Using any of the above changes to the stimulus parameters to “tune” the operating range, as shown for stimulus size and stimulus emission spectrum (first and second approaches, respectively), the physiologic phenomenon of the hill of vision becomes apparent, that is, LIS is highest in the center of the retina. The hill of vision is a basic psychophysical feature of the light-adapted retina. It is well known for Goldmann kinetic perimetry
28,29 in which smaller or dimmer targets yield smaller isopters. Furthermore, the phenomenon has long been shown to be present in static perimetry normative data obtained with conventional apparatus,
30–32 as well as with other scanning-laser ophthalmoscopes.
3,5 Others have compared the relative measurements obtained with the MP1, for example with the Octopus 101,
19,33 the HFA,
9,11,34 or the Opko OCT/SLO.
11 However, in all of these studies except that of Seiple et al.,
11 only a Goldmann III was used in the MP1, which according to the data presented here for the central retina is very problematic. Using 2-color–threshold perimetry,
35,36 it is possible to demonstrate with the MP1 that thresholds in the normal retina are determined by cones (Friedburg C, et al.
IOVS 2012;53:ARVO E-Abstract 4843). Their density
13 and those of the connecting cone bipolar and ganglion cells
14 are highest in the fovea and quickly decrease toward the periphery. The same stimulus placed in the fovea will thus illuminate and activate more cones and ganglion cells than a peripheral stimulus. Thus, a plateau of sensitivity in the center measured with Goldmann I, as depicted in
Figure 4C for case 3 or described by Midena et al.
6 for Goldmann III, indicates macular dysfunction.