in the effectiveness of the stimuli from tear film alterations, why these mechanisms would occur only for the chemical stimuli and not for the others is not apparent. 
This study has provided evidence that the human cornea is similar in its somatosensory processing to other parts of the body, with adaptation to suprathreshold nociceptive mechanical and nonnociceptive cooling stimuli when the inter-stimulus interval is short (10 seconds). Response to chemical stimulation is more complex, perhaps reflecting pneumatic stimulus peculiarities and peripheral neural and cortical factors. Adaptation of the corneal surface to the stimuli, especially mechanical, might have direct implication in the understanding of dry eye symptoms and adaptation to contact lens wear. 
In conclusion, we found that the human corneal surface showed adaptation to repeated suprathreshold mechanical and cold stimuli but not to chemical stimuli (CO2). 
Footnotes
 Supported by Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada and Canada Foundation for Innovation.
Footnotes
 Disclosure: J. Chen, None; Y. Feng, None; T.L. Simpson, None
The authors thank the subjects who participated in the study. 
References
LaMotte RH Campbell JN . Comparison of responses of warm and nociceptive C-fiber afferents in monkey with human judgments of thermal pain. J Neurophysiol. 1978; 41: 509–528. [PubMed]
Dalton P . Psychophysical and behavioral characteristics of olfactory adaptation. Chem Senses. 2000; 25: 487–492. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Adelman JP Herson PS . Making scents of olfactory adaptation. Nat Neurosci. 2004; 7: 689–690. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Ohman A Lader M . Selective attention and “habituation” of the auditory averaged evoked response in humans. Physiol Behav. 1972; 8: 79–85. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Condes-Lara M Calvo JM Fernandez-Guardiola A . Habituation to bearable experimental pain elicited by tooth pulp electrical stimulation. Pain. 1981; 11: 185–200. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Ernst M Lee MH Dworkin B Zaretsky HH . Pain perception decrement produced through repeated stimulation. Pain. 1986; 26: 221–231. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Adriaensen H Gybels J Handwerker HO Van Hees J . Suppression of C-fibre discharges upon repeated heat stimulation may explain characteristics of concomitant pain sensations. Brain Res. 1984; 302: 203–211. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Honore E Patel AJ Chemin J Suchyna T Sachs F . Desensitization of mechano-gated K2P channels. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2006; 103: 6859–6864. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Adriaensen H Gybels J Handwerker HO Van Hees J . Nociceptor discharges and sensations due to prolonged noxious mechanical stimulation—a paradox. Hum Neurobiol. 1984; 3: 53–58. [PubMed]
Milne RJ Kay NE Irwin RJ . Habituation to repeated painful and non-painful cutaneous stimuli: a quantitative psychophysical study. Exp Brain Res. 1991; 87: 438–444. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Kleinbohl D Trojan J Konrad C Holzl R . Sensitization and habituation of AMH and C-fiber related percepts of repetitive radiant heat stimulation. Clin Neurophysiol. 2006; 117: 118–130. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Walters ET . Injury-related behavior and neuronal plasticity: an evolutionary perspective on sensitization, hyperalgesia, and analgesia. Int Rev Neurobiol. 1994; 36: 325–427. [PubMed]
Woolf CJ . Evidence for a central component of post-injury pain hypersensitivity. Nature. 1983; 306: 686–688. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Treede RD Meyer RA Raja SN Campbell JN . Peripheral and central mechanisms of cutaneous hyperalgesia. Prog Neurobiol. 1992; 38: 397–421. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Belmonte C Giraldez F . Responses of cat corneal sensory receptors to mechanical and thermal stimulation. J Physiol. 1981; 321: 355–368. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Belmonte C Gallar J Pozo MA Rebollo I . Excitation by irritant chemical substances of sensory afferent units in the cat's cornea. J Physiol. 1991; 437: 709–725. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Belmonte C Garcia-Hirschfeld J Gallar J . Neurobiology of ocular pain. Prog Retinal Eye Res. 1997; 16: 117–156. [CrossRef]
Belmonte C Tervo T . Pain in and around the eyes. In: McMahon S Koltzenburg M eds. Wall and Melzacks Textbook of Pain. Philadelphia: Elsevier/Churchill Livingstone; 2005: 887–901.
Rozsa AJ Beuerman RW . Density and organization of free nerve endings in the corneal epithelium of the rabbit. Pain. 1982; 14: 105–120. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Muller LJ Vrensen GF Pels L Cardozo BN Willekens B . Architecture of human corneal nerves. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1997; 38: 985–994. [PubMed]
Beuerman RW Tanelian DL . Corneal pain evoked by thermal stimulation. Pain. 1979; 7: 1–14. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Giraldez F Geijo E Belmonte C . Response characteristics of corneal sensory fibers to mechanical and thermal stimulation. Brain Res. 1979; 177: 571–576. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Vega JA Simpson TL Fonn D . A noncontact pneumatic esthesiometer for measurement of ocular sensitivity: a preliminary report. Cornea. 1999; 18: 675–681. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Pozo MA Cervero F . Neurons in the rat spinal trigeminal complex driven by corneal nociceptors: receptive-field properties and effects of noxious stimulation of the cornea. J Neurophysiol. 1993; 70: 2370–2378. [PubMed]
Nishida Y Yokota T . Corneal representation within the trigeminal subnucleus caudalis and adjacent bulbar lateral reticular formation of the cat. Jpn J Physiol. 1991; 41: 551–565. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Murphy PJ Patel S Morgan PB Marshall J . The minimum stimulus energy required to produce a cooling sensation in the human cornea. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 2001; 21: 407–410. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Witkovsky P . Excitation and adaptation in the vertebrate retina. Curr Top Eye Res. 1980; 2: 1–66. [PubMed]
Feng Y Simpson TL . Characteristics of human corneal psychophysical channels. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2004; 45: 3005–3010. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Situ P Simpson TL Fonn D . Eccentric variation of corneal sensitivity to pneumatic stimulation at different temperatures and with CO2 . Exp Eye Res. 2007; 85: 400–405. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Situ P Simpson T Fonn D Jones LW . Conjunctival and corneal pneumatic sensitivity is associated with signs and symptoms of ocular dryness. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2008; 49: 2971–2976. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Chen X Gallar J Pozo MA Baeza M Belmonte C . CO2 stimulation of the cornea: a comparison between human sensation and nerve activity in polymodal nociceptive afferents of the cat. Eur J Neurosci. 1995; 7: 1154–1163. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Gescheider GA . Psychophysics: The Fundamentals. Philadelphia: Psychology Press; 1997.
Cain WS Drexler M . Scope and evaluation of odor counteraction and masking. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1974; 237: 427–439. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Campero M Serra J Bostock H Ochoa JL . Slowly conducting afferents activated by innocuous low temperature in human skin. J Physiol. 2001; 535: 855–865. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Darian-Smith I Johnson KO Dykes R . “Cold” fiber population innervating palmar and digital skin of the monkey: responses to cooling pulses. J Neurophysiol. 1973; 36: 325–346. [PubMed]
Torebjork HE Hallin RG . Responses in human A and C fibres to repeated electrical intradermal stimulation. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1974; 37: 653–664. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Peng YB Ringkamp M Meyer RA Campbell JN . Fatigue and paradoxical enhancement of heat response in C-fiber nociceptors from cross-modal excitation. J Neurosci. 2003; 23: 4766–4774. [PubMed]
Gallar J Pozo MA Tuckett RP Belmonte C . Response of sensory units with unmyelinated fibres to mechanical, thermal and chemical stimulation of the cat's cornea. J Physiol. 1993; 468: 609–622. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Tracey I Ploghaus A Gati JS . Imaging attentional modulation of pain in the periaqueductal gray in humans. J Neurosci. 2002; 22: 2748–2752. [PubMed]
Bantick SJ Wise RG Ploghaus A Clare S Smith SM Tracey I . Imaging how attention modulates pain in humans using functional MRI. Brain. 2002; 125: 310–319. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Bushnell MC Duncan GH Dubner R Jones RL Maixner W . Attentional influences on noxious and innocuous cutaneous heat detection in humans and monkeys. J Neurosci. 1985; 5: 1103–1110. [PubMed]
Green BG Pope JV . Innocuous cooling can produce nociceptive sensations that are inhibited during dynamic mechanical contact. Exp Brain Res. 2003; 148: 290–299. [PubMed]
Acosta MC Belmonte C Gallar J . Sensory experiences in humans and single-unit activity in cats evoked by polymodal stimulation of the cornea. J Physiol. 2001; 534: 511–525. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Feng Y Simpson TL . Nociceptive sensation and sensitivity evoked from human cornea and conjunctiva stimulated by CO2 . Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2003; 44: 529–532. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Steen KH Reeh PW . Sustained graded pain and hyperalgesia from harmless experimental tissue acidosis in human skin. Neurosci Lett. 1993; 154: 113–116. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Bevan S Geppetti P . Protons: small stimulants of capsaicin-sensitive sensory nerves. Trends Neurosci. 1994; 17: 509–512. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
DeSimone JA Lyall V . Taste receptors in the gastrointestinal tract III: salty and sour taste: sensing of sodium and protons by the tongue. Am J Physiol Gastrointest Liver Physiol. 2006; 291: G1005–G1010. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Lyall V Alam RI Phan TH Phan DQ Heck GL DeSimone JA . Excitation and adaptation in the detection of hydrogen ions by taste receptor cells: a role for cAMP and Ca(2+). J Neurophysiol. 2002; 87: 399–408. [PubMed]
Acosta MC Peral A Luna C Pintor J Belmonte C Gallar J . Tear secretion induced by selective stimulation of corneal and conjunctival sensory nerve fibers. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2004; 45: 2333–2336. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Torebjork HE Hallin RG . Recordings of impulses in unmyelinated nerve fibres in man; afferent C fibre activity. Acta Anaesthesiol Scand Suppl. 1978; 70: 124–129. [PubMed]
MacIver MB Tanelian DL . Structural and functional specialization of A delta and C fiber free nerve endings innervating rabbit corneal epithelium. J Neurosci. 1993; 13: 4511–4524. [PubMed]
Tanelian DL Beuerman RW . Responses of rabbit corneal nociceptors to mechanical and thermal stimulation. Exp Neurol. 1984; 84: 165–178. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
MacIver MB Tanelian DL . Free nerve ending terminal morphology is fiber type specific for A delta and C fibers innervating rabbit corneal epithelium. J Neurophysiol. 1993; 69: 1779–1783. [PubMed]
Steen KH Reeh PW Anton F Handwerker HO . Protons selectively induce lasting excitation and sensitization to mechanical stimulation of nociceptors in rat skin, in vitro. J Neurosci. 1992; 12: 86–95. [PubMed]
Porro CA Baraldi P Pagnoni G . Does anticipation of pain affect cortical nociceptive systems? J Neurosci. 2002; 22: 3206–3214. [PubMed]
Koyama T Tanaka YZ Mikami A . Nociceptive neurons in the macaque anterior cingulate activate during anticipation of pain. Neuroreport. 1998; 9: 2663–2667. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Keltner JR Furst A Fan C Redfern R Inglis B Fields HL . Isolating the modulatory effect of expectation on pain transmission: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J Neurosci. 2006; 26: 4437–4443. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Miron D Duncan GH Bushnell MC . Effects of attention on the intensity and unpleasantness of thermal pain. Pain. 1989; 39: 345–352. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Sawamoto N Honda M Okada T . Expectation of pain enhances responses to nonpainful somatosensory stimulation in the anterior cingulate cortex and parietal operculum/posterior insula: an event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J Neurosci. 2000; 20: 7438–7445. [PubMed]
Figure 1.
 
The experimental sequence. Interstimulus intervals were 10 seconds for cooling and mechanical stimulation and 30 seconds for chemical stimulation.
Figure 1.
 
The experimental sequence. Interstimulus intervals were 10 seconds for cooling and mechanical stimulation and 30 seconds for chemical stimulation.
Figure 2.
 
(a) Subthreshold mechanical stimulation with 10-second ISI, Friedman nonparametric ANOVA; P = 0.86. (b) Threshold mechanical stimulation with 10-second ISI; P = 0.15. (c) Suprathreshold mechanical stimulation with 10-second ISI; P = 0.004. (d) Suprathreshold mechanical stimulation with 20-second ISI; P = 0.42.
Figure 2.
 
(a) Subthreshold mechanical stimulation with 10-second ISI, Friedman nonparametric ANOVA; P = 0.86. (b) Threshold mechanical stimulation with 10-second ISI; P = 0.15. (c) Suprathreshold mechanical stimulation with 10-second ISI; P = 0.004. (d) Suprathreshold mechanical stimulation with 20-second ISI; P = 0.42.
Figure 3.
 
(a) Subthreshold cooling stimulation with 10-second ISI, Friedman nonparametric ANOVA; P = 0.88. (b) Threshold cooling stimulation with 10-seconds ISI; P = 0.27. (c) Suprathreshold cooling stimulation with 10-second ISI; P = 0.004.
Figure 3.
 
(a) Subthreshold cooling stimulation with 10-second ISI, Friedman nonparametric ANOVA; P = 0.88. (b) Threshold cooling stimulation with 10-seconds ISI; P = 0.27. (c) Suprathreshold cooling stimulation with 10-second ISI; P = 0.004.
Figure 4.
 
(a) Subthreshold chemical stimulation with 30-second ISI; Friedman nonparametric ANOVA; P = 0.19. (b) Threshold chemical stimulation with 30-second ISI; P = 0.03. (c) Suprathreshold chemical stimulation with 30-second ISI; P = 0.11.
Figure 4.
 
(a) Subthreshold chemical stimulation with 30-second ISI; Friedman nonparametric ANOVA; P = 0.19. (b) Threshold chemical stimulation with 30-second ISI; P = 0.03. (c) Suprathreshold chemical stimulation with 30-second ISI; P = 0.11.
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×