The straylight value is defined
5 as the “straylight parameter”
s (unit, square degrees per steradian [deg
2/sr]). Simply stated, the straylight parameter reflects how much of the light entering the eye is not focused by the optical media to form a retinal image, but is instead scattered by disturbances in the internal optical elements, causing a veil of light over the retina and leading to a reduction of retinal image contrast. The relation between the straylight parameter and the PSF is given by
s(θ) = θ
2 · PSF(θ), with θ the visual angle in degrees. Because of the approximate Stiles-Holladay law (described earlier), the straylight parameter only weakly depends on θ. Note that because of this definition of the straylight parameter, the total amount of light entering the eye cancels out, since the PSF is normalized to unity. So, pupil size per se does not influence the straylight effects, as discussed earlier in this section. Throughout this article the base 10 (Briggs’) log(
s) will be given.
In a previous study of ocular wall translucency, the relationship between the straylight parameter
s and the so-called diffuse filter value
dfv of a certain piece of ocular wall was derived.
4 The
dfv is defined as the total fraction of light transmitted through the layer under consideration. In other words, the
dfv is the ratio between the total amount of light transmitted by a layer and the total amount of light falling on that layer. Because the eye wall is a very turbid layer in the optical sense, the light exiting at the interior of the eye can be assumed to be fully diffuse. In that case, the following calculation was derived
\[dfv{=}\ \frac{{\pi}{\cdot}s_{\mathrm{t}}}{{\theta}^{2}}\ {\cdot}\ \frac{\mathrm{pupil\ area}}{\mathrm{wall\ area}}\ ,\]
with
s t the contribution to the straylight parameter of the piece of eye wall concerned
4 (e.g., in a light-blue–eyed individual, the
dfv of the iris for red light is found to be 0.01). Note that in both the eye-white and the iris, the pigmented layers on the interior side are the dominant factors for the amount of transmitted light. In that study, ocular wall area was approximated by the area of an annulus around the iris. Because the exact value of the wall area is not well defined, an alternative way to express the amount of transmitted light is used in this study, by calculating the size of a hole in an otherwise opaque eye wall that would transmit the same amount of light. The size of this equivalent hole would correspond to
dfv · (wall area), or to derive it more directly from the translucency part
s t of the straylight parameter
s itself
\[\mathrm{equivalent\ hole\ area}{=}\ \frac{{\pi}{\cdot}s_{\mathrm{t}}}{{\theta}^{2}}\ {\cdot}\ \mathrm{pupil\ area}.\]
From the data given in an earlier article (see Ref.
4 ,
Fig. 3 ), the size of this hole can be derived for the respective cases. For the iris and eye-white of the light-blue–eyed individual, hole areas of respectively 0.19 and 0.51 mm
2 follow. In the blue-eyed individual, the respective values are 0.12 and 0.24 mm
2. In the same study, the straylight contribution for the combination of iris and eye-white was also determined. From the results given in the same
Figure 3 ,
4 the equivalent hole sizes for the combination are 0.70 and 0.35 mm
2 for the light blue and blue eyes, respectively, virtually identical with the mathematical sum of the equivalent hole sizes for iris and eye-white separately. Note that the size of the equivalent hole is a property of the eye wall and is therefore independent of the pupil area.
In the present study, the different parts of the eye wall were not differentiated. We were interested only in the total amount of light penetrating the eye through the eye wall. This value does not change much with pupil size. The examples just given show that the eye white dominates the iris in this respect. Moreover, because the equivalent holes are much smaller than normal pupil sizes, they gain importance only with very small pupils.
In mathematical terms, the pupil-size dependence of straylight can be formulated as follows. In the midregion of pupil sizes, where neither translucency nor lens periphery plays a role, a simple assumption could be that log(
s) is linearly related to pupil diameter
p. In mathematical terms
\[\mathrm{log}(s_{\mathrm{nt}}){=}a{\cdot}p{+}b,\]
with
s nt the part of
s that does not originate from translucency. In practice, this assumption worked well (see the Results section). The parameters
a and
b should be fitted for each angle and subject. In fact, the slope parameter
a was found not to vary significantly between different angles. This would correspond to a rule of constancy of the light-scattering material characteristics over the pupillary plane. Only the
amount of light-scattering material would have to change (for
a ≠ 0).
If we reverse the formula
(equation 2)that derives the equivalent hole as a function of
s t (the part of
s that originates from translucency) we obtain
s t = (equivalent hole area)/(pupil area) · θ
2 /π. If this component is added to the mathematical model for pupil size dependence of the straylight parameter
(equation 3)we obtain
(Fig. 1) :
\[s{=}s_{\mathrm{nt}}{+}s_{\mathrm{t}}{=}10^{a{\cdot}p{+}b}{+}\ \frac{\mathrm{equivalent\ hole\ area}}{\mathrm{pupil\ area}}{\cdot}\ \frac{{\theta}^{2}}{{\pi}}.\]
This function was fitted to the straylight parameter data of the present paper as a function of pupil diameter p, using a least-squares criterion on a logarithmic basis (i.e., the log of this equation was fitted). For each subject, all angles were simultaneously fitted, resulting in one estimate per subject for the slope parameter a and the equivalent hole area. Parameter b was estimated for each angle separately. The angles available with the CRT-based setup were 3.5°, 7°, and 14° and, with the LED instrument, 3.5°, 10°, and 28°. When the parameters in the model are known, the angle at which the translucency part starts to dominate the linear part can be calculated. This value was denoted as the crossing point and calculated for small, intermediate, and large pupil diameters (2.5, 5, and 7.5 mm, respectively). Note that translucency results in a uniform veil of light over the retina. In case the light source is a point, the total light distribution (PSF) at the retina consists of the typical central peak, sloping off to the periphery according to the approximate Stiles-Holladay 1/θ2 law, summed with the uniform translucency background. The crossing point corresponds to where the sloping portion reaches the same value as the uniform background originating from translucency.