The four different kinds of stimuli—mechanical, thermal cold, thermal heat, and chemical CO
2—delivered by the Belmonte pneumatic esthesiometer provide us with an ideal method to test the parallel processing hypothesis in the human cornea. In the present study, at 20°C, the initially detected sensation was described as cool or cold, which is the same as reported by us and others.
15 16 This is not surprising, because there is substantial new evidence that the sensation evoked by stimulating the human cornea with room temperature air is one of cold.
15 16 19 32 33 This detection is most likely subserved by the reportedly 10% of fibers in the cornea that respond to downward change in temperature in the non-noxious range.
18 34 The cold sensation gradually changed to irritation with less report of cold, as the stimulus temperatures increased from 20° to 50°C. This too is consistent with recent evidence that pneumatic stimulation of the cornea excites nociceptors that are either broadly tuned to a variety of stimuli (polymodal nociceptors) or are more specific mechanonociceptors.
12 21 22 24 34 35 36 The sensation evoked by CO
2 was stinging or burning pain, as has been reported in other studies.
17 18 19 20 The putative neuronal substrate for this is polymodal nociceptors or chemonociceptors.
22 23 24 25 35 36
The sensory quality change with different submodalities at threshold supports the view that nociceptive sensation is not the only sensation that may be evoked in the human cornea,
13 19 and it provides psychophysical evidence that subcomponents of corneal sensory information are sensed and processed differently. If corneal sensory processing were similar to the skin, with mechanical stimuli processed by different psychophysical channels,
3 4 37 then corneal sensory information would be processed by different psychophysical channels, which, according to psychophysical channel theory,
1 possess different sensitivities. This actually was demonstrated by this study in which it was shown that the threshold of cold (20°C) was different from that of irritation (50°C), and it was also supported by the finding that, at 20°C and 50°C, the irritation thresholds were approximately equal, but each was different from the cold threshold.
In the visual system, different psychophysical channels are supposed to be independent at threshold.
1 38 This, however, is not the case in the skin. For example, it has been demonstrated that heat-induced pain may diminish vibrotactile perception,
6 and heat may increase the sense of pain.
5 To demonstrate whether there is a thermal effect on mechanical sensitivity, experiment II was designed to test mechanical irritation threshold at 20°C and 50°C, respectively. No effect of temperature on mechanical sensitivity was found.
Corneal chemical sensitivity was not affected by temperature when the stimulus temperature ranged from 20°C to 50°C. This demonstrates that the non-noxious temperature change has no effect on chemical channels. On the other hand, when the air was set at 60°C, the chemical threshold decreased, implying that noxious heat could facilitate the sensing of chemical stimulation. These results are supported by the electrophysiological finding that the cold stimulus would not have reached noxious levels and therefore would not be expected to stimulate polymodal nociceptors, whereas chemical and noxious thermal stimuli could each affect corneal polymodal nociceptors, therefore inducing the chemical and thermal threshold interaction.
12 18 21 24 25
Another result demonstrating an interaction was that as the flow rate increased, the chemical threshold decreased, and as the concentration of CO
2 increased, the mechanical threshold decreased. This additive effect demonstrates that these two submodalities are not processed independently at threshold. At the neuron level, this is also supported by the observation that both mechanical and chemical stimuli excited corneal polymodal nociceptors.
22 23 24 This may also reflect mechanical and chemical receptors interacting with each other at the molecular level and/or an interaction at a more proximal level in the neurons of the spine, thalamus, or cortex. There is an additional possibility that the higher mechanical stimulation caused the greater availability of CO
2 molecules to dissolve in the tears, and this mechanical stimulus may have reduced the thickness of tear film and thus increased the concentration of CO
2. This stimulus-based explanation does not account for the fact that CO
2 affected the mechanical threshold, however.
Electrophysiological and molecular studies provide evidence of the anatomic and physiological substrates of psychophysical channels. These include different molecular receptors and nerves that respond to different stimuli. Based collectively on these anatomical and physiological experiments and our psychophysical experiments, we propose that the psychophysical corneal innocuous cold channel (ICC) processes cold stimulation mediated by cold menthol receptor type 1 (CMR1)/transient receptor potential menthol 8 (TRPM8) which responds from 8°C to 30°C and is expressed on trigeminal ganglion C fibers
30 31 or the complex interaction between ion channels,
39 expressed on the cold neurons, producing the cooling sensation. A corneal noxious heat channel (NHC) mediates thermal pain, and its possible substrate may be the vanilloid receptor (VR)-1, which is expressed on Aδ and C fibers.
26 The corneal chemical channel-H
+ (CCH
+) senses hydrogen ion through VR-1 and/or the acid-sensitive ion channels (ASICs)
27 that are expressed on Aδ and C fibers and produce stinging or burning pain, and the corneal itching channel (CIC) that possibly processes histamine and other itch stimuli is itch specific.
40 41
The irritative sensation evoked by air (without CO
2) is nociceptive, but is different from that of stinging or burning pain caused by CO
2. It may be proposed that this difference occurs because both mechanonociceptors and polymodal nociceptors are recruited by mechanical stimulation, and the joint excitation of these two kinds of sensory nerves may produce a slightly different sensation from that produced by the polymodal nociceptor excitation by the chemical stimulus.
18 This seems unnecessarily complex and involves a novel interaction between mechano- and polymodal nociceptors never before described. In contrast, a simpler hypothesis, derived from the physiological description of the rabbit cornea is that the chemical stimuli excite predominantly C-fiber chemonociceptors, whereas the mechanical stimuli excite predominantly Aδ driving mechanonociceptors.
35 36 This hypothesis is in line with the phenomenology of the sensations inasmuch as chemical stimuli typically burn and mechanical stimuli give rise to foreign-body (sharp) sensations
42 as well as the temporal characteristics of these neurons, with C-fiber stimulation being associated with late (“second”) pain and Aδ stimulation occurring more quickly (“first” pain).
43 At the molecular level, the mechanical irritation may be mediated by mechanical receptors rather than VR-1 at the peripheral level. Mechanically sensitive receptors such as the degenerin/epithelial sodium channel (DEG/ENaC) family, which mediate mechanical force transduction and are expressed by trigeminal ganglion Aδ and possibly C fibers,
28 29 may be the potential substrate of the corneal mechanical channel (MC).
The interaction between thermal and chemical and between mechanical and chemical modalities presented by this study demonstrates that these corneal channels are not completely independent at threshold. In experiment I, when the air was set at 20°C, the cold component of the sensation went down if the concentration of CO
2 was high
(Figs. 5 6) . The suppression of innocuous cold by CO
2-induced pain may share a mechanism similar to that of heat-induced pain that diminishes touch sensitivity.
6 This possibly reflects “gate control theory” at the spinal level in which noxious and innocuous inputs are controlled.
44 We could not, however, exclude the possibility that this may happen at the peripheral level, as it has been shown that approximately 50% of CMR1-expressing trigeminal neurons also express VR1, and it has been proposed that cold and heat may elicit different neuronal activities.
30 In addition, when one stimulus contains two components, such as in experiment I, the activity of these two receptors may be modulated by another receptor.
When the air was set at 60°C, the subjects became more sensitive to CO
2. This may occur at the neuron level by both heat and chemical stimuli exciting corneal polymodal nociceptors because at the molecular receptor level, both heat and H
+ excite the VR-1 receptor.
26 45 Alternatively, it could be that chemical and warm stimuli have a facilitative interaction when they activate separate molecular systems.
5 The mechanical and chemical interaction may also imply a similar underlying mechanism. Although ASICs have been demonstrated to be the substrate of hydrogen ion sensing,
27 as members of DEG/ENaC family, they have been proposed to be a mechanoreceptor.
46 Furthermore, it has been found that both VR-1 and ASICs are expressed on the same neurons in rat dorsal root ganglia (DRG).
47
It has been shown that cooling (with a temperature decrease of 17°C) inhibits capsaicin-induced currents in rat dorsal root ganglion neurons.
48 In addition, strong cooling relieves dermal pain.
49 In the present study, neither corneal chemical sensitivity nor mechanical sensitivity was decreased by ocular surface cooling. This possibly implies that decreasing the corneal surface temperature within the limited range, as in this experiment, is insufficient to affect VR1 or mechanical receptors.
Most natural external stimuli do not contain only one submodality. It seems that corneal nerves represent the sensory information simultaneously and in parallel when external stimuli are presented on the corneal surface—for example, cold, and stinging. However, unlike somatosensory psychophysical channels arising particularly from the skin possessing receptors with different nerve morphology,
3 4 corneal nociceptors and cold nerve fibers posses the same morphology (i.e., they are bare terminals).
10 11 12 18 22 23 25 Despite this similar microscopic architecture, they give rise to distinct sensations and they therefore must be distinctive in ways not yet revealed using microscopy.
How may these individual neurons integrate multidimensional sensory information peripherally and transmit these different submodalities to the brain to give rise to different sensations? It is possible that in the periphery, the molecular receptors such as CMR1, VR-1, ASIC, and DEG/ENaC, are expressed on the same neuron and excite it in different dynamic ways, such as has been suggested with CMR1 and VR1.
30 This may be related to the observation that electrophysiologically the rates of discharge of cat corneal nerves increased sharply when the corneal temperature was higher than 44°C,
12 14 and this temperature is close to the threshold of noxious thermal pain sensation in humans. In addition, there may be some intermediate neurons or neuronal networks that operate on the neuronal signal by, for example, a lateral inhibitory mechanism. A higher process would then need to decode this multiplexed temporal signal.
50 In contrast, at the somatosensory cortex, the afferent sensory information that arose from physically distinct sensory (corneal) receptors may arrive in physically distinct neurons. It then would require higher processing to deal with physically segregated sensory dimensions.
The human cornea is innervated by Aδ and C fibers and both are believed to convey nociceptive information.
24 In a previous study, we reported that the burning pain evoked by CO
2 was delayed in comparison with the irritation evoked by a purely mechanical stimulus.
20 We have proposed a number of hypothetical reasons for this observation. The more rapidly appearing foreign-body, scratching sensation and the more slowly emerging, stinging sensation evoked by CO
2 suggest that mechanical and chemical stimuli trigger fast and slow pain mechanisms that are mediated by Aδ and C fibers.
43 In addition, the fast and slow perceptions may be attributable to the superficial and deep distribution of these two kinds of nerve fibers.
21 35 36 Both of these though, provide physiological evidence of the substrates of the parallel processing of mechanical and chemical stimuli in humans. The delay in the reported burning pain evoked by CO
2, however, may also be related to the time necessary for CO
2 to react in the tears to produce protons.
In summary, this study demonstrates the interaction of different submodalities at sensory threshold, psychophysically. We propose that there are at least five corneal psychophysical sensory channels, an innocuous cold channel, a noxious heat channel, a nociceptive mechanical channel, a chemical channel+, and an itching channel. These channels are not completely independent. The interaction between the different psychophysical channels could be related to the type of corneal receptors stimulated, the relative distribution of these membrane receptors expressed on their respective nerve terminals, and the interaction between the receptors and/or nerves.
The authors thank the subjects who participated in this study and the reviewers for their suggestions.