Experiment 1 showed that visual control of equilibrium in the
lateral direction in subjects with CN appears to have greatest efficacy
for low-frequency components of sway (<1 Hz). However, visual control
of equilibrium was less effective in subjects with CN than in control
subjects. We could detect a marginal loss of visual stabilization of
high-frequency head movements in subjects with CN compared with control
subjects. However, high-frequency components to head movement were
minimal in both the CN-affected and the control subjects (acuity and
normal controls) and thus had little implication for postural stability
as measured. COP had more power at high frequencies than head movement.
The reduction of COP instability due to vision, was smaller in subjects
with CN than in control subjects across all frequencies, but
particularly at frequencies of more than 1 Hz. No difference was
observed between the two control groups tested (acuity controls and
normal controls), indicating that the differences between the subjects
with CN and the control subjects was not a consequence of the slightly
reduced visual acuity of the subjects with CN included in experiment 1.
With eyes closed, COP and head stability were similar in the subjects
with CN and the control subjects. These results are consistent with
previous observations,
7 which support the thesis that
somatosensory and vestibular controls of posture in subjects with CN
are normal.
Experiment 2 showed that the use of visual information to control body
orientation in space (i.e., overall tilt) was normal in CN. Visually
induced body sway under conditions of absolute motion and motion
parallax did not differ among subject groups. Consistent with reports
in the literature, absolute motion induced ipsidirectional body
sway,
10 11 12 whereas juxtaposing a stationary target
between subject and background provoked sway contradirectional to
background motion.
13 14 Thus, despite their nystagmus,
subjects with CN made normal use of visual motion cues, including
motion parallax, to control postural orientation.
Insight into the impairment of visual control of high-frequency
postural instability in CN is given by the behavior of normal subjects
in stroboscopic light, when the flashes are presented at a strobing
frequency of 3 to 5 Hz.
22 23 24 25 26 27 This frequency range is
similar to the nystagmus frequency in this sample of subjects with CN
(see
Table 1 ). Isableu et al.
22 showed that subjects with
normal vision, standing in front of a tilted frame, leaned in the
direction of tilt under normal or stroboscopic lighting (2.8 Hz).
Displacement of an oscillating background under strobed light also
causes a continuous modulation of low-frequency postural
sway.
23 These results indicate that discrete visual
sampling is sufficient for controlling body orientation. However,
unlike body orientation, normal equilibrium appears to be degraded
under stroboscopic vision at frequencies lower than 6
Hz.
24 25 26 Measured at different levels, from ankle to
head, the destabilizing effect of such discrete visual sampling
principally affects the lower parts of the body.
27 The
latter investigators concluded that discrete visual information (static
cues) were sufficient to control the upper part of the body, which has
predominantly low-frequency dynamics. Visual motion cues (dynamic cues)
control oscillations of the lower part of the body, which extend
through a higher frequency range. Thus, subjects with CN appear to be
similar to normal subjects in stroboscopic light, in that they share
some ability to orient and control low-frequency head instability but
are less able to control the higher frequency instabilities of the COP
with the visual cues available. Dynamic visual cues, requiring
continuous visual feedback, appear to be particularly crucial for fast
stabilization of the COP.
The similarity between normal subjects in stroboscopic light and
subjects with CN is consistent with the concept that the waveform of CN
affords intermittent, low-frequency visual sampling at the time of the
foveation periods. Subjects with CN do not behave as though they were
exposed to continuous visual motion because of their nystagmus, and
this intermittent sampling of vision to control posture may be related
to the mechanism whereby they suppress oscillopsia.
The mechanisms proposed for suppressing oscillopsia in CN include a
reduced sensitivity to retinal image motion
5 6 28 ; an
ability to extract visual information during foveation periods (the
parts of the nystagmus waveform when the eyes are quiescent and
images are most stable on the fovea) and to ignore the smeared vision
during high-velocity slow phases
2 29 30 ; and the use of
extraretinal signals—i.e., efference copy of the CN waveform—to
negate the visual effects of the oscillation.
4 31 32 Of
these, the most recent evidence suggests that the efference copy of the
CN waveform is the major factor in oscillopsia
suppression.
4 32 Although foveation periods may not be
primarily responsible for oscillopsia suppression, they are important
for visual acuity,
32 and they may be responsible for the
discrete sampling of visual cues to postural orientation in CN.
The authors thank the volunteers who enthusiastically participated
in this project.