In addition to the influences bestowed by foveal histology, central nervous system processing of foveal input itself also may explain why subfoveal placement of a visual implant is favorable. In the fovea, there is a high density of ganglion cells, and the number of optic nerve fibers in this area is almost equal to the number of cones.
22 In addition, the central visual pathway is devoted primarily to the central visual area. Approximately 50% of the cortical visual area is occupied by the central 5° of the visual field, with the greater part belonging to the 2.5° area of central vision.
24
Animal experiments have shown that the size of the retinal receptive fields in the cortex increases with eccentricity, the so-called “cortical magnification factor.”
42,43 In the primate visual cortex, approximately 1° of foveal vision is represented by approximately 20 mm of cortex. In contrast, at 10° peripheral vision, 1° is represented by only approximately 1.5 mm.
43
Even more specialized tasks are processed by the primary visual cortex, thanks to the enlarged area that is devoted to the central retinal field. For example, for every receptive field, there exists ocular dominance and orientation preference columns
44 that already are decoding for direction of motion, spatial frequency, and orientation input from the retinal field. The primary visual cortex also contributes to the processing of size, binocular vision, and depth perception.
23,45,46
In human vision, poor resolution, poor recognition, and poor interpretation of noncentral vision are phenomena that are demonstrated easily by the phenomenon of “crowding” in peripheral vision.
47,48 Crowding appears to be independent of peripheral stimulus size, and depends only on eccentricity. Reading experiments have shown that crowding may start at 5° visual periphery.
49 Thus, the explanation cannot lie solely in the increased cortical magnification factor; there must be an additional central nervous difference in quality of visual perception between central and peripheral vision.
47 It may be noted that the 5° visual angle corresponds to a retinal distance of approximately 1200 μm, a distance that is nearly one-third of the rim-size of our implant. This suggests that a slightly paracentral location may exert a great effect on failure of object recognition via the chip.
Taken together, processing from foveal input is highly specialized and detailed, thus, affording optimum performance of a number of tasks that require combining information across different scales.
Alternative explanations as to why the central retina is able to mediate visual functions better via a subretinally placed implant versus a parafoveally placed implant also must be addressed. Significant among these may be the amount of time that has elapsed since final photoreceptor degeneration in a particular retinal location. Typically, in RP, degeneration of the photoreceptors begins in the periphery and proceeds (over years) into the central area. In end-stage disease, however, although the foveal photoreceptors also are degenerated, the time elapsed after the last photoreceptor has degenerated is much shorter, the shortest of the entire retina, in fact. Also, in RP, it is known that remodeling of the inner retinal layers occurs after photoreceptor degeneration.
23,50 What happens to the inner retina following photoreceptor degeneration has not been clarified completely to our knowledge. It has been reported that bipolar cells retract their dendrites or create new axon-like structures, glutamate receptors of the ON bipolar cells change to the receptors of the OFF bipolar cells, and many neurons of the inner layer die along with the degenerating photoreceptors.
23,50,51 There also is a marked loss of ganglion, but not bipolar, cells in end-stage RP.
36,52
If the missing input to the inner retina leads to neuronal disorganization, the amount of time elapsed after photoreceptor degeneration may have an impact on the best possible functioning of the inner retinal layer.
In patients (P12, C5, C8) who experienced very good functional results, 15 to 25 years have passed since they last were able to read. Furthermore, patient C12, who suffered from cone-rod dystrophy, first received the implant in a paracentral location; in a second surgery, the implant was repositioned to a subfoveal location. If the time elapsed from local photoreceptor degeneration had had a role, this patient's functional results would have worsened, since, in cone-rod degeneration, the central cones are the first to degenerate. However, after the repositioning, the patient reported subjective improvement, which was verified by his improved results in standardized tests. While this represents only one case, and a training-effect over time might have had a role as well, it also might indicate the retinal and central advantages of central location placement for subretinal visual implants.