We thank Kotliar and Lanzl for their interesting thoughts, which we believe contribute to the development of the best scientific analysis of ocular blood vessels in response to disease. This topic is rapidly developing and has clear indications for other diseases, such as diabetes. Nevertheless, we would like to take the opportunity to respond to the points raised by our colleagues.
In their opening paragraph, they suggest that our characterization of vessel elasticity is flawed by the nature of the assessment. They contend (1) that vessel elasticity cannot be characterized by a single assessment of reactive vessel dilation in response to flicker light and a determination of the magnitude of spontaneous vascular vasomotions. We are puzzled by this point, as a major premise of our new method is that a single assessment alone is insufficient because of the baseline diameter fluctuation (BDF) of the vessel. We analyzed flicker response three times to ensure a greater confidence in our data. In the final paragraph of the Results section, we present data that show no difference (
P = 0.343) in APR taken at three time points.
1 In addition, (2) they suggest that we do not explain the reason for calling APR a measure of elasticity and that the results of the APR are not disclosed anywhere in the paper. We call APR a measure of elasticity because it accounts for the increase in vessel diameter, which then returns to baseline. This we showed with the relationship between maximum vasomotion under resting conditions and under stimulatory (e.g., flicker) conditions. In their final sentence, they say that the results of the APR are not disclosed in our paper. The data are indeed presented at end of the Results section.
In their second paragraph, our colleagues question the index that we have named ΔD, which represents the degree of vessel dilation that occurs between the time point of flicker initiation and maximum vascular dilation. They suggest that the problem inherent in this new parameter is its arbitrary limitation to a 1-second diameter assessment before flicker initiation as its main reference point. First, we would like to further explain why we believe a 1-second mean diameter is a suitable measure. The human heart beats at intervals of roughly one second, which is also visible in most RVA recordings (
Fig. 1), and therefore an average of the 25 diameter recordings before flicker initiation, which are sampled in 1 second, appears to be a suitable means of reflecting the vessel size directly before the stimulation cycle. We explained that there is a limit to how much any vessel can dilate (in the Methods section on SDRA and in the Results section, paragraph five
1 ). In addition, our colleagues state that reduced vessel dilation during the second and third flicker episodes in our healthy patients differed from those in the smoking sample for ΔD. We want to emphasize that we showed correlations only between the 1-second mean baseline diameter and the ΔD in healthy subjects and did not compare or show the correlations for the smoking sample. Our comparison between groups is solely for the parameters shown in Tables 4, 5, and 6 of our paper. In the figure showing two individuals assessed by Kotliar and Lanzl, they present two arterial plots to justify the view that individuals differ significantly in flicker pattern during each cycle. While these data are interesting, we do not share their view, because the data for only 21 nonsmokers in our Table 4 in the original paper show that the individual parameters (BDF, DA [diameter amplitude], and BFG) for each of three flicker cycles are no different in arteries and veins. To further underline this, we present, with considerably greater power, new supporting data from 78 healthy individuals (
Table 1), unequivocally demonstrating that there is no change in response to three cycles of flicker.
In paragraph three, Kotliar and Lanzl suggest a possible source of error in our calculations. First, there seems to be a basic misunderstanding: All parameters (BDF, MD [maximum dilation], MC [maximum constriction], DA, and time responses) were not set in any relation or based on or calculated to ΔD, contrary to their statement. BDF is the relative baseline diameter fluctuation, MD is the relative maximum dilation due to flicker, and so on and has been calculated from the raw data individually for each flicker episode.
Paragraph four has several issues. Kotliar and Lanzl further question the relevance of using three stimulation cycles, highlighting that, in their measurements, healthy individuals respond differently in each cycle and also that sometimes not all cycles are recorded, and so the average response should be taken. Correctly, this tripling of the number of analyzed parameters can reduce the level of statistical significance. This may be correct, but our analysis of the data that we presented in Table 4 (and elsewhere) is according to repeated-measures analysis of variance (Friedman's two-way method), which adjusts for individual variances and so is a particularly powerful method.
2 Second, our colleagues cite the paper by Kvernmo et al.
3 to explain their different hypothesis on retinal vasomotion; however, although informative in a general setting, we cannot see the relevance of this paper to our study, since Kvernmo et al. used a laser Doppler technique to examine the cutaneous blood flow of capillaries, arterioles, venules, and dermal vascular plexi, rather than single vessel motion. Third, Kotliar and Lanzl state that, in their example, the two individuals would show significant dilatory results if our analysis paradigm were used and raise the question of which flicker should be used for analysis. In addition, they question whether the difference detected between smokers and nonsmokers is unique and manifests in a similar way in other diseases, and they mention that healthy volunteers may demonstrate differing results despite their similar pathophysiologic status. In regard to the question of which flicker should be used, we stress that, if a repeated-measures protocol is used, all measures should be taken into account and that, if a data recording is incomplete, it should be excluded from further analysis. The advantage of a repeated procedure lies not only in increasing confidence but also in enabling a reaction pattern and examining any “exhaustive” effect. Any measurement taken from two individuals will have differences; these do not generally relate to disease but are part of a “normal range” within a chosen sample. For this reason, we measured a large number of people, to examine the range and dependencies of our proposed marker.
In their fifth paragraph, Kotliar and Lanzl state that a qualitative assessment of each single flicker episode is not always possible in all subjects (elderly population, subjects with bad fixation, and those with excessive blinking during the assessment), which represents a serious limitation in our novel method. We are aware that it can be difficult, in some patients, to obtain all three cycles; however, in our study we analyzed only the data sets in which all three cycles were completely recorded. Assessments of individuals with poor fixation or excessive blinking should be removed if all cycles were not recorded (especially when evaluating normative parameters, good fixation is essential), since eye movements can introduce a significant error especially in regard to diameter fluctuations attributable to vasomotion. The conclusion that fixation, eye movements, and incomplete recordings can influence the result is therefore correct but does not apply to our data set, since we included only subjects with good fixation and in whom all three cycles were completely recorded.
A protocol with repeated stimulation cycles should be analyzed stepwise, in that each stimulation is evaluated, and a reaction pattern is identified. No two subjects will react identically, in that dilation and recovery times can vary; however, this is not reason enough to abandon a multiple stimulation cycle with fixed recovery and stimulation times. Furthermore, it enables us to examine and determine the range in which a normal subject will be placed and can then be compared to different pathophysiological states (i.e., smokers and different diseases). The purpose of our paper was to highlight the importance of evaluating these different cycles and the danger of false interpretation when averaging results. There are many different ways of evaluating dilatory responses and elasticity, but each of them must be carefully evaluated and should not be overaveraged, which can result in a loss of sensitivity and specificity.