The present results reveal that AD patients with mild cognitive impairment already evidenced significant alterations in their eye movements during reading compared with healthy individuals. Patients with mild AD remarkably increased the amount of total fixations and saccade regressions. They also showed longer fixation durations in spite of a reduction in the size of outgoing saccades.
The basic processes involved in eye movements during reading have been thoroughly described in the literature.
1,33 There is now a growing consensus that eye movement behavior could be used to evaluate cognitive processing during reading,
11,34 since several cognitive processes, including working memory, have been shown to influence saccade parameters.
1,35 Changes in eye movements have been linked to neurological conditions such as Parkinson disease and prefrontal cortex damage.
36,37 Mild cognitive impairment and a deficit in working memory are early symptoms of AD, and AD patients have been reported to evidence saccade dysfunctions.
13,38 Our results show that eye movements during reading were affected in AD patients at very early stages of the disease. Patients participating in this study had MMSE and ACE-R score values of 23.2 and 82.4, respectively, which reflect slight cognitive impairment. These patients evidenced a significant increase in their total number of fixations, with both the amount of first- and second-pass fixations being increased, compared with controls. In healthy individuals, as is observed in the
Table, second-pass fixations are less frequent than first-pass fixations; noteworthy, the former showed the highest increase in AD patients, and almost doubled the amount of first-pass fixations observed for this group. Consistently, AD patients exhibited a simultaneous decrease in the amount of single fixations. A number of hypotheses have been proposed to explain the origin of second-pass fixations, including the necessity for comparison with the past image and interpretation of a given word in the context of a sentence.
39 The increase in these parameters might reflect the deficit in processing the current and incoming information in AD patients.
Backward eye movements, saccade regressions, which also occur during reading, are likely aimed to integrate previously acquired data with the new incoming information.
1 The higher number of regressions observed in AD patients compared with controls is consistent with the impairment in perceptual and cognitive processes associated to word processing in these patients and might be an attempt to compensate for this disability. Remarkably, AD patients also skipped a higher number of words than controls, an observation that might be related to mislocated fixations or a consequence of involuntary triggered saccades.
4
Our work also revealed that fixation duration was significantly longer in AD patients than in controls. Consistent with these data, longer fixation durations have been reported in AD patients during visual search.
40 Information uptake is largely restricted to fixations
1,4 ; hence, the longer fixations registered in AD patients might reflect the difficulties in acquiring and processing the visual inputs in these patients. This finding, together with the increase in the amount of fixations, agree with previous observations that readers make more fixations and fixate for longer time when they experience processing difficulty.
41
In a recent study with subjects reading Chinese,
42 the size of outgoing saccades was proposed to depend on the complexity of the region in which vision is fixated; in general terms: The easier the processing of the fixated region is, the longer the outgoing saccade is. Interestingly, the size of the outgoing saccades was consistently shorter in AD patients than in controls. This reduction might reflect the impairment in memory and interpretation of words in AD patients compared with healthy individuals. Our results also show that the size of the outgoing saccade was more uniform in controls than in AD during processing of word length, word frequency, or word predictability, probably because controls use reading strategies such as word-based strategies or processing-based strategies. These reading strategies seem to be the rule for words of two or more letters. Outgoing saccades were remarkably longer for one-letter words in controls, which might be due to an enhanced parafoveal processing for one-letter words. Alternatively, one-letter words might be processed differently since they are usually connectors and recognized as such by healthy readers. Their use of them for joining previous and incoming words might thus lead to longer saccades. This capacity was absent in AD patients, which showed the shortest outgoing saccades for one-letter words.
The striking modifications observed in the saccadic movements in AD patients at their early stages of development reveal a considerable deterioration of one or more brain structures involved in controlling these behaviors. Visual fixation and saccadic movements are sophisticated responses in which several brain regions participate, including parietal and frontal cortex, basal ganglia, substantia nigra, and subthalamic nuclei, the caudate nuclei and the reticular formation in the brain stem, along with the cerebellum and the superior colliculus in the mesencephalon
43–48 ; all of them must act in a coordinate way. The precise identification of which of the areas affected in AD are responsible for each or all of the alterations observed in eye movements requires further study.
The main conclusion of the present work is that AD patients at early stages of their disease, as those participating in this study, evidenced markedly altered eye movements during reading. This suggests that evaluation of these movements might provide an additional and sensitive tool for the much needed early diagnosis of AD disease.