Hypermetropic refractive errors increased over time in some of the
children of the cohort, and our longitudinal data allow us to examine
the effect of refractive error changes on accommodation. First, for the
purposes of comparison, accommodative responses in a normal adult
emmetrope with presbyopia were examined. In
Figure 2 , total accommodation produced by the subject for the target at the
three distances, in response to increasing accommodative demand due to
(simulated) effective refractive error is shown. As expected, the
curves for the different stimulus distances superimposed (i.e., the
total accommodation exerted) were similar at all stimulus distances,
being subject presumably to the individual’s maximum amplitude of
accommodation (in the case illustrated, 4.50 D). Thus, no matter what
distance the target was from the subject, he exerted maximum
accommodation in an attempt to make the target as optically clear as
possible. Further, the total accommodation changed very little (just
over 1.00 D) when the simulated refractive error increased.
By contrast, the longitudinal data for each individual child with Down
syndrome show quite a different, and unexpected, response.
Figure 3 shows the total accommodation in response to changing accommodative
demand of one child seen at regular intervals between the ages of 8.5
and 18.5 months.
Figures 3A 3B and 3C show responses for target
distance equivalent to 4.00, 6.00, and 10.00 D, respectively. The
curves for each stimulus distance closely follow any change in
accommodative demand, imposed by a change in refractive error. At the
age of 8.5 months, the refractive error was +1.75 D. When the child
fixated the 10.00-D stimulus
(Fig. 3C) , the demand was 10.00 +
1.75 = 11.75 D, and the total accommodative response was 5.32 D.
Between 12 and 18 months of age, her refractive error increased; at
18.5 months it was +3.75 D. If the accommodation were limited at these
demands, as in the case of a presbyope, little, if any, change in total
accommodation would be expected. However, in response to a 10.00-D
stimulus, we now measured a total accommodative response of 6.98 D, an
increase of 1.66 D. At each target distance, as effective refractive
error (and therefore accommodative demand) changed, the total
accommodative response changed accordingly. Further, the response to a
stimulus at 10.00 D was always greater than the response to a stimulus
at the other two distances.
Figure 3D shows the AEI for this particular
child, which remained similar (4.54 D at 8.5 months and 4.54 D at 18.5
months), despite large changes in refractive error and therefore in
accommodative demand.
Figure 4 shows the response curves for the three target distances on a single
graph. Rather than superimposing, as was the case for the presbyope
(Fig. 2) , the curves remained separate. This phenomenon was observed in
each child who participated in the longitudinal study and who had an
AEI greater than zero on each occasion (
n = 64). In no
case did the curves for the different target distances superimpose.