The use of motion perception tests as possible predictors of driving performance and safety has been gaining momentum in the past decade with a handful of studies demonstrating relationships between the two.
7,9 The goal of our study was to expand on these efforts in two ways: expanding the content of the motion test battery and evaluating the feasibility of motion perception training as a possible means to improve driving safety.
Given the importance of 3D motion perception
2 and eye movements
9 in driving, we hypothesized that the strength of the relationship between motion perception tests and driving performance would be increased if these two variables were incorporated better into the test battery. As discussed above, the majority of studies in this area have used only tests of 2D motion perception, with the one exception being a study in which driving performance/safety was not assessed directly.
18 While the motion perception tests that requires eye movements, namely DVA, have been used in past research in the area,
5 the relationship between DVA threshold and target speed (“velocity susceptibility”), which has shown to be important in other domains,
11 has not been examined in the context of driving to our knowledge.
Consistent with our hypotheses, the 3D motion perception test and our measure of velocity susceptibility (the DVA threshold × target speed slope) along with the UFOV test, showed the strongest relationship with driving performance in our study. The MID test was correlated significantly with emergency braking performance, while the velocity susceptibility was correlated significantly with performance on a hazard perception test. Consistent with some past research, we also found that 2D motion perception was related significantly to our driving performance measures. Taken together, our study suggested that a motion perception test battery should incorporate tests of 2D motion, 3D motion, and DVA (with target speeds that are varied systematically) to maximize predictability. It is important to assess correlations among visual tests to avoid the use of overlapping/redundant tests in a clinical setting, or for administration of driver testing, where large numbers of individuals must be tested as quickly and efficiently as possible. Our results indicated that, while most of the vision tests were uncorrelated, VA and contrast sensitivity were correlated significantly, and DVAs and UFOV also were correlated significantly. The correlation between VA and CS is not surprising; however, the significant correlation between DVAs and UFOV was not expected. As noted above, it may be that deployment of visual attention to a peripheral location is important preceding an eye movement. This relationship could be of interest in further research.
Experiment 2 of our study provides evidence to suggest that motion perception training can have a positive influence on the driving behavior of younger drivers, namely reduced braking reaction times in response to a simulated potential collision. As can be seen in
Figure 4B, a training program that involved repeating 2D, 3D, and DVA tests for 6 weeks resulted in a significant reduction in emergency braking reaction time that was not observed in a control group that received training in driver theory. Given that both groups completed the braking action the same number of times, we would argue that this difference was due to a change at the level of motion processing (e.g., greater sensitivity to looming) rather than at the motor response stage (e.g., faster foot movements from accelerator to brake). However, as discussed above, there also may have been motivational differences between the two groups. Therefore, it will be important for future research to investigate this type of training further (using other types of control/comparison groups) to determine to what extent this effect is due to improved motion perception and to what extent similar effects might be achieved with other types of training (e.g., contrast sensitivity or UFOV).
It also should be noted that the present training study involved young, healthy drivers. It will be important for future research to investigate motion perception training effects in individuals with compromised abilities resulting from ageing, ocular disease, or cognitive impairment. It is reasonable to assume that the training benefits may be even larger in these populations than those observed in our study, but of course that needs to be tested. Consistent with this idea, previous research has shown that one of the tests used in our present study (the MDL test) is sensitive to deficits in a variety of conditions for which standard VA is normal. These include amblyopia,
29 early enucleation,
30 multiple sclerosis,
31 and glaucoma.
32
There are some important limitations to our study. First, it will be important for future research to expand the range of driving tasks used. It will be interesting to examine driving tasks that are associated with a high number of accidents and involve a strong 3D motion component, such as overtaking and passing,
33 and across path turns.
34 Second, it will be important for future research to use stimulus speeds that better represent those experienced in real driving. In our study, we chose to use values similar to those used in previous experiments
11,19 (e.g., 0.05 m/s for the MID test and 0.05–0.2 m/s for the DVA test) instead of trying to match the speeds to those experience in the driving task (11–15 m/s). Finally, as discussed above, it will be important to determine the effectiveness of motion perception training relative to other types of perceptual and attention training.