The group of subjects with ARM recruited for this study comprised a well-defined subset of patients at high risk of development of AMD, who had relatively good visual acuity
(Table 1) , either multiple soft drusen bilaterally with or without pigmentary changes, or unilateral soft drusen and an end-stage lesion (choroidal neovascularization) in the fellow eye. These subjects were at risk for development of the later complications of AMD,
6 and in this group, cone steady state thresholds were impaired in 31% to 44% of subjects
(Table 2) .
Because ARM is primarily a disease that affects the outer retina,
4 7 43 it is not surprising that a loss in rod function has been reported, particularly in rod adaptation.
20 21 This loss is thought to reflect either a barrier effect at Bruch’s membrane or dysfunction in the RPE. Both factors are likely also to produce widespread involvement of cone function. The purpose of our study was to consider whether cone adaptation was abnormal and whether it was more affected than steady state cone function, as has been reported previously for rods.
21 Table 2 compares the percentage of the ARM group who showed abnormal (
P < 0.05) steady state and kinetic outcomes for cone function. In this table, the time constant of cone adaptational recovery was the single most affected parameter in most subjects with ARM and caused significantly more individuals (χ
2 = 4.03,
P < 0.05) to be classified as having abnormal performance than did any of the steady state thresholds. This finding is consistent with the report of abnormalities in rods (85%), particularly the abnormal kinetics of rod adaptation (65%) noted in a similar cohort of patients with ARM.
21 Although the findings for photopic and scotopic adaptational recovery were collected by using different test procedures and populations, both findings support the hypothesis that the kinetic aspects of visual function are more affected than are steady state thresholds at this stage of the disease.
The delayed kinetics of recovery in patients with ARM probably reflects a slowing in photopigment dynamics, consis tent with the theory that both aging
44 and disease
45 cause a delay in cone photopigment regeneration. A delay in photopigment regeneration, whether it be rod or cone mediated, is a consistent finding in ARM. The molecular mechanisms underlying dark adaptation are becoming clearer in rod photoreceptors,
46 and recent studies have suggested a similar process occurs for cone adaptation.
47 From these studies, it is apparent that the ARM-related Bruch’s membrane and RPE changes would act to slow the recycling of visual photopigments, which could account for the increased time constant of adaptation observed in the present study.
It is important to determine whether steady state and kinetic thresholds identify the same or different aspects of ARM-related dysfunction. This was obtained by establishing correlations across test outcomes (Spearman rank-order coefficient), with the results given in
Table 3 , and a subset shown in
Figure 5 .
Table 3 shows that most of the losses in all steady state thresholds (photopic, spatiotemporal and color) were either significantly correlated or gave a trend (
P < 0.10) toward correlation. Visual acuity (results not shown) correlated significantly (
P < 0.05) with spatial (
r = 0.449) and temporal contrast sensitivity (
r = 0.594), but not with the other functions tested. It is interesting to note that the time constant of cone adaptation did not correlate significantly with any steady state function. This demonstrates that recovery time identifies a unique aspect of ARM not available to the other tests. As it is likely that the abnormality in recovery dynamics arises from a dysfunction in pigment regenerative capacity, the other functional losses probably reflect another receptor-mediated malfunction. It is possible that this arises from the lower optical density and reduced quantal catch of the photopigment.
13 48 Our findings do not exclude postreceptoral involvement, although we believe that this is less likely, given the nature of the disease, and the high correlations among the visual processes, implying a common deficit early in the visual pathway.
Our analysis shows that a combination of kinetic and steady state testing will be needed to identify visual deficits in early ARM, because these tests assay different aspects of visual function. The capacity of various combinations of steady state and kinetic tests to identify ARM is shown in
Table 4 . From this table, it is apparent that 12 of the 16 ARM eyes (75%) could be correctly classified with just two tests, providing that one of these tests includes a test of adaptation dynamics, whereas at least three tests are needed to identify all ARM eyes (88%) that have functional losses. If the same criteria were to be applied to the control subjects, all would be classified as having normal visual performance, using the test combinations given in
Table 4 .
To this extent,
Figure 5 plots the relative loss of color sensitivity, compared with the time constant for adaptation. This figure illustrates the poor correlation between these two functions, reported in
Table 3 . Poor correlations were also evident for the other steady state parameters tested when compared with the time constant for adaptation (data not shown). Although some subjects were abnormal in both color sensitivity and adaptation (four subjects, 25%), most had prolonged recovery time (11 subjects, 69%). Very few had only color loss (one subject, 6%), whereas almost half (7 subjects, 44%) had only a delayed recovery from bleaching.
Figure 5 emphasizes that adaptation dynamics identified the greatest number of subjects with ARM (69%), and the high
z-scores for adaptation mean that subjects with ARM were easy to identify by using this test, particularly when using a linear scale. Finally, that control subjects recovered within 1 minute after bleaching makes this a simple and fast test for clinical application.
Previous reports suggest that subjects who have both abnormal color sensitivity and adaptational dynamics have a high likelihood of development of neovascularization,
13 48 and, consistent with this theory, we find a delayed cone recovery in our subjects with ARM, who are all considered to be at high-risk for the development of neovascular complications. Because such persons often report difficulties recovering from bright lights and moving from light to dim situations,
29 a test of cone adaptation may be a useful objective measurement in determining the extent of cone visual compromise in patients with ARM.