June 2013
Volume 54, Issue 15
Free
ARVO Annual Meeting Abstract  |   June 2013
Luminance variations can reduce or reverse plus lens compensation in chicks
Author Affiliations & Notes
  • Alan Busby
    Biology, City College of New York, New York, NY
  • Footnotes
    Commercial Relationships Alan Busby, None
  • Footnotes
    Support None
Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science June 2013, Vol.54, 5181. doi:
  • Views
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Alan Busby; Luminance variations can reduce or reverse plus lens compensation in chicks. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2013;54(15):5181.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract
 
Purpose
 

Luminance varies significantly in the natural environment, often by 5-6 log units or more. Published experiments have shown that bright lights can alter the expected emmetropization responses in animals (Ashby & Schaeffel, IOVS, 2010; Smith et al, IOVS, 2012). It was hypothesized that a visual scene’s brighter areas would affect emmetropization more than its dimmer areas. The experiments below tested whether an environment’s luminance variations could alter chick lens compensation.

 
Methods
 

1-week-old Chicks (8 birds/group with 2 birds/cage) wore a +3D lens on one eye and an occluder on the other in a cylindrically shaped cage (18cm x 12cm high), so that all objects inside the cage had hyperopic defocus while objects outside had myopic defocus. 50% of the cage’s walls were covered with 4 equidistant 7cm wide paper strips, and 4 equal open sections permitted distance views of 0.5 to 3m. The experimental cage had a 5000 lux near light (a 50 watt desk lamp 12cm from the cage floor), while the outside was illuminated with ~60 lux, ~140 lux, or typical overhead fluorescent lighting (~600 lux). The control cage had no near light but equal outside illumination. One eye, with the lens, viewed the experimental cage for 1hr. The lens and occluder were then switched, and the fellow eye viewed the control cage through the lens for 1hr. Chicks had 2 exposures per condition on one day, and 2 more the following day over 24hrs.

 
Results
 

The control eyes became hyperopic as expected with +3D lenses. However, the experimental eye’s hyperopic responses were statistically significantly reduced or reversed in the experimental eye for all three background light levels (~60, ~140, ~650 lux), based on a thinner choroid (-69±12 vs. 6±17, -53±12 vs. -4±20, 31±16 vs. 64±16µm), larger vitreous chamber depth (86±18 vs. 2±29, 55±23 vs. -11±17, -27±19 vs. -99±12µm), and more negative refraction (-1.59±0.53 vs. +1.19±0.64, -1.85±0.58 vs. +0.12±0.9D, not tested). Axial changes were not expected due to the short time period.

 
Conclusions
 

A visual scene’s brighter areas seem to affect lens compensation more than darker areas, even when light levels are comparable to common environments. Plus lens compensation was inhibited or even reversed when the brightest areas had hyperopic defocus. This phenomenon may help explain some of the seemingly contradictory results between human epidemiological and animal emmetropization studies.

 
 
View from inside cage
 
View from inside cage
 
Keywords: 677 refractive error development • 605 myopia • 511 emmetropization  
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×