Free
Visual Psychophysics and Physiological Optics  |   September 2013
Capturing Age-Related Changes in Functional Contrast Sensitivity With Decreasing Light Levels in Monocular and Binocular Vision
Author Notes
  • Applied Vision Research Centre, The Henry Wellcome Laboratories for Vision Sciences, School of Health Sciences, City University London, London, United Kingdom 
  • Correspondence: Hanna Gillespie-Gallery, Applied Vision Research Centre, The Henry Wellcome Laboratories for Vision Sciences, School of Health Sciences, City University London, London, EC1V 0HB; [email protected]
Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science September 2013, Vol.54, 6093-6103. doi:https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.13-12119
  • Views
  • PDF
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Hanna Gillespie-Gallery, Evgenia Konstantakopoulou, Jonathan A. Harlow, John L. Barbur; Capturing Age-Related Changes in Functional Contrast Sensitivity With Decreasing Light Levels in Monocular and Binocular Vision. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2013;54(9):6093-6103. https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.13-12119.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract

Purpose.: It is challenging to separate the effects of normal aging of the retina and visual pathways independently from optical factors, decreased retinal illuminance, and early stage disease. This study determined limits to describe the effect of light level on normal, age-related changes in monocular and binocular functional contrast sensitivity.

Methods.: We recruited 95 participants aged 20 to 85 years. Contrast thresholds for correct orientation discrimination of the gap in a Landolt C optotype were measured using a 4-alternative, forced-choice (4AFC) procedure at screen luminances from 34 to 0.12 cd/m2 at the fovea and parafovea (0° and ±4°). Pupil size was measured continuously. The Health of the Retina index (HRindex) was computed to capture the loss of contrast sensitivity with decreasing light level. Participants were excluded if they exhibited performance outside the normal limits of interocular differences or HRindex values, or signs of ocular disease.

Results.: Parafoveal contrast thresholds showed a steeper decline and higher correlation with age at the parafovea than the fovea. Of participants with clinical signs of ocular disease, 83% had HRindex values outside the normal limits. Binocular summation of contrast signals declined with age, independent of interocular differences.

Conclusions.: The HRindex worsens more rapidly with age at the parafovea, consistent with histologic findings of rod loss and its link to age-related degenerative disease of the retina. The HRindex and interocular differences could be used to screen for and separate the earliest stages of subclinical disease from changes caused by normal aging.

Introduction
As light levels decrease, visual performance worsens making everyday tasks difficult, such as reading and face discrimination, 13 especially for older people and those with retinal diseases. 4,5 It has been suggested that more information about the state of the retina is evident from mesopic rather than photopic visual function. 5 Our study assessed monocular contrast sensitivity in a functionally relevant task to quantify normal aging of the foveal and parafoveal retina between photopic and mesopic light levels, as well as binocular summation, while accounting for age-related confounding variables. 
When assessing the difficulties older people face at low light levels due to retinal changes, there are a number of confounding factors to account for. Pupil miosis, 6,7 and increased scattering and absorption of light by the lens 811 reduce the contrast of the image on the retina and retinal illuminance. However, simulating these changes in younger participants does not produce the same reduction in contrast sensitivity, 12,13 suggesting that neural, age-related changes to the retina and visual pathways 1417 result in a reduction in performance on psychophysical measures and reveal retinal disease, 18 such as age related macular degeneration (AMD). 19,20  
Contrast sensitivity declines with age 21,22 and is more sensitive to the effects of normal aging or disease than high contrast visual acuity. 2329 Mesopic contrast sensitivity declines in the 50s, 10 years before photopic contrast sensitivity. 4 This could be attributed to the age-related reduction in the number of rods at the parafovea, 14,15 which is more severe in cases of AMD. 30 In accordance with these histopathologic findings, parafoveal deficits in rod adaptation have been found at the parafovea in age-related maculopathy, 3134 but not normal aging. 35,36  
The investigation of age-related changes in binocular summation of contrast thresholds can provide useful information on the status of visual pathways. Based on signal detection theory, binocular viewing provides two independent estimates of the stimulus, provided the noise in the two eyes is uncorrelated. 37 Therefore, binocular vision should improve detectability by a factor of Display Formula Image not available ⋍ 1.4; however, summation often shows different values, suggesting the involvement of neural summation comprised of mechanisms that respond only to binocular inputs. 38 Binocular summation for contrast can be reduced significantly in older people, and some experience inhibition (performance in binocular viewing, which is worse than monocular viewing). 39,40 The decline in binocular summation with age often has been attributed to large interocular differences in sensitivity or image contrast. 4143 However, this association has not always been found. 40 Binocular summation may decrease with eccentricity or light level, but results in the literature are mixed. 41,4448  
Our study determined whether there is greater parafoveal than foveal loss in visual function with age, by calculating the Health of the Retina index (HRindex) to quantify changes over mesopic and photopic light levels. Additionally, we explored whether interocular differences can account for changes in binocular summation with age. 
Methods
Participants
Participants were recruited by advertising the study within City University London. Tests were approved by the City University Research and Ethics Committee and the study adhered to the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki. Informed consent was obtained for all participants. The participants underwent an ophthalmic assessment, which included measurement of visual acuity, refraction, binocular vision assessment, pupil reactions, slit-lamp assessment of the anterior eye, and indirect ophthalmoscopy of the macula, optic nerve head, and peripheral retina using a 90 diopter (D) lens. 
Contrast Sensitivity Assessment
The contrast vision of each participant was assessed using a “Functional Contrast Sensitivity” (FCS) test. 49 Stimuli were presented on a high resolution NEC Multisync Diamondtron CRT monitor (model FR2141 SB, 19.5 inches; NEC, Tokyo, Japan), using a 30 bit color graphics card (ELSA; Model Gloria, SL, Aachen, Germany) with 1280 × 1024 pixels, at a frame rate of 120 Hz. The monitor was calibrated automatically with an LMT 1009 luminance meter and bespoke software (LUMCAL; City Occupational Ltd., London, UK). 
Participants viewed the display from 2 m. The task was to discriminate the direction of the gap in a Landolt ring optotype, which occurred in one of four diagonal directions. Between presentations, a fixation cross and four oblique guides were displayed to help maintain central fixation and accommodation. The spectral composition of the background had predominantly long-wavelength (LW) and middle-wavelength (MW) content (CIE x = 0.43, y = 0.485) to minimize chromatic aberrations and variation in short wavelength (SW) absorption of light by the macular pigment and the crystalline lens. 50 The stimulus was presented for 80 ms at the specified contrast with 2σ Gaussian-weighted, rising and falling profiles (σ = 53 ms). Stimuli were presented in one of three randomly interleaved locations, at +4°, 0°, or −4° from fixation, along the horizontal meridian. A staircase procedure with 10 reversals was used to vary the Weber Contrast of the stimulus using a two-down, one-up procedure, reducing the chance response probability to 1/16. 51 Interleaved staircases used increments that decreased according to an exponential function. Starting contrast increments were 5%, and ending contrast increments were 1% for the highest light level and 10% and 2%, respectively, for the lowest light level. 
Size scaling of the stimulus was used to account for the reduction in spatial resolution with increasing eccentricity. A gap size was 4 min arc at 0° (diameter 20 min arc) and 6 min arc at ±4° (diameter 30 min arc), corresponding to spatial frequencies of 7.5 and 5 c/deg, important in tasks on visual displays 49 and are affected by aging, whereas lower spatial frequencies are mostly unaffected by aging. 22 The fixed gap size was significantly larger than the acuity limit at high light levels to ensure it would not be below the acuity limit at low light levels, resulting in mid to high spatial frequencies being used to discriminate the location of the gap. 
Participants were tested at background luminances, 34.00, 7.60, 3.20, 1.60, and 0.12 cd/m2. Spectrally calibrated neutral density filters were used for background luminances below 3 cd/m2
Participants viewed the screen binocularly, followed by the right eye alone and then the left eye alone at each light level. 52 The eye not being tested was covered with an opaque, infrared transmitting filter, which allowed the iris illumination needed for pupil measurements. The participants were tested at the brightest screen luminance first, followed by the next, lower screen luminance, meaning that less time was required for adaptation between luminance levels than using a randomized procedure. A minimum of five minutes of adaptation time was provided for the lowest luminance from the second lowest luminance and two minutes for other luminances. 
Estimates of Lens Optical Density
The SW absorption of the crystalline lens was measured with the Macular Assessment Profile (MAP) test, for which a full description has been provided previously. 53 The MAP test estimates lens optical density (OD) for SW light with respect to the mean density of young observers. The test was performed monocularly for each eye at a viewing distance of 0.7 m. The OD was measured to ensure firstly that no participants had higher values than found in previous samples and secondly that there were no significant differences in OD between the two eyes. 
Pupil Measurements
Pupil diameter was measured during the FCS test using the P_SCAN 100 system, 54,55 which uses infrared video imaging techniques with pulsed infrared illumination to measure the center coordinates of the pupil and to compute its size. Pupil measurements were taken monocularly while the participant performed the test and were averaged to produce a mean pupil size for each luminance; separate estimates were made for binocular and monocular viewing. 
Estimating Retinal Illuminance
Retinal Illuminance (E) was measured in trolands (td) as E = L × P, where L is the screen luminance in cd/m2 and P is the pupil area in mm2
Calculating HRIndex for Contrast Sensitivity
The group data provided an average measure of the change in contrast sensitivity threshold with retinal illuminance for five light levels. The HRindex reflects the fractional difference between the area under the participant's threshold curve and the corresponding median curve for the group (see Appendix). For each participant, an HRindex at three retinal locations, one foveal and two parafoveal, was calculated for each eye. For the group data, results at ±4° were combined because the area did not differ between −4° and +4° (t[53] = 1.28, P = 0.21). 
Identifying Participants With Significantly Elevated Contrast Thresholds
Participants with detectable clinical signs of disease were excluded from the calculation of the HRindex. Participants also were excluded if they exhibited differences outside the 95% limits in lens optical density or contrast sensitivity at corresponding loci between their eyes, as early stage retinal diseases tend to affect the eyes asymmetrically and/or start at the parafovea. 30,56 To identify participants with substantial interocular differences in contrast thresholds (IOdifference ) the following parameter was calculated:  where ALE is the area under the curve for one eccentricity for the left eye and ARE is the area under the curve for the corresponding eccentricity in the right eye. If a participant was excluded at one retinal location, all results were excluded.  
Calculating Binocular Summation Ratio (BSR) and Interocular Percentage Increase (IPI)
BSRs were calculated as the ratio of the best eye's contrast threshold to the binocular contrast threshold.    
IPI was calculated to investigate its influence on binocular summation. It was calculated as the absolute difference of the thresholds between the eyes as a ratio of the best eye threshold, where TLE is the average left eye threshold and TRE is the corresponding right eye threshold.    
Statistical Analysis
The JMP statistical software was used to fit the nonlinear function that describes the variation in the participant's threshold with retinal illuminance (SAS Institute, Inc., Cary, NC). MATLAB (The MathsWorks, Inc., Natick, MA) was used to estimate the probability density functions for the measured HRindex values and to compute the 95% limits. For statistical analysis, each participant contributed one data point only for each condition, obtained by averaging results across eyes and eccentricities. 
Results
Identification of Outliers
We recruited 95 participants (age range, 20–85 years), 12 of whom (12.6%) were excluded due to a presence or history of ocular disease or injury (Table 1). The lens optical density values were within the range reported previously. 53 A total of 13 participants had significant interocular differences and were excluded; four participants (4.2%) showed asymmetrical optical density of the crystalline lens and nine (9.5%) participants had differences in the area under the threshold curve outside the 95% limits. 
Table 1
 
Description of Participants Excluded Following the Clinical Exam
Table 1
 
Description of Participants Excluded Following the Clinical Exam
Condition Frequency (%)
Early signs of AMD (drusen, RPE changes) 6 (50%)
Diabetes 2 (17%)
Corneal opacities 1 (8.3%)
Iris trauma 1 (8.3%)
Retinal holes 1 (8.3%)
Retinitis pigmentosa 1 (8.3%)
There were 16 participants (16.8%) excluded because the area under the curve for calculation of the HRindex fell outside the 95% limits at the fovea or parafovea. The HRindex was calculated for each remaining participant separately for each parafoveal and foveal location. Figure 1 shows the age distribution of all participants (n = 54 normals; mean age ± SD, 43.9 ± 14.7 years). The ratio of males to females was 16:11. 
Figure 1
 
Age distribution of included participants and those excluded due to ocular disease or interocular differences outside the 95% limits. Those with either interocular lens or contrast threshold differences outside the normal limit are combined into the “interocular differences” category.
Figure 1
 
Age distribution of included participants and those excluded due to ocular disease or interocular differences outside the 95% limits. Those with either interocular lens or contrast threshold differences outside the normal limit are combined into the “interocular differences” category.
HRindex for Monocular Functional Contrast Thresholds
Figure 2 shows the contrast thresholds as a function of retinal illuminance for foveal and parafoveal targets for the included participants. The asymptote of the foveal data revealed improved contrast thresholds at the fovea at high light levels; parameter p1 suggested that as the light level decreases performance declines more gradually at the parafovea (226.1) than the fovea (241.7). The age dependence of contrast thresholds on adaptation luminance is more apparent at the parafovea (Fig. 2D) than the fovea (Fig. 2A). Figures 2C and 2F show results for excluded participants, who may show high thresholds only at a particular retinal illuminance or retinal location, while other results can be within the normal range. 
Figure 2
 
Contrast thresholds for participants at the range of light levels. For the foveal data, each participant contributes two points for each screen luminance, one from each eye. For the parafoveal data, each participant contributes four points for each luminance level, from ±4° in each eye. (AC) Show foveal data. (A) Contrast thresholds for the five background luminances used. Note that each bar represents one decade, that is, 20 indicates results from ages 20 to 29 years and error bars show one SD. (B) Contrast thresholds for normal participants and curve fitted to the data in the form, T = p1 × ep2logE + p3. (C) data as in (B) with data from participants with ocular disease (red) or outlying interocular differences (green). (DF) Show the corresponding graphs for parafoveal data.
Figure 2
 
Contrast thresholds for participants at the range of light levels. For the foveal data, each participant contributes two points for each screen luminance, one from each eye. For the parafoveal data, each participant contributes four points for each luminance level, from ±4° in each eye. (AC) Show foveal data. (A) Contrast thresholds for the five background luminances used. Note that each bar represents one decade, that is, 20 indicates results from ages 20 to 29 years and error bars show one SD. (B) Contrast thresholds for normal participants and curve fitted to the data in the form, T = p1 × ep2logE + p3. (C) data as in (B) with data from participants with ocular disease (red) or outlying interocular differences (green). (DF) Show the corresponding graphs for parafoveal data.
Figure 3 shows the HRindex as a function of age at the fovea and parafovea (r 2 = 0.19, P < 0.001 and r 2 = 0.34, P < 0.001, respectively), where no differences were found in the variance of older and younger participants at the fovea or parafovea (Levene's statistic = 0.591, P = 0.446 and Levene's statistic = 1.908, P = 0.173, respectively). Although the gradient of decline of the HRindex was steeper at the parafovea, this did not reach significance (repeated measures ANCOVA, F[1,52] = 2.554, P = 0.116). 
Figure 3
 
HRindex as a function of age. (A, C) The HRindex for the fovea and parafoveal, respectively. Dashed lines show the 95% limits. (B, D) The probability density distributions of the errors of values from the regression line. For the foveal data, each participant contributes two points, one from each eye. For the parafoveal data, each participant contributes four points, from ± 4° in each eye.
Figure 3
 
HRindex as a function of age. (A, C) The HRindex for the fovea and parafoveal, respectively. Dashed lines show the 95% limits. (B, D) The probability density distributions of the errors of values from the regression line. For the foveal data, each participant contributes two points, one from each eye. For the parafoveal data, each participant contributes four points, from ± 4° in each eye.
Figure 4 shows the HRindex for participants with ocular disease and interocular differences outside the 95% limits. Ten of 12 of those with ocular disease and 11 of 13 of those with significant interocular differences had HRindex values outside the normal limits for at least one eccentricity. 
Figure 4
 
HRindex for normal participants, those with ocular disease,e and with interocular differences outside the 95% limits. For the foveal data (A), each participant contributes two points, one from each eye. For the parafoveal data (B), each participant contributes four points, from ± 4° in each eye. In total, each participant has six points over the two graphs.
Figure 4
 
HRindex for normal participants, those with ocular disease,e and with interocular differences outside the 95% limits. For the foveal data (A), each participant contributes two points, one from each eye. For the parafoveal data (B), each participant contributes four points, from ± 4° in each eye. In total, each participant has six points over the two graphs.
Normal participants showed a steady increase in contrast thresholds with decreasing retinal illuminance (Fig. 5). A participant with macular drusen exhibited HRindex values outside of the normal limits, with particularly elevated thresholds at low light levels in the parafovea. 
Figure 5
 
Examples of contrast thresholds, and the corresponding HRindex values for normal and excluded participants. (A, B) The contrast thresholds for two normal observers at the fovea and parafovea, respectively, and for a 59-year-old with drusen. The younger participant has a smaller area under the curve than the group data, resulting in a positive HRindex. However, the older participant has a greater area under the curve than the group data, resulting in a negative HRindex, yet still within the 95% limits in Figure 3. The participant with drusen also has a negative HRindex, but is outside the 95% limits at both eccentricities.
Figure 5
 
Examples of contrast thresholds, and the corresponding HRindex values for normal and excluded participants. (A, B) The contrast thresholds for two normal observers at the fovea and parafovea, respectively, and for a 59-year-old with drusen. The younger participant has a smaller area under the curve than the group data, resulting in a positive HRindex. However, the older participant has a greater area under the curve than the group data, resulting in a negative HRindex, yet still within the 95% limits in Figure 3. The participant with drusen also has a negative HRindex, but is outside the 95% limits at both eccentricities.
Figure 6 shows how contrast thresholds change at the fovea and parafovea for three retinal illuminance levels as a function of age. Points were derived from curves fitted to each individual's data. A repeated measures ANCOVA with two factors, eccentricity (fovea and parafovea) and light level (900, 25, 5 td) with age as a covariate was performed. Thresholds were best at the fovea (F[1,53] = 13.570, P < 0.001), at higher light levels (F[1,53] = 1253.731, P < 0.001) and in younger participants (F[1,52] = 36.203, P < 0.001). More interestingly contrast thresholds increased more rapidly with age at the parafovea than the fovea (F[1,52] = 4.718, P < 0.05) and at lower light levels (F[1,52] = 11.250, P < 0.01). Correlations with age were stronger at the parafovea than fovea. 
Figure 6
 
Foveal and parafoveal contrast thresholds at 900, 25, and 5 td. (A) Foveal contrast thresholds (at 5 td y = 0.6495x + 68.906, R 2 = 0.129; at 25 td y = 0.4946x + 21.299, R 2 = 0.2044; at 900 td y = 0.242x + 2.9257, R 2 = 0.2046). (B) Parafoveal contrast thresholds (at 5 td y = 1.1275x + 53.396, R 2 = 0.3422; at 25 td y = 0.6783x + 20.686, R 2 = 0.3275; at 900 td y = 0.3827x + 2.3396, R 2 = 0.3485).
Figure 6
 
Foveal and parafoveal contrast thresholds at 900, 25, and 5 td. (A) Foveal contrast thresholds (at 5 td y = 0.6495x + 68.906, R 2 = 0.129; at 25 td y = 0.4946x + 21.299, R 2 = 0.2044; at 900 td y = 0.242x + 2.9257, R 2 = 0.2046). (B) Parafoveal contrast thresholds (at 5 td y = 1.1275x + 53.396, R 2 = 0.3422; at 25 td y = 0.6783x + 20.686, R 2 = 0.3275; at 900 td y = 0.3827x + 2.3396, R 2 = 0.3485).
Binocular Summation
Of the 54 participants 52 had binocular vision. BSRs were calculated at 1 td increments between 2 and 900 td, and then averaged to produce one BSR value using the curve fitted to each participant's thresholds to account for differing retinal illuminance between participants, and in monocular and binocular conditions. The BSRs for contrast sensitivity are variable (mean 1.52, range 0.75–2.75 57 ) and all but one participant fell within this range. A repeated measures ANCOVA with eccentricity and age revealed that they both had a significant effect on binocular summation (Table 2), suggesting that BSRs are higher at the parafovea (M = 1.43, SD = 0.28) than the fovea (M = 1.33, SD = 0.31, P < 0.05) and that BSRs decreases with age (P < 0.01). The interaction between age and eccentricity was not significant. BSRs were correlated significantly with age at the fovea (r 2 = 0.12, P < 0.05) and the parafovea (r 2 = 0.11, P < 0.05, Fig. 7A). Eight, participants showed binocular inhibition (BSR <1), seven of whom were over the mean age of 43.9. 
Figure 7
 
BSR values <1 (the BSR inhibition threshold) indicate binocular inhibition. (A) Binocular summation ratio for normal, binocular participants (n = 52). Binocular summation was calculated as the best monocular contrast threshold divided by the binocular contrast threshold at retinal illuminances between 900 and 2 td. The solid line shows the linear fit to the foveal data (−0.0077*Age + 1.667, r 2 = 0.12, P < 0.05) and the dashed line the fit to parafoveal data (−0.0063*Age + 1.705, r 2 = 0.11, P < 0.05). (B) A linear fit to foveal and parafoveal points, as the ANCOVA revealed no effects of eccentricity (−0.363*interocular percentage increase + 1.5082, r 2 = 0.10, P < 0.01). (C) The IPI has no relationship with age at the fovea (r 2 = 0.05, P = 0.23) or parafovea (r 2 = 0.004, P = 0.66).
Figure 7
 
BSR values <1 (the BSR inhibition threshold) indicate binocular inhibition. (A) Binocular summation ratio for normal, binocular participants (n = 52). Binocular summation was calculated as the best monocular contrast threshold divided by the binocular contrast threshold at retinal illuminances between 900 and 2 td. The solid line shows the linear fit to the foveal data (−0.0077*Age + 1.667, r 2 = 0.12, P < 0.05) and the dashed line the fit to parafoveal data (−0.0063*Age + 1.705, r 2 = 0.11, P < 0.05). (B) A linear fit to foveal and parafoveal points, as the ANCOVA revealed no effects of eccentricity (−0.363*interocular percentage increase + 1.5082, r 2 = 0.10, P < 0.01). (C) The IPI has no relationship with age at the fovea (r 2 = 0.05, P = 0.23) or parafovea (r 2 = 0.004, P = 0.66).
Table 2
 
Description of ANCOVAs Describing the Effects of Age, Foveal Location, and Normalized Interocular Thresholds Difference on Binocular Summation
Table 2
 
Description of ANCOVAs Describing the Effects of Age, Foveal Location, and Normalized Interocular Thresholds Difference on Binocular Summation
ANCOVA F P Value
Age, IV and eccentricity
 Age, covariate 9.03 P < 0.01
 Eccentricity, fovea, and parafovea 5.79 P < 0.05
 Age × eccentricity 0.25 P = 0.62
IPI and eccentricity
 IPI, covariate 12.31 P < 0.01
 Eccentricity, fovea, and parafovea 0.02 P = 0.89
 IPI × eccentricity 1.88 P = 0.17
Increasing interocular differences can reduce binocular summation and cause inhibition. An independent measures ANCOVA with eccentricity and IPI (Table 2) revealed a main effect of IPI on binocular summation, but no effect of eccentricity or an interaction between these factors. Low values of IPI resulted in high levels of binocular summation and vice versa (r 2 = 0.10, P < 0.01, Fig. 7B). IPI has no relationship with age at the fovea (r 2 = 0.05, P = 0.23) or parafovea (r 2 = 0.004, P = 0.66, Fig. 7C). 
Discussion
The HRindex at the Fovea and Parafovea
The findings in our study showed that contrast vision declines with age, consistent with large population studies of aging and contrast vision. 27,58 The current approach, however, isolates the decline in contrast vision to retinal factors, independently of decreased retinal illuminance and increased optical density of the lens. The HRindex method provides a single number to simply represent contrast performance over a range of light levels. 
A linear decline in contrast sensitivity was found from 20 to 74 years of age (Figs. 3, 6), by measuring an observer's contrast threshold and at their retinal illuminance. Studies that did not account for retinal illuminance suggest that declines in visual performance with age over the lifespan are best fit with bilinear and/or exponential functions, 27,59 or only show a decline in performance from 50 years, 59 highlighting the need to measure retinal illuminance for a wide range of light levels. 
Parafoveal performance showed a significantly steeper decline with age than foveal performance (Fig. 6). Earlier studies have not found greater functional declines at the parafovea compared to the fovea in normal aging, 35,36 but have in early AMD. 3134 The parafovea exhibits a significant loss of rod photoreceptors with healthy aging, particularly in patients diagnosed with AMD. 30 Since older eyes have 13.5% larger rods, resulting in similar rod coverage 14 and increased parafoveal spatial summation, 60,61 age-related functional loss at the macula may manifest as a loss of contrast or other spatial perception rather than absolute sensitivity. No difference in the rate of parafoveal and foveal decline was found using the HRindex, which summarizes performance at photopic and mesopic light levels, suggesting that to quantify the effects of aging research should focus on performance at lower light levels. Therefore, our results suggested that age-related rod loss at the parafovea affects contrast vision in normal aging and not just in macular disease. 
The HRindex limits identified 83% of participants who had signs of ocular disease; Hahn et al. 59 identified only 67% of those with early AMD in a parafoveal letter recognition task. The range of light levels used in our study may have allowed the identification of more participants with ocular disease, although the participants in this study had a range of conditions. A larger sample and longitudinal study, however, would be required to determine reliably the sensitivity and specificity of the HRindex
Interocular differences can have functional consequences, such as increasing the number of driving accidents. 62 In our study 85% of those with large interocular differences also had HRindex values outside the normal limits and interocular differences were larger in those with ocular disease (P < 0.01). Differences in contrast sensitivity between the eyes could be due to differences in optical aberrations, accommodation, and scattered light; however, the use of Landolt ring gap sizes of 4 and 6 arc min, and the restriction of light to MW and LW are likely to minimize these effects. Selective structural changes in the retina or an imbalance in the cortical area dedicated to each eye may contribute more to the measured differences in contrast sensitivity between the eyes, suggesting that any deficits in HRindex might be related to photoreceptor/retinal or higher processing deficits. 
The decline in contrast sensitivity with age shows a greater decrease than previously calculated for color vision 63 ; at the parafovea the gradient is more than double that computed for chromatic HRindex. The assessment of more retinal locations, the extension into the lower mesopic range and the use of interocular differences as an additional filter may have made this assessment more sensitive. 
Binocular Summation of Contrast Signals
BSRs were calculated, for the first time to our knowledge accounting for retinal illuminance difference between participants and monocular and binocular conditions. 64 BSR decreased with age in accordance with previous finings. 39,40 In addition, eight of 52 participants showed binocular inhibition, a greater proportion than previously reported, 39,65 despite the fact that our methods maximized BSR, which is highest for stimuli at threshold. 43,46 These findings could be because BSR was averaged from 900 to 2 td, whereas previous studies are conducted under photopic conditions and BSR is reduced at lower retinal illuminances (paired t-test, t[51] = 2.509, P < 0.05; at 900 td, M = 1.34, SD = 0.37; at 2 td, M = 1.15, SD = 0.37). These results suggested that when measuring visual function over a large range of light levels, a greater proportion of people may experience difficulties in binocular vision than reported previously. 
The decrease in BSR in normal aging often has been attributed to increases in interocular differences with age. 27,41,43 However, as the thresholds of the eyes increase, the interocular difference also should increase proportionately in accordance with Weber's Law. If one defines the interocular difference as the IPI in contrast thresholds, as described above, IPI has no relationship with age at the fovea (r 2 = 0.05, P = 0.23) or parafovea (r 2 = 0.004, P = 0.66, Fig. 7C). Therefore, any decrease in BSR with age must be explained by neural factors either at the retina or cortex. In support of a central, neural etiology, BSR declines at the same rate with age for foveal and parafoveal locations. Possible explanations included poorer photoreceptor activity, increases in cortical noise, or delayed signal timing with age. 16,66,67  
BSR was higher at the parafovea compared to the fovea, contrary to previous findings. 41,44 In our study a slightly larger target size was used at the parafovea compared to the fovea, which improves summation. 44 Most studies of binocular summation use the same target size across the visual field; this results in a corresponding reduction in sensitivity as the receptive fields of retinal ganglion cells increase, acting as an additional extraneous factor. The stimuli in our study were size scaled to control for differences in sensitivity, possibly revealing a real increase in binocular summation when sensitivity changes are corrected for. The size scaling was appropriate, as the area under the curves for foveal and parafoveal data were similar. 
Conclusions
Independently of retinal illuminance, older people have difficulty with contrast vision due to neural changes in the retina and reduced binocular summation. The parafovea is more susceptible to aging than the fovea and advanced testing of its function may prove useful in detecting retinal disease. Methods used in our study have identified individuals with losses of spatial vision despite minimizing the effects of pupil miosis, light scatter, and the use of MW and LW light. The contrast-based HRindex confirms previous findings on chromatic sensitivity and extends its applicability. BSR revealed a number of older individuals showing binocular inhibition, raising questions about the quality of binocular vision in older people in the absence of clinically recognizable deficits or disease. 
Acknowledgments
Supported by EPSRC Grant Number EP/I003940/1. 
Disclosure: H. Gillespie-Gallery, None; E. Konstantakopoulou, None; J.A. Harlow, None; J.L. Barbur, None 
References
Legge GE Rubin GS Luebker A. Psychophysics of reading—V. The role of contrast in normal vision. Vision Res . 1987; 27: 1165–1177. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Owsley C Sekuler R Boldt C. Aging and low-contrast vision: face perception. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci . 1981; 21: 362–365. [PubMed]
Owsley C McGwin G Scilley K Kallies K. Development of a questionnaire to assess vision problems under low luminance in age-related maculopathy. Ophthalmol Vis Sci . 2006; 47: 528–535. [CrossRef]
Puell MC Palomo C Sanchez-Ramos C Villena C. Normal values for photopic and mesopic letter contrast sensitivity. J Refract Surg . 2004; 20: 484–488. [PubMed]
Petzold A Plant GT. Clinical disorders affecting mesopic vision. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt . 2006; 26: 326–341. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Loewenfeld IE. Pupillary changes related to age. In: Thompson HS Daroff R Frisén L Glaser JS Sanders MD eds. Topics in Neuro-Ophthalmology . Philadelphia, PA: Williams and Wilkins; 1972: 124–150.
Loewenfeld IE. The Pupil: Anatomy, Physiology, and Clinical Applications . Oxford, UK: Butterworth and Heinemann; 1999.
Pokorny J Smith VC Lutze M. Aging of the human lens. Appl Opt . 1987; 26: 1437–1440. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Sample P Esterson F Weinreb R Boynton R. The aging lens: in vivo assessment of light absorption in 84 human eyes. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci . 1988; 29: 1306–1311. [PubMed]
Artal P Guirao A Berrio E Piers P Norrby S. Optical aberrations and the aging eye. Int Ophthalmol Clin . 2003; 43: 63–77. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Hennelly M Barbur J Edgar D Woodward E. The effect of age on the light scattering characteristics of the eye. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt . 1998; 18: 197–203. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Elliott SL Choi SS Doble N Hardy JL Evans JW Werner JS. Role of high-order aberrations in senescent changes in spatial vision. J Vis . 2009; 9: 1–16. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Whitaker D Elliott DB. Simulating age-related optical changes in the human eye. Doc Ophthalmol . 1992; 82: 307–316. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Curcio CA Millican CL Allen KA Kalina RE. Aging of the human photoreceptor mosaic: evidence for selective vulnerability of rods in central retina. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci . 1993; 34: 3278–3296. [PubMed]
Gao H Hollyfield JG. Aging of the human retina. differential loss of neurons and retinal pigment epithelial cells. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci . 1992; 33: 1–17. [PubMed]
Yang Y Liang Z Li G Wang Y Zhou Y Leventhal AG. Aging affects contrast response functions and adaptation of middle temporal visual area neurons in rhesus monkeys. J Neurosci . 2008; 156: 748–757. [CrossRef]
Devaney KO Johnson HA. Neuron loss in the aging visual cortex of man. J Gerontol . 1980; 35: 836–841. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Curcio CA Owsley C Jackson GR. Spare the rods, save the cones in aging and age-related maculopathy. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci . 2000; 41: 2015–2018. [PubMed]
Midena E Degli Angeli C, Blarzino MC, Valenti M, Segato T. Macular function impairment in eyes with early age-related macular degeneration. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci . 1997; 38: 469–477. [PubMed]
Feigl B Brown B Lovie-Kitchin J Swann P. Monitoring retinal function in early age-related maculopathy: visual performance after 1 year. Eye . 2005; 19: 1169. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Derefeldt G Lennerstrand G Lundh B. Age variations in normal human contrast sensitivity. Acta Ophthalmol . 1979; 57: 679–690. [CrossRef]
Ross JE Clarke DD Bron AJ. Effect of age on contrast sensitivity function: uniocular and binocular findings. Br J Ophthalmol . 1985; 69: 51–56. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Eisner A Fleming SA Klein ML Mauldin WM. Sensitivities in older eyes with good acuity: cross-sectional norms. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci . 1987; 28: 1824–1831. [PubMed]
Kleiner RC Enger C Alexander MF Fine SL. Contrast sensitivity in age-related macular degeneration. Arch Ophthalmol . 1988; 106: 55–57. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Sunness JS Rubin GS Applegate CA Visual function abnormalities and prognosis in eyes with age-related geographic atrophy of the macula and good visual acuity. Ophthalmology . 1997; 104: 1677–1691. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Dimitrov PN Robman LD Varsamidis M Visual function tests as potential biomarkers in age-related macular degeneration. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci . 2011; 52: 9457–9469. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Haegerstrom-Portnoy G Schneck ME Brabyn JA. Seeing into old age: vision function beyond acuity. Optom Vis Sci . 1999; 76: 141–158. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Brabyn J Schneck M Haegerstrom-Portnoy G Lott L. The Smith-Kettlewell Institute (SKI) longitudinal study of vision function and its impact among the elderly: an overview. Optom Vis Sci . 2001; 78: 264–269. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Schneck ME Haegerstrom-Portnoy G Lott LA Brabyn JA Gildengorin G. Low contrast vision function predicts subsequent acuity loss in an aged population: the SKI study. Vision Res . 2004; 44: 2317–2325. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Curcio CA Medeiros NE Millican CL. Photoreceptor loss in age-related macular degeneration. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci . 1996; 37; 1236–1249. [PubMed]
Owsley C McGwin G Jr Jackson GR Kallies K Cone- Clark M. and rod-mediated dark adaptation impairment in age-related maculopathy. Ophthalmology . 2007; 114: 1728–1735. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Steinmetz RL Haimovici R Jubb C Fitzke FW Bird AC. Symptomatic abnormalities of dark adaptation in patients with age-related Bruch's membrane change. Br J Ophthalmol . 1993; 77: 549–554. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Brown B Adams AJ Coletta NJ Haegerstrom-Portnoy G. Dark adaptation in age-related maculopathy. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt . 1986; 6: 81–84. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Gaffney AJ Binns AM Margrain TH. Topography of cone dark adaptation deficits in age-related maculopathy. Optom Vis Sci . 2011; 88: 1080–1087. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Jackson GR Owsley C Price Cordle E Finley CD. Aging and scotopic sensitivity. Vision Res . 1998; 38: 3655–3662. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Jackson GR Owsley C. Scotopic sensitivity during adulthood. Vision Res . 2000; 40: 2467–2473. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Bárány E. A theory of binocular visual acuity and an analysis of the variability of visual acuity. Acta Ophthalmol . 1946; 24: 63–92.
Campbell FW Green DG. Monocular versus binocular visual acuity. Nature . 1965; 208: 191–192. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Pardhan S. A comparison of binocular summation in young and older patients. Curr Eye Res . 1996; 15: 315–319. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Gagnon RWC Kline DW. Senescent effects on binocular summation for contrast sensitivity and spatial interval acuity. Curr Eye Res . 2003; 27: 315–321. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Pardhan S. A comparison of binocular summation in the peripheral visual field in young and older patients. Curr Eye Res . 1997; 16: 252–255. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Gilchrist J Pardhan S. Binocular contrast detection with unequal monocular illuminance. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt . 1987; 7: 373–377. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Cagenello R Arditi A Halpern DL. Binocular enhancement of visual acuity. JOSA A . 1993; 10: 1841–1848. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Wood JM Collins MJ Carkeet A. Regional variations in binocular summation across the visual field. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt . 1992; 12: 46–51. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Whitaker A Pardhan S. Binocular summation in the peripheral visual field. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt . 1997; 17: 173–173. [CrossRef]
Home R. Binocular summation: a study of contrast sensitivity, visual acuity and recognition. Vision Res . 1978; 18: 579–585. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Legge GE. Binocular contrast summation—II. Quadratic summation. Vision Res . 1984; 24: 385–394. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Connolly DM. Spatial contrast sensitivity at twilight: luminance, monocularity, and oxygenation. Aviat Space Environ Med . 2010; 81: 475–483. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Chisholm CM Evans AD Harlow JA Barbur JL. New test to assess pilot's vision following refractive surgery. Aviat Space Environ Med . 2003; 74: 551–559. [PubMed]
van de Kraats J van Norren D. Optical density of the aging human ocular media in the visible and the UV. J Opt Soc Am . 2007; 24: 1842–1857. [CrossRef]
Levine MW Shefner JM. Fundamentals of Sensation and Perception . Belmont, CA: Brooks/Cole; 1991.
Grimson JM Schallhorn SC Kaupp SE. Contrast sensitivity: establishing normative data for use in screening prospective naval pilots. Aviat Space Environ Med . 2002; 73: 28–35. [PubMed]
Barbur JL Konstantakopoulou E Rodriguez-Carmona M Harlow JA Robson AG Moreland JD. The macular assessment profile test—a new VDU-based technique for measuring the spatial distribution of the macular pigment, lens density and rapid flicker sensitivity. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt . 2010; 30: 470–483. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Barbur JL Thomson WD. Pupil response as an objective measure of visual acuity. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt . 1987; 7: 425–429. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Alexandridis E Leendertz J Barbur J. Methods for studying the behaviour of the pupil. Int J Psychophysiol . 1992; 5: 223–239.
Medeiros NE Curcio CA. Preservation of ganglion cell layer neurons in age-related macular degeneration. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci . 2001; 42: 795–803. [PubMed]
Baker DH. Interocular Suppression and Contrast Gain Control in Human Vision [thesis]. Birmingham, UK: Aston University; 2008.
Rubin GS West SK Munoz B A comprehensive assessment of visual impairment in a population of older Americans. The SEE Study. Salisbury Eye Evaluation Project. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci . 1997; 38: 557–568. [PubMed]
Hahn GA Messias A MacKeben M Parafoveal letter recognition at reduced contrast in normal aging and in patients with risk factors for AMD. Graefes Arch Clin Exp Ophthalmol . 2009; 247: 43–51. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Schefrin BE Bieber ML McLean R Werner JS. The area of complete scotopic spatial summation enlarges with age. J Opt Soc Am . 1998; 15: 340–348. [CrossRef]
Malania M Devinck F Hardy JL Delahunt PB Knoblauch K Werner JS. Test of senescent change in photopic spatial summation. J Vis . 2009; 9: 1074–1074. [CrossRef]
Ivers RQ Mitchell P Cumming RG. Sensory impairment and driving: the Blue Mountains eye study. Am J Public Health . 1999; 89: 85–87. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Barbur JL Konstantakopoulou E. Changes in color vision with decreasing light level: separating the effects of normal aging from disease. J Opt Soc Am . 2012; 29: A27–A35. [CrossRef]
Boxer Wachler BS. Effect of pupil size on visual function under monocular and binocular conditions in LASIK and non-LASIK patients. J Cataract Refract Surg . 2003; 29: 275–278. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Azen SP Varma R Preston-Martin S Ying-Lai M Globe D Hahn S. Binocular visual acuity summation and inhibition in an ocular epidemiological study: the Los Angeles Latino Eye Study. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci . 2002; 43: 1742–1748. [PubMed]
Zhang J Wang X Wang Y Spatial and temporal sensitivity degradation of primary visual cortical cells in senescent rhesus monkeys. Eur J Neurosci . 2008; 28: 201–207. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Wang Y Zhou Y Ma Y Leventhal AG. Degradation of signal timing in cortical areas V1 and V2 of senescent monkeys. Cereb Cortex . 2005; 15: 403–408. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Appendix
Change in the contrast discrimination thresholds as a function of retinal illuminance were fitted with the equation  where T is the measure of contrast threshold, E is the retinal illuminance, p3 is the asymptotic threshold, and p1 and p2 are constants. The best-fit parameters p1, p2, and p3 and were computed for the group of participants; the fitted curves are shown in Figures 2B and 2E.  
The equation then was integrated to compute the area under the curve for thresholds at each of the three retinal locations in each eye producing six values for each participant.    
The fitted curve for the group was used as a reference against which every participant was compared at each retinal location. Then, the equations above were used separately to compute participant-specific dependence on retinal illuminance and the corresponding HRindex. To improve stability of the nonlinear fitting algorithm, a sixth point was added to the dataset to correspond to 80% of the best threshold (predicted, best threshold at high retinal illuminance at 3000 td, corresponding to approximately 150 cd/m2). 
The HRindex was defined as the difference between the area under the participant's threshold curve (Ap ) and the corresponding area computed for the normal group (Agroup ):    
A positive HRindex indicates performance better than the average normal participant. Correspondingly, a negative value indicates contrast discrimination that falls below that expected for the average normal participant. 
Figure 1
 
Age distribution of included participants and those excluded due to ocular disease or interocular differences outside the 95% limits. Those with either interocular lens or contrast threshold differences outside the normal limit are combined into the “interocular differences” category.
Figure 1
 
Age distribution of included participants and those excluded due to ocular disease or interocular differences outside the 95% limits. Those with either interocular lens or contrast threshold differences outside the normal limit are combined into the “interocular differences” category.
Figure 2
 
Contrast thresholds for participants at the range of light levels. For the foveal data, each participant contributes two points for each screen luminance, one from each eye. For the parafoveal data, each participant contributes four points for each luminance level, from ±4° in each eye. (AC) Show foveal data. (A) Contrast thresholds for the five background luminances used. Note that each bar represents one decade, that is, 20 indicates results from ages 20 to 29 years and error bars show one SD. (B) Contrast thresholds for normal participants and curve fitted to the data in the form, T = p1 × ep2logE + p3. (C) data as in (B) with data from participants with ocular disease (red) or outlying interocular differences (green). (DF) Show the corresponding graphs for parafoveal data.
Figure 2
 
Contrast thresholds for participants at the range of light levels. For the foveal data, each participant contributes two points for each screen luminance, one from each eye. For the parafoveal data, each participant contributes four points for each luminance level, from ±4° in each eye. (AC) Show foveal data. (A) Contrast thresholds for the five background luminances used. Note that each bar represents one decade, that is, 20 indicates results from ages 20 to 29 years and error bars show one SD. (B) Contrast thresholds for normal participants and curve fitted to the data in the form, T = p1 × ep2logE + p3. (C) data as in (B) with data from participants with ocular disease (red) or outlying interocular differences (green). (DF) Show the corresponding graphs for parafoveal data.
Figure 3
 
HRindex as a function of age. (A, C) The HRindex for the fovea and parafoveal, respectively. Dashed lines show the 95% limits. (B, D) The probability density distributions of the errors of values from the regression line. For the foveal data, each participant contributes two points, one from each eye. For the parafoveal data, each participant contributes four points, from ± 4° in each eye.
Figure 3
 
HRindex as a function of age. (A, C) The HRindex for the fovea and parafoveal, respectively. Dashed lines show the 95% limits. (B, D) The probability density distributions of the errors of values from the regression line. For the foveal data, each participant contributes two points, one from each eye. For the parafoveal data, each participant contributes four points, from ± 4° in each eye.
Figure 4
 
HRindex for normal participants, those with ocular disease,e and with interocular differences outside the 95% limits. For the foveal data (A), each participant contributes two points, one from each eye. For the parafoveal data (B), each participant contributes four points, from ± 4° in each eye. In total, each participant has six points over the two graphs.
Figure 4
 
HRindex for normal participants, those with ocular disease,e and with interocular differences outside the 95% limits. For the foveal data (A), each participant contributes two points, one from each eye. For the parafoveal data (B), each participant contributes four points, from ± 4° in each eye. In total, each participant has six points over the two graphs.
Figure 5
 
Examples of contrast thresholds, and the corresponding HRindex values for normal and excluded participants. (A, B) The contrast thresholds for two normal observers at the fovea and parafovea, respectively, and for a 59-year-old with drusen. The younger participant has a smaller area under the curve than the group data, resulting in a positive HRindex. However, the older participant has a greater area under the curve than the group data, resulting in a negative HRindex, yet still within the 95% limits in Figure 3. The participant with drusen also has a negative HRindex, but is outside the 95% limits at both eccentricities.
Figure 5
 
Examples of contrast thresholds, and the corresponding HRindex values for normal and excluded participants. (A, B) The contrast thresholds for two normal observers at the fovea and parafovea, respectively, and for a 59-year-old with drusen. The younger participant has a smaller area under the curve than the group data, resulting in a positive HRindex. However, the older participant has a greater area under the curve than the group data, resulting in a negative HRindex, yet still within the 95% limits in Figure 3. The participant with drusen also has a negative HRindex, but is outside the 95% limits at both eccentricities.
Figure 6
 
Foveal and parafoveal contrast thresholds at 900, 25, and 5 td. (A) Foveal contrast thresholds (at 5 td y = 0.6495x + 68.906, R 2 = 0.129; at 25 td y = 0.4946x + 21.299, R 2 = 0.2044; at 900 td y = 0.242x + 2.9257, R 2 = 0.2046). (B) Parafoveal contrast thresholds (at 5 td y = 1.1275x + 53.396, R 2 = 0.3422; at 25 td y = 0.6783x + 20.686, R 2 = 0.3275; at 900 td y = 0.3827x + 2.3396, R 2 = 0.3485).
Figure 6
 
Foveal and parafoveal contrast thresholds at 900, 25, and 5 td. (A) Foveal contrast thresholds (at 5 td y = 0.6495x + 68.906, R 2 = 0.129; at 25 td y = 0.4946x + 21.299, R 2 = 0.2044; at 900 td y = 0.242x + 2.9257, R 2 = 0.2046). (B) Parafoveal contrast thresholds (at 5 td y = 1.1275x + 53.396, R 2 = 0.3422; at 25 td y = 0.6783x + 20.686, R 2 = 0.3275; at 900 td y = 0.3827x + 2.3396, R 2 = 0.3485).
Figure 7
 
BSR values <1 (the BSR inhibition threshold) indicate binocular inhibition. (A) Binocular summation ratio for normal, binocular participants (n = 52). Binocular summation was calculated as the best monocular contrast threshold divided by the binocular contrast threshold at retinal illuminances between 900 and 2 td. The solid line shows the linear fit to the foveal data (−0.0077*Age + 1.667, r 2 = 0.12, P < 0.05) and the dashed line the fit to parafoveal data (−0.0063*Age + 1.705, r 2 = 0.11, P < 0.05). (B) A linear fit to foveal and parafoveal points, as the ANCOVA revealed no effects of eccentricity (−0.363*interocular percentage increase + 1.5082, r 2 = 0.10, P < 0.01). (C) The IPI has no relationship with age at the fovea (r 2 = 0.05, P = 0.23) or parafovea (r 2 = 0.004, P = 0.66).
Figure 7
 
BSR values <1 (the BSR inhibition threshold) indicate binocular inhibition. (A) Binocular summation ratio for normal, binocular participants (n = 52). Binocular summation was calculated as the best monocular contrast threshold divided by the binocular contrast threshold at retinal illuminances between 900 and 2 td. The solid line shows the linear fit to the foveal data (−0.0077*Age + 1.667, r 2 = 0.12, P < 0.05) and the dashed line the fit to parafoveal data (−0.0063*Age + 1.705, r 2 = 0.11, P < 0.05). (B) A linear fit to foveal and parafoveal points, as the ANCOVA revealed no effects of eccentricity (−0.363*interocular percentage increase + 1.5082, r 2 = 0.10, P < 0.01). (C) The IPI has no relationship with age at the fovea (r 2 = 0.05, P = 0.23) or parafovea (r 2 = 0.004, P = 0.66).
Table 1
 
Description of Participants Excluded Following the Clinical Exam
Table 1
 
Description of Participants Excluded Following the Clinical Exam
Condition Frequency (%)
Early signs of AMD (drusen, RPE changes) 6 (50%)
Diabetes 2 (17%)
Corneal opacities 1 (8.3%)
Iris trauma 1 (8.3%)
Retinal holes 1 (8.3%)
Retinitis pigmentosa 1 (8.3%)
Table 2
 
Description of ANCOVAs Describing the Effects of Age, Foveal Location, and Normalized Interocular Thresholds Difference on Binocular Summation
Table 2
 
Description of ANCOVAs Describing the Effects of Age, Foveal Location, and Normalized Interocular Thresholds Difference on Binocular Summation
ANCOVA F P Value
Age, IV and eccentricity
 Age, covariate 9.03 P < 0.01
 Eccentricity, fovea, and parafovea 5.79 P < 0.05
 Age × eccentricity 0.25 P = 0.62
IPI and eccentricity
 IPI, covariate 12.31 P < 0.01
 Eccentricity, fovea, and parafovea 0.02 P = 0.89
 IPI × eccentricity 1.88 P = 0.17
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×