September 2004
Volume 45, Issue 9
Free
Cornea  |   September 2004
Characteristics of Human Corneal Psychophysical Channels
Author Affiliations
  • Yunwei Feng
    From the Centre for Contact Lens Research, School of Optometry, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada.
  • Trefford L. Simpson
    From the Centre for Contact Lens Research, School of Optometry, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada.
Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science September 2004, Vol.45, 3005-3010. doi:https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.04-0102
  • Views
  • PDF
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Yunwei Feng, Trefford L. Simpson; Characteristics of Human Corneal Psychophysical Channels. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2004;45(9):3005-3010. https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.04-0102.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract

purpose. To characterize human corneal psychophysical channels.

methods. Twenty subjects participated in this study. A Belmonte pneumatic esthesiometer was used to deliver stimuli, and the ascending method of limits and the method of constant stimuli were used to estimate thresholds. Sensation was characterized for different stimuli. Corneal mechanical and chemical thresholds were measured at different temperatures.

results. The qualities of the sensations induced by stimuli with different temperatures were different, and the corresponding detection thresholds of the pneumatic stimuli at four temperatures gradually increased (repeated measures ANOVA (F(3,12) = 10.326, P = 0.000). There were no temperature effects on chemical thresholds (repeated measures ANOVA F(3,12) = 0.235, P = 0.870) or mechanical discomfort thresholds from 20°C to 50°C (paired t (14)= −0.233, P = 0.818). There were strong interactions when chemical and mechanical stimuli were added. Chemical thresholds were progressively lower when the flow rate increased and mechanical thresholds went down as the percentage of added CO2 increased (repeated measures ANOVA F(3,12) = 6.407, P = 0.007, F(4,16) = 19.904, P = 0.000).

conclusions. This study suggests that humans sense corneal non-noxious cold and noxious mechanical and chemical stimuli, and that the sensitivity of some submodalities can be modulated by others. There are at least five psychophysical channels (non-noxious cold, noxious mechanical, noxious chemical-H+, noxious heat, and itching) processing corneal sensory information. Both decreased corneal chemical thresholds at high flow rates and decreased mechanical thresholds with an added chemical stimulation demonstrate that corneal psychophysical channels are not independent.

Psychophysical channel theory has been applied in many sensory systems such as vision, audition, and somatosensation. 1 2 In the somatosensory system, different submodalities such as temperature and pressure are hypothesized to be processed by different psychophysical channels and produce different sensations—for example, cold, touch, and pain. 1 2 3 4 Unlike the visual and auditory systems, however, in which different submodalities stimulation are processed independently, psychophysical evidence of the contribution of non-nociceptive stimulation to nociception, for example, suggests that the somatosensory psychophysical channels are not independent. 5 6  
The cornea is innervated by the first branch of the trigeminal nerve. Its afferent information is delivered by the trigeminal nerve to the trigeminal spinal nucleus. The nerve bundles then project to the somatosensory cortex through the thalamus. 7 8 Free nerve endings are the only nerve terminals in the corneal epithelium, which is different from the case in the skin. 9 10 11 This morphologic difference lead von Frey to his specific nerve and function theory, and the concept that nociception is the only sensation that could be evoked from the human cornea has dominated this field for approximately 100 years. 8 12  
Although there have been challenges of von Frey’s conclusion about corneal nociceptive sensation, 13 14 15 16 only recently, with the Belmonte pneumatic esthesiometer, which could deliver mechanical, chemical, and thermal stimuli, has it become possible to characterize systematically the sensation induced by the three submodalities and the sensitivity of the human cornea to these stimuli. 17 18 19 20  
Electrophysiological studies have shown that the corneal nerves respond to thermal, mechanical, and chemical stimulation. 12 18 21 22 23 24 25 Molecular biological techniques have shown that several molecular receptors are expressed by trigeminal neurons, 26 27 28 29 30 31 demonstrating that different submodalities may be sensed or processed differently at the peripheral level. We believe that this multiple representation of receptors provides the substrate for corneal psychophysical channels. With this idea in mind, we designed this study to investigate the characteristics of human corneal psychophysical channels at threshold. Specifically, experiment I was designed to study the interactions of three submodalities at detection threshold, and experiment II was designed to study whether there is a thermal effect on mechanical sensitivity. 
Methods
Subjects
This study followed the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki for research involving human subjects and received approval from the University of Waterloo, Office of Research Ethics (Waterloo, Ontario, Canada). The inclusion criteria were: age 20 to 40 years with no history of contact lens wear, and no ocular or systemic disease. 
The Computer-Controlled Belmonte Pneumatic Esthesiometer
The original Belmonte esthesiometer was manually controlled. 17 20 Mechanical stimuli are generated by pneumatic flow and the chemical stimuli by the controlled proportion of CO2 in the air dissolving in the tears to produce carbonic acid thus decreasing tear pH. 25 The air temperature is varied to deliver (calibrated) thermal stimulation of the ocular surface. There are two flow controllers regulating air and CO2 flow rate and a thermostat controls stimulus temperature. A solenoid is used to control the duration of the pneumatic stimuli. We developed a computerized controller and software that added greater flexibility and ease of use. 
A microcomputer controlled the experiment: It received responses from the subject’s response box and regulated the flow rate and concentration of the CO2 during the experiment. It also received feedback from the temperature sensor and regulated the stimulus temperature. The distance between the tip of the nozzle and the ocular surface was continually monitored by means of a calibrated signal from a video camera and set at 5 mm. 
Procedure
Measurements were taken on the left eye and, during measurements, subjects were seated comfortably in front of the instrument. The subjects were asked to look at a target using the untested eye, and the investigator positioned the tip of the esthesiometer to stimulate the central cornea. 
Experiment I was designed to measure the mechanical and chemical sensory threshold at different temperatures as well as the quality of the sensation. Five male subjects (30–38 years) participated in this measurement. The method of constant stimuli was used for estimating sensory threshold. The ascending method of limits was used to get an approximate stimulus range, which was then used to set five levels of the stimuli for the method of constant stimuli. The step between each stimulus level was 10 mL/min for mechanical and 10% CO2 for chemical stimulation, respectively. The stimuli were presented randomly, with each stimulus level presented six times. Stimulus duration was 2 seconds. This stimulus duration was selected because it was long enough to allow particularly the slowly developing chemical sensation to occur, not long enough to induce discomfort while subjects kept their eyes open during stimulus delivery and brief enough so that the method of constant stimuli was not too protracted. 
The threshold was defined as the flow rate or percentage of CO2 which the subject sensed 50% of the time. The psychometric data were fitted using a logistic function  
\[y{=}1\ {-}\ 1{/}{[}1\ {+}\ (x{/}t)^{S}{]}\]
from which the threshold (t) was estimated. 
The measurements were made at 20°C, 30°C, 40°C, and 50°C. Only two subjects were able to complete the 60°C measurements. Flow thresholds were estimated with 0% CO2 added as well as with systematically increased (10% step size), randomly presented additional CO2. For each temperature, therefore, the data were collected in a cube with the z-axis being the percentage correct and the x- and y-axis being flow rate and percentage of CO2, respectively. 
Experiment II was designed to test thermal effects on mechanical threshold. Four female and 11 male subjects (age range, 20–40 years) participated. The ascending method of limits was used to estimate threshold. The lowest flow rate was set at 20 mL/min. If the stimulus was not detected, the flow rate was increased in steps of 20 mL/min. When the subject felt the stimulus, the intensity was decreased by 20 mL/min and then increased in 10-mL/min steps. If it was detected again, the intensity was recorded. Then the procedure was repeated twice, and the threshold was defined as the average intensity of the three detections. The stimulus duration was 2 seconds. Detection thresholds were measured at 20°C and 50°C. Irritation threshold was measured at 20°C. 
Subjects reported whether the stimulus was detected using a response box. In addition, when subjects reported the stimulus as present, they were required to report the attributes of the sensation. This was noted, and, when required, additional questions were asked by the experimenter to categorize more clearly the information from the subject. 
Statistical Analysis
Repeated measures ANOVA and post hoc tests were used to evaluate the statistical significance in experiment I, set at P < 0.05. Two-tailed paired t-tests with Bonferroni correction were used in experiment II to estimate the significance, set at P < 0.017. 
Results
Experiment I demonstrated that the quality of sensation at threshold changed gradually from cold to irritation (occasionally itching) when the temperature of the air without CO2 was gradually increased from room temperature to 50°C. The sensation evoked by CO2 was a stinging or burning pain that could be easily differentiated from the sensation evoked by air without CO2. When the air was set at 60°C, it evoked a pricking or stinging pain. Figure 1 shows that without CO2, the detection thresholds at four temperatures gradually increased. Repeated-measures ANOVA showed a significant temperature effect (F(3,12) = 10.326, P = 0.000). Post hoc tests showed significant differences in thresholds between 20°C and 40°C, 20°C and 50°C, and 30°C and 50°C. 
There was no significant temperature effect on chemical threshold from 20°C to 50°C (Fig. 2 , repeated measures ANOVA F(3,12) = 0.235, P = 0.870). The chemical threshold, however, was decreased at 60°C (without statistical analysis, as only two subjects could complete this experiment). There were strong interactions when chemical and mechanical stimuli were added (Fig. 3 , repeated measures ANOVA F(3,12) = 6.407, P = 0.007, Fig. 4 , F(4,16) = 19.904, P = 0.000). Chemical thresholds were progressively lower when the flow rate increased and mechanical thresholds went down as the percentage of added CO2 increased. 
When the flow rate was at threshold at 20°C, the burning pain evoked by CO2 contained a cooling component. This thermal cooling sensation, however, gradually disappeared when the concentration of CO2 reached higher levels. This is illustrated in Figures 5 and 6 , showing the data of subjects 2 and 5, respectively. 
In experiment II, in addition to detection thresholds at 20°C and 50°C, we measured each subject’s irritation threshold at 20°C. The corneal cold threshold was 56.1 ± 4.9 mL/min (mean ± SE), and mechanical irritation thresholds were 88.0 ± 7.2 mL/min (20°C) and 89.8 ± 6.3 mL/min (50°C) (Fig. 7) . Two-tailed paired t-tests with Bonferroni correction showed that there was no difference between the irritation thresholds at the two temperatures (t (14)= −0.233, P = 0.818). There was, however, a significant difference between cold and irritation thresholds at both temperatures (t (14) = −5.789, P = 0.000 and t (14)= −5.004, P = 0.000). 
Discussion
The four different kinds of stimuli—mechanical, thermal cold, thermal heat, and chemical CO2—delivered by the Belmonte pneumatic esthesiometer provide us with an ideal method to test the parallel processing hypothesis in the human cornea. In the present study, at 20°C, the initially detected sensation was described as cool or cold, which is the same as reported by us and others. 15 16 This is not surprising, because there is substantial new evidence that the sensation evoked by stimulating the human cornea with room temperature air is one of cold. 15 16 19 32 33 This detection is most likely subserved by the reportedly 10% of fibers in the cornea that respond to downward change in temperature in the non-noxious range. 18 34 The cold sensation gradually changed to irritation with less report of cold, as the stimulus temperatures increased from 20° to 50°C. This too is consistent with recent evidence that pneumatic stimulation of the cornea excites nociceptors that are either broadly tuned to a variety of stimuli (polymodal nociceptors) or are more specific mechanonociceptors. 12 21 22 24 34 35 36 The sensation evoked by CO2 was stinging or burning pain, as has been reported in other studies. 17 18 19 20 The putative neuronal substrate for this is polymodal nociceptors or chemonociceptors. 22 23 24 25 35 36  
The sensory quality change with different submodalities at threshold supports the view that nociceptive sensation is not the only sensation that may be evoked in the human cornea, 13 19 and it provides psychophysical evidence that subcomponents of corneal sensory information are sensed and processed differently. If corneal sensory processing were similar to the skin, with mechanical stimuli processed by different psychophysical channels, 3 4 37 then corneal sensory information would be processed by different psychophysical channels, which, according to psychophysical channel theory, 1 possess different sensitivities. This actually was demonstrated by this study in which it was shown that the threshold of cold (20°C) was different from that of irritation (50°C), and it was also supported by the finding that, at 20°C and 50°C, the irritation thresholds were approximately equal, but each was different from the cold threshold. 
In the visual system, different psychophysical channels are supposed to be independent at threshold. 1 38 This, however, is not the case in the skin. For example, it has been demonstrated that heat-induced pain may diminish vibrotactile perception, 6 and heat may increase the sense of pain. 5 To demonstrate whether there is a thermal effect on mechanical sensitivity, experiment II was designed to test mechanical irritation threshold at 20°C and 50°C, respectively. No effect of temperature on mechanical sensitivity was found. 
Corneal chemical sensitivity was not affected by temperature when the stimulus temperature ranged from 20°C to 50°C. This demonstrates that the non-noxious temperature change has no effect on chemical channels. On the other hand, when the air was set at 60°C, the chemical threshold decreased, implying that noxious heat could facilitate the sensing of chemical stimulation. These results are supported by the electrophysiological finding that the cold stimulus would not have reached noxious levels and therefore would not be expected to stimulate polymodal nociceptors, whereas chemical and noxious thermal stimuli could each affect corneal polymodal nociceptors, therefore inducing the chemical and thermal threshold interaction. 12 18 21 24 25  
Another result demonstrating an interaction was that as the flow rate increased, the chemical threshold decreased, and as the concentration of CO2 increased, the mechanical threshold decreased. This additive effect demonstrates that these two submodalities are not processed independently at threshold. At the neuron level, this is also supported by the observation that both mechanical and chemical stimuli excited corneal polymodal nociceptors. 22 23 24 This may also reflect mechanical and chemical receptors interacting with each other at the molecular level and/or an interaction at a more proximal level in the neurons of the spine, thalamus, or cortex. There is an additional possibility that the higher mechanical stimulation caused the greater availability of CO2 molecules to dissolve in the tears, and this mechanical stimulus may have reduced the thickness of tear film and thus increased the concentration of CO2. This stimulus-based explanation does not account for the fact that CO2 affected the mechanical threshold, however. 
Electrophysiological and molecular studies provide evidence of the anatomic and physiological substrates of psychophysical channels. These include different molecular receptors and nerves that respond to different stimuli. Based collectively on these anatomical and physiological experiments and our psychophysical experiments, we propose that the psychophysical corneal innocuous cold channel (ICC) processes cold stimulation mediated by cold menthol receptor type 1 (CMR1)/transient receptor potential menthol 8 (TRPM8) which responds from 8°C to 30°C and is expressed on trigeminal ganglion C fibers 30 31 or the complex interaction between ion channels, 39 expressed on the cold neurons, producing the cooling sensation. A corneal noxious heat channel (NHC) mediates thermal pain, and its possible substrate may be the vanilloid receptor (VR)-1, which is expressed on Aδ and C fibers. 26 The corneal chemical channel-H+ (CCH+) senses hydrogen ion through VR-1 and/or the acid-sensitive ion channels (ASICs) 27 that are expressed on Aδ and C fibers and produce stinging or burning pain, and the corneal itching channel (CIC) that possibly processes histamine and other itch stimuli is itch specific. 40 41  
The irritative sensation evoked by air (without CO2) is nociceptive, but is different from that of stinging or burning pain caused by CO2. It may be proposed that this difference occurs because both mechanonociceptors and polymodal nociceptors are recruited by mechanical stimulation, and the joint excitation of these two kinds of sensory nerves may produce a slightly different sensation from that produced by the polymodal nociceptor excitation by the chemical stimulus. 18 This seems unnecessarily complex and involves a novel interaction between mechano- and polymodal nociceptors never before described. In contrast, a simpler hypothesis, derived from the physiological description of the rabbit cornea is that the chemical stimuli excite predominantly C-fiber chemonociceptors, whereas the mechanical stimuli excite predominantly Aδ driving mechanonociceptors. 35 36 This hypothesis is in line with the phenomenology of the sensations inasmuch as chemical stimuli typically burn and mechanical stimuli give rise to foreign-body (sharp) sensations 42 as well as the temporal characteristics of these neurons, with C-fiber stimulation being associated with late (“second”) pain and Aδ stimulation occurring more quickly (“first” pain). 43 At the molecular level, the mechanical irritation may be mediated by mechanical receptors rather than VR-1 at the peripheral level. Mechanically sensitive receptors such as the degenerin/epithelial sodium channel (DEG/ENaC) family, which mediate mechanical force transduction and are expressed by trigeminal ganglion Aδ and possibly C fibers, 28 29 may be the potential substrate of the corneal mechanical channel (MC). 
The interaction between thermal and chemical and between mechanical and chemical modalities presented by this study demonstrates that these corneal channels are not completely independent at threshold. In experiment I, when the air was set at 20°C, the cold component of the sensation went down if the concentration of CO2 was high (Figs. 5 6) . The suppression of innocuous cold by CO2-induced pain may share a mechanism similar to that of heat-induced pain that diminishes touch sensitivity. 6 This possibly reflects “gate control theory” at the spinal level in which noxious and innocuous inputs are controlled. 44 We could not, however, exclude the possibility that this may happen at the peripheral level, as it has been shown that approximately 50% of CMR1-expressing trigeminal neurons also express VR1, and it has been proposed that cold and heat may elicit different neuronal activities. 30 In addition, when one stimulus contains two components, such as in experiment I, the activity of these two receptors may be modulated by another receptor. 
When the air was set at 60°C, the subjects became more sensitive to CO2. This may occur at the neuron level by both heat and chemical stimuli exciting corneal polymodal nociceptors because at the molecular receptor level, both heat and H+ excite the VR-1 receptor. 26 45 Alternatively, it could be that chemical and warm stimuli have a facilitative interaction when they activate separate molecular systems. 5 The mechanical and chemical interaction may also imply a similar underlying mechanism. Although ASICs have been demonstrated to be the substrate of hydrogen ion sensing, 27 as members of DEG/ENaC family, they have been proposed to be a mechanoreceptor. 46 Furthermore, it has been found that both VR-1 and ASICs are expressed on the same neurons in rat dorsal root ganglia (DRG). 47  
It has been shown that cooling (with a temperature decrease of 17°C) inhibits capsaicin-induced currents in rat dorsal root ganglion neurons. 48 In addition, strong cooling relieves dermal pain. 49 In the present study, neither corneal chemical sensitivity nor mechanical sensitivity was decreased by ocular surface cooling. This possibly implies that decreasing the corneal surface temperature within the limited range, as in this experiment, is insufficient to affect VR1 or mechanical receptors. 
Most natural external stimuli do not contain only one submodality. It seems that corneal nerves represent the sensory information simultaneously and in parallel when external stimuli are presented on the corneal surface—for example, cold, and stinging. However, unlike somatosensory psychophysical channels arising particularly from the skin possessing receptors with different nerve morphology, 3 4 corneal nociceptors and cold nerve fibers posses the same morphology (i.e., they are bare terminals). 10 11 12 18 22 23 25 Despite this similar microscopic architecture, they give rise to distinct sensations and they therefore must be distinctive in ways not yet revealed using microscopy. 
How may these individual neurons integrate multidimensional sensory information peripherally and transmit these different submodalities to the brain to give rise to different sensations? It is possible that in the periphery, the molecular receptors such as CMR1, VR-1, ASIC, and DEG/ENaC, are expressed on the same neuron and excite it in different dynamic ways, such as has been suggested with CMR1 and VR1. 30 This may be related to the observation that electrophysiologically the rates of discharge of cat corneal nerves increased sharply when the corneal temperature was higher than 44°C, 12 14 and this temperature is close to the threshold of noxious thermal pain sensation in humans. In addition, there may be some intermediate neurons or neuronal networks that operate on the neuronal signal by, for example, a lateral inhibitory mechanism. A higher process would then need to decode this multiplexed temporal signal. 50 In contrast, at the somatosensory cortex, the afferent sensory information that arose from physically distinct sensory (corneal) receptors may arrive in physically distinct neurons. It then would require higher processing to deal with physically segregated sensory dimensions. 
The human cornea is innervated by Aδ and C fibers and both are believed to convey nociceptive information. 24 In a previous study, we reported that the burning pain evoked by CO2 was delayed in comparison with the irritation evoked by a purely mechanical stimulus. 20 We have proposed a number of hypothetical reasons for this observation. The more rapidly appearing foreign-body, scratching sensation and the more slowly emerging, stinging sensation evoked by CO2 suggest that mechanical and chemical stimuli trigger fast and slow pain mechanisms that are mediated by Aδ and C fibers. 43 In addition, the fast and slow perceptions may be attributable to the superficial and deep distribution of these two kinds of nerve fibers. 21 35 36 Both of these though, provide physiological evidence of the substrates of the parallel processing of mechanical and chemical stimuli in humans. The delay in the reported burning pain evoked by CO2, however, may also be related to the time necessary for CO2 to react in the tears to produce protons. 
In summary, this study demonstrates the interaction of different submodalities at sensory threshold, psychophysically. We propose that there are at least five corneal psychophysical sensory channels, an innocuous cold channel, a noxious heat channel, a nociceptive mechanical channel, a chemical channel+, and an itching channel. These channels are not completely independent. The interaction between the different psychophysical channels could be related to the type of corneal receptors stimulated, the relative distribution of these membrane receptors expressed on their respective nerve terminals, and the interaction between the receptors and/or nerves. 
 
Figure 1.
 
Corneal mechanical threshold and temperature. The higher the temperature of the stimuli, the higher the threshold. Error bars are 95% confidence intervals for the means in each of the relevant figures.
Figure 1.
 
Corneal mechanical threshold and temperature. The higher the temperature of the stimuli, the higher the threshold. Error bars are 95% confidence intervals for the means in each of the relevant figures.
Figure 2.
 
Corneal chemical threshold at different temperatures. Each line represents chemical thresholds at different temperatures.
Figure 2.
 
Corneal chemical threshold at different temperatures. Each line represents chemical thresholds at different temperatures.
Figure 3.
 
Corneal chemical thresholds and flow rates (20°C–50°C). As the flow rate increased, the chemical threshold decreased.
Figure 3.
 
Corneal chemical thresholds and flow rates (20°C–50°C). As the flow rate increased, the chemical threshold decreased.
Figure 4.
 
The relationship between mechanical threshold and the concentration of CO2. As the concentration of CO2 increased, the mechanical threshold decreased (the concentration of CO2 is the percent of CO2 of the total volume).
Figure 4.
 
The relationship between mechanical threshold and the concentration of CO2. As the concentration of CO2 increased, the mechanical threshold decreased (the concentration of CO2 is the percent of CO2 of the total volume).
Figure 5.
 
The inhibitory effect of burning pain on cooling sensation (50 mL/min).
Figure 5.
 
The inhibitory effect of burning pain on cooling sensation (50 mL/min).
Figure 6.
 
The inhibitory effect of burning pain on cooling sensation (60 mL/min).
Figure 6.
 
The inhibitory effect of burning pain on cooling sensation (60 mL/min).
Figure 7.
 
The effect of temperature on corneal mechanical thresholds. There was no temperature effect (20°C and 50°C) on corneal mechanical irritation thresholds.
Figure 7.
 
The effect of temperature on corneal mechanical thresholds. There was no temperature effect (20°C and 50°C) on corneal mechanical irritation thresholds.
The authors thank the subjects who participated in this study and the reviewers for their suggestions. 
Regan D. Human Brain Electrophysiology. Evoked Potentials and Evoked Magnetic Fields in Science and Medicine. 1989;176–191. Elsevier Amsterdam.
Gescheider GA. Psychophysics: The Fundamentals. 1997; 3rd ed. 1–44. Lawrence Erlbaum Mahwah, NJ.
Bolanowski SJ, Gescheider GA, Checkosky CM. Four channels mediate the mechanical aspects of touch. J Acoust Soc Am. 1988;84:1680–1694. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Bolanowski SJ, Gescheider GA, Verrillo RT. Hairy skin: psychophysical channels and their physiological substrates. Somatosens Mot Res. 1994;11:279–290. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Green BG. Interactions between chemical and thermal cutaneous stimuli: inhibition (counterirritation) and integration. Somatosens Mot Res. 1991;8:301–312. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Apkarian AV, Stea RA, Bolanowski SJ. Heat-induced pain diminishes vibrotactile perception: a touch gate. Somatosens Mot Res. 1994;11:259–267. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Kaufman HE, Barron BA, McDonald MB. The Cornea. 1998; 2nd ed. 23–26. Butterworth-Heinemann Boston.
Miller NR, Newman NJ. Walsh & Hoyt’s Clinical Neuro-ophthalmology. Vol. 1. 1998; 5th ed. 1595–1606. Williams & Wilkins Baltimore.
Schimmelpfennig B. Nerve structures in human central corneal epithelium. Graefes Arch Clin Exp Ophthalmol. 1982;218:14–20. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Muller LJ, Pels L, Vrensen GFJM. Ultrastructural organization of human corneal nerves. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1996;37:476–488. [PubMed]
Muller LJ, Vrensen GFJM, Pels L, Cardozo BN, Willekens B. Architecture of human corneal nerves. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1997;38:985–994. [PubMed]
Giraldez F, Geijo E, Belmonte C. Response characteristics of corneal sensory fibres to mechanical and thermal stimulation. Brain Res. 1979;177:571–576. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Lele PP, Weddell G. The relationship between neurohistology and corneal sensibility. Brain. 1956;79:119–154. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Lele PP, Weddell G. Sensory nerves of the cornea and cutaneous sensibility. Exp Neurol. 1959;1:334–359. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Murphy PJ, Patel S, Marshall J. A new non-contact corneal aesthesiometer. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 1996;16:101–107. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Vega JA, Simpson TL, Fonn D. A noncontact pneumatic esthesiometer for measurement of ocular sensitivity: a preliminary report. Cornea. 1999;18:675–681. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Belmonte C, Acosta MC, Scbmelz M, Gallar J. Measurement of corneal sensitivity to mechanical and chemical stimulation with a CO2 esthesiometer. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1999;40:513–519. [PubMed]
Acosta MC, Belmonte C, Gallar J. Sensory experiences in humans and single-unit activity in cats evoked by polymodal stimulation of the cornea. J Physiol. 2001;534:511–525. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Acosta MC, Tan ME, Belmonte C, Gallar J. Sensations evoked by selective mechanical, chemical, and thermal stimulation of the conjunctiva and cornea. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2001;42:2063–2067. [PubMed]
Feng Y, Simpson TL. Nociceptive sensation and sensitivity evoked from human cornea and conjunctiva stimulated by CO2. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2003;44:529–532. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Tanelian DL, Beuerman RW. Responses of rabbit corneal nociceptors to mechanical and thermal stimulation. Exp Neurol. 1984;84:165–178. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Gallar J, Pozo MA, Tuckett RP, Belmonte C. Response of sensory units with unmyelinated fibres to mechanical, thermal and chemical stimulation of the cat’s cornea. J Physiol. 1993;468:609–622. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Belmonte C, Gallar J, Pozo MA, Rebollo I. Excitation by irritant chemical substances of sensory afferent units in the cat’s cornea. J Physiol. 1991;437:709–725. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Belmonte C, Garcia-Hirschfeld J, Gallar J. Neurobiology of ocular pain. Prog Retin Eye Res. 1997;16:117–156. [CrossRef]
Chen X, Gallar J, Pozo MA, Baeza M, Belmonte C. CO2 stimulation of the cornea: a comparison between human sensation and nerve activity in polymodal nociceptive afferents of the cat. Eur J Neurosci. 1995;7:1154–1163. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Caterina MJ, Schumacher MA, Tominaga M, Rosen TA, Levine JD, Julius D. The capsaicin receptor: a heat-activated ion channel in the pain pathway. Nature. 1997;389:816–824. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Waldmann R, Champigny G, Bassilana F, Heurteaux C, Lazdunski MA. Proton-gated cation channel involved in acid-sensing. Nature. 1997;386:173–177. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Fricke B, Lints R, Stewart G, et al. Epithelial Na+ channels and stomatin are expressed in rat trigeminal mechanosensory neurons. Cell Tissue Res. 2000;299:327–334. [PubMed]
Ugawa S, Ueda T, Takahashi E, et al. Cloning and functional expression of ASIC-beta2, a splice variant of ASIC-beta. Neuroreport. 2001;12:2865–2869. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
McKemy DD, Neuhausse WM, Julius D. Identification of a cold receptor reveals a general role for TRP channels in thermosensation. Nature. 2002;416:52–58. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Peier AM, Moqrich A, Hergarden AC, et al. A TRP channel that senses cold stimuli and menthol. Cell. 2002;108:705–715. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Murphy PJ, Morgan PB, Patel S, Marshall J. Corneal surface temperature change as the mode of stimulation of the non-contact corneal aesthesiometer. Cornea. 1999;18:333–342. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Murphy PJ, Patel S, Morgan PB, Marshall J. The minimum stimulus energy required to produce a cooling sensation in the human cornea. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 2001;1:407–410.
Belmonte C, Acosta MC, Gallar J. Neural basis of sensation in intact and injured corneas. Exp Eye Res. 2004;78:513–525. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
MacIver MB, Tanelian DL. Free nerve ending terminal morphology is fibre type specific for Aδ and C fibres innervating rabbit corneal epithelium. J Neurophysiol. 1993;69:1779–1783. [PubMed]
MacIver MB, Tanelian DL. Structural and functional specialization of A delta and C fiber free nerve endings innervating rabbit corneal epithelium. J Neurosci. 1993;13:4511–4524. [PubMed]
Hollins M, Roy EA. Perceived intensity of vibrotactile stimuli: the role of mechanoreceptive channels. Somatosens Mot Res. 1996;13:273–286. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Stone J. Parallel Processing in Visual System: the Classification of Retinal Ganglion Cells and Its Impact on the Neurobiology of Vision. 1983; Plenum Press New York.
Viana F, de la Pena E, Belmonte C. Specificity of cold thermotransduction is determined by differential ionic channel expression. Nat Neurosci. 2002;5:254–260. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Schmelz M. A neural pathway for itch. Nat Neurosci. 2001;4:9–10. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Andrew D, Craig AD. Spimothalamic lamina I neurons selectively sensitive to histamine: a central neural pathway for itch. Nat Neurosci. 2001;4:72–77. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Handwerker HO, Kobal G. Psychophysiology of experimentally induced pain. Physiol Rev. 1993;73:639–671. [PubMed]
Ploner M, Gross J, Timmermann L, Schnitzler A. Cortical representation of first and second pain sensation in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2002;17:12444–12448.
Melzack R, Wall PD. Pain mechanisms: a new theory. Science. 1965;150:971–978. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Tominaga M, Caterina MJ, Malmberg AB, et al. The cloned capsaicin receptor integrates multiple pain-producing stimuli. Neuron. 1998;21:531–543. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Babinski K, Le KT, Seguela P. Molecular cloning and regional distribution of a human proton receptor subunit with biphasic functional properties. J Neurochem. 1999;72:51–57. [PubMed]
de la Rosa DA, Zhang P, Shao D, White F, Canessa CM. Functional implications of the localization and activity of acid-sensitive channels in rat peripheral nervous system. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2002;99:2326–2331. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Babes A, Amuzescu B, Krause U, Scholz A, Flonta ML, Reid G. Cooling inhibits capsaicin-induced currents in cultured rat dorsal root ganglion neurones. Neurosci Lett. 2002;317:131–134. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Jandera V, Hudson DA, de Wet PM, Innes PM, Rode H. Cooling the burn wound: evaluation of different modalities. Burns. 2000;26:265–270. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Optican LM, Richmond BJ. Temporal encoding of two-dimensional patterns by single units in primate inferior temporal cortex. III. Information theoretic analysis. J Neurophysiol. 1987;57:162–178. [PubMed]
Figure 1.
 
Corneal mechanical threshold and temperature. The higher the temperature of the stimuli, the higher the threshold. Error bars are 95% confidence intervals for the means in each of the relevant figures.
Figure 1.
 
Corneal mechanical threshold and temperature. The higher the temperature of the stimuli, the higher the threshold. Error bars are 95% confidence intervals for the means in each of the relevant figures.
Figure 2.
 
Corneal chemical threshold at different temperatures. Each line represents chemical thresholds at different temperatures.
Figure 2.
 
Corneal chemical threshold at different temperatures. Each line represents chemical thresholds at different temperatures.
Figure 3.
 
Corneal chemical thresholds and flow rates (20°C–50°C). As the flow rate increased, the chemical threshold decreased.
Figure 3.
 
Corneal chemical thresholds and flow rates (20°C–50°C). As the flow rate increased, the chemical threshold decreased.
Figure 4.
 
The relationship between mechanical threshold and the concentration of CO2. As the concentration of CO2 increased, the mechanical threshold decreased (the concentration of CO2 is the percent of CO2 of the total volume).
Figure 4.
 
The relationship between mechanical threshold and the concentration of CO2. As the concentration of CO2 increased, the mechanical threshold decreased (the concentration of CO2 is the percent of CO2 of the total volume).
Figure 5.
 
The inhibitory effect of burning pain on cooling sensation (50 mL/min).
Figure 5.
 
The inhibitory effect of burning pain on cooling sensation (50 mL/min).
Figure 6.
 
The inhibitory effect of burning pain on cooling sensation (60 mL/min).
Figure 6.
 
The inhibitory effect of burning pain on cooling sensation (60 mL/min).
Figure 7.
 
The effect of temperature on corneal mechanical thresholds. There was no temperature effect (20°C and 50°C) on corneal mechanical irritation thresholds.
Figure 7.
 
The effect of temperature on corneal mechanical thresholds. There was no temperature effect (20°C and 50°C) on corneal mechanical irritation thresholds.
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×