We have used three experimental measures (RVC, CS, RI) and one
anecdotal observation (subjective point spread function), all of which
support the hypothesis that tear break-up introduces significant
optical changes in the eye that can result in highly degraded retinal
image quality and reduced spatial vision. These data confirm and extend
the results of Albarran
1 and may explain the fluctuating
blurry vision reported by groups of clinical patients with dry
eye.
16 18 19 20
It is well known that the cornea has a microscopically irregular
surface that is neutralized or “optically smoothed” by the
tears.
2 Our results add further weight to this conclusion
by demonstrating the loss of image quality that results when the tear
film is disrupted. However, it is uncertain from our results whether
the decrease in optical performance during a period of nonblinking is
due to exposure of the underlying irregular corneal surface,
nonuniformity of the refractive index of the tears, the irregular
surface of the tear film during disruption, or a combination these
factors. It is also possible that tear break-up leads to dehydration of
the cornea, which in turn may produce structural and thus optical
changes. Our results are quantitatively very different from those
reported by Timberlake et al.,
9 who found no measurable
decline in low contrast acuity after several minutes of nonblinking
when there was not a SCL covering the eye; however, our results do show
the same trend as Timberlake in that psychophysical CS was affected
more by prolonged periods of nonblinking with a SCL than without
(Fig. 6) . This suggests that differences between studies may be due to
inter-subject variability in tear break-up.
In the case of contact lens wear, it has been suggested that
psychophysical losses of low-contrast acuity
9 or light
scattering
12 associated with periods of nonblinking in
eyes wearing SCL
(Fig. 5) are due to changes in lens parameters (e.g.,
curvature, refractive index, transparency) secondary to lens
dehydration. However, the irregular surface of the tear film during
disruption must also be a contributing factor to reduced image quality
in eyes wearing soft contact lenses. In either case, Begley et
al.
21 have shown that blurry or changeable vision is a
common symptom among contact lens wearers.
Measures of image quality such as those used in this study (RVC, CS,
subjective point spread function) cannot easily identify the optical
cause of the image degradation accompanying tear break-up. In the
introduction, we argue that tear break-up could produce either scatter,
aberrations, or both, which may be responsible for the reductions in
image quality seen in this and previous experiments.
1 9 12 Significant local aberrations are probable at the edge of a break in
the tear film where surface slope is likely to be very different from
the underlying cornea and surrounding intact tear film. Therefore, the
question arises as to whether aberrations associated with tear break-up
could be responsible for reducing image contrast at the spatial scale
monitored in our experiment (vessels and character stroke widths of
approximately 1/4 to 1/3 of a degree). Experiments have
shown that even the relatively small-amplitude, higher-order, irregular
aberrations present in normal eyes significantly attenuate image
contrast at spatial frequencies below 15 cycles per
degree.
25 Therefore, it is likely that the
increased aberrations and the optical scatter observed after tear
break-up will further reduce image contrast at these low
frequencies.
10 11
RI of the tear film showed that disruption of the optical surface that
began very soon after blinking was suspended in some eyes
(Fig. 7) .
This was consistent with the observed decrements in RVC and CS
(Figs. 4 5 6) . Initial foci of disruption developed into rod-shaped or
branching rivulets or spots rapidly spreading across the RI image. The
pupil center was not always involved in the initial disruption, and
therefore, initial tear break-up in noncyclopleged eyes would not be
expected to have any immediate optical impact on foveal image quality.
However, we found that tear disruption eventually encroached upon the
central pupil and thus degraded retinal image quality. Although we have
argued that the RI data represent the spatial distribution of
aberrations across the tear film,
23 this method (unlike
the Shack-Hartmann wavefront sensor
10 11 ) cannot yet
specify these aberrations in quantitative terms (e.g., micrometers of
wavefront deviation).
Although we observed significant trial-to-trial variability, image
degradation progressed at similar rates for all eyes in SCL and NSCL
trials. Albarran
1 also observed only small inter-subject
variability, and only slightly greater effects of nonblinking in eyes
with SCLs when compared with exposed corneas. These results are in
stark contrast to the data of Timberlake et al.
9 and
Lohmann et al.,
12 who compared eyes wearing soft contact
lenses and rigid gas permeable lenses (RGP) to the uncovered cornea.
They found that eyes wearing soft contact lenses were affected more by
nonblinking than were eyes with RGPs or without contact lenses, which
were virtually unaffected by up to 5 minutes of nonblinking. The
discrepancy between their studies and our data may be attributable to
one of the following factors: First, in our study it was essential to
use local anesthetic and control light levels to prevent reflexive
tearing during extended periods of nonblinking. Methods to prevent
reflex tearing were not reported in either previous study. Second, we
have observed subjects in our laboratory who fail to show tear break-up
up to 1 minute after a blink, and thus the inter-study difference may
simple reflect inter-subject differences in the stability of tears.
In this study, we monitored tear film changes with prolonged periods of
nonblinking, either with or without a soft contact lens. Under natural
conditions, it is likely that blinking would occur much more
frequently. The average blink rate is approximately 12
blinks/min,
26 although there is considerable variation
between individuals and visual tasks.
27 28 29 Thus, under
normal viewing conditions in nondiseased eyes, it is likely that the
retinal image degradation observed in our experiments and the
accompanying visual disturbance would be minimized. However, Patel et
al.
29 found that visual tasks requiring concentration,
such as VDT use, caused a fivefold decrease in blink rate, from an
average of 18.4 blinks/min to only 3.6 blinks/min. According to our
data, this could be associated with a 20% to 40% loss in image
contrast. In addition, patients with dry eye often have an inadequate
or unstable tear film, which breaks up quickly over the surface of the
eye.
14 15 16 If blinking does not occur rapidly enough
retinal image quality should be compromised in these patients. This
prediction was verified by Goto et al.,
17 who demonstrated
a drop in visual acuity of 0.3 when 16 patients with dry eye held their
eyes open for only 10 seconds. Blurry or disturbed vision has also been
reported in a number of clinical studies of patients with dry
eye.
16 18 19 20 Therefore, it is likely that tear film
disruption will cause some degree of retinal image degradation in
patients with dry eye or normal individuals in everyday life, depending
on the visual task.