February 2008
Volume 49, Issue 2
Free
Eye Movements, Strabismus, Amblyopia and Neuro-ophthalmology  |   February 2008
The Effect of Aging on Torsional Optokinetic Nystagmus
Author Affiliations
  • Shegufta J. Farooq
    From the University of Leicester, Ophthalmology Group, Faculty of Medicine and Biological Sciences, Leicester, United Kingdom; and the
  • Irene Gottlob
    From the University of Leicester, Ophthalmology Group, Faculty of Medicine and Biological Sciences, Leicester, United Kingdom; and the
  • Sherwin Benskin
    University Hospital of Leicester, NHS Trust, Leicester, United Kingdom.
  • Frank A. Proudlock
    From the University of Leicester, Ophthalmology Group, Faculty of Medicine and Biological Sciences, Leicester, United Kingdom; and the
Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science February 2008, Vol.49, 589-593. doi:https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.07-0899
  • Views
  • PDF
  • Share
  • Tools
    • Alerts
      ×
      This feature is available to authenticated users only.
      Sign In or Create an Account ×
    • Get Citation

      Shegufta J. Farooq, Irene Gottlob, Sherwin Benskin, Frank A. Proudlock; The Effect of Aging on Torsional Optokinetic Nystagmus. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 2008;49(2):589-593. https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.07-0899.

      Download citation file:


      © ARVO (1962-2015); The Authors (2016-present)

      ×
  • Supplements
Abstract

purpose. The effect of aging on torsional optokinetic nystagmus (tOKN) is unknown. The authors investigated changes in tOKN associated with aging in a group of healthy subjects.

methods. Monocular torsional eye movements were recorded from 30 subjects between 19 and 72 years of age. Constant-velocity rotary stimuli in clockwise and counterclockwise directions were used to elicit tOKN at 40°/s and 400°/s.

results. The number of subjects in whom tOKN could not be detected increased with age and was consistent in both directions of stimulation and at both angular velocities of stimulation.

conclusions. tOKN appears to fail increasingly with age, in contrast to previous reports of horizontal and vertical OKN systems. This indicates that the ability to respond to rotary motion is more sensitive to the effects of aging.

Torsional optokinetic nystagmus (tOKN) is an ocular motor response that occurs during viewing of a rotating stimulus. Similar to horizontal and vertical OKN responses, it consists of a slow phase in the direction of the stimulus followed by a fast phase in the opposite direction. It contributes to stabilization of the retinal images during rotary movement of the visual field. We have recently described the normal response characteristics of the tOKN response in young adults 1 ; however, the effect of aging on the response has not been investigated. 
In contrast, the horizontal OKN response has been widely investigated with age and has been shown to undergo a mild but significant deterioration. 2 3 4 5 This has been attributed to age-related degeneration in cortical areas responsible for motion perception and in the retinogeniculate pathway, 6 though degeneration of ocular motor areas cannot be ruled out. The effect of vertical OKN has also been investigated. 7 Similarly, OKN responses in elderly subjects have been found to be inferior to those in younger subjects. 
OKN in the horizontal and vertical directions can be influenced by components of the voluntary pursuit system. Therefore, it has been postulated that the effects of aging on the pursuit system could influence the OKN system. 3 Published reports in this area generally agree that the pursuit system deteriorates with age. 3 5 7 8 9 tOKN, however, is purely reflexive and is not influenced by a pursuit mechanism because little or no movement occurs at the fovea when a subject fixates the stimulus. 10 It would be of additional interest, therefore, to see how the tOKN system compares with previously published results on horizontal and vertical OKN. 
We examined the monocular tOKN response in a group of healthy subjects between 19 and 72 years of age by testing eye rotation in intorsion and extorsion directions in response to 40°/s and 400°/s stimulation. 
Subjects and Methods
Subjects
Thirty subjects (19 women, 11 men) between 19 and 72 years of age (mean ± SD, 50.1 ± 18.1 years) were included in the study. All subjects had normal corrected visual acuity of 6/9 (20/30) or better in the viewing eye. Orthoptic examination was performed to exclude any ocular motility and binocular vision defects. All tests were performed without refractive correction or with contact lenses if refractive correction was necessary. On questioning, all subjects reported that they were free of neurologic and otologic problems. Nine of the 30 subjects were taking medication for hypertension (n = 4), cholesterol lowering (n = 4), diabetes (noninsulin-dependent diabetes mellitus, n = 2), or hiatus hernia (n = 1). Seven subjects were older than 65. None of the subjects were taking medication for depression. 
Responses from the right eye were recorded unless visual acuity was less than 6/9 (20/30) or eye movement recording quality was lower than 0.5, in which case the left eye was used (n = 3; torsional eye movement recording quality is defined in Data Analysis). The study received local ethical approval and was performed with consent after explanation of the nature and possible consequences of the study. The study was performed in accordance with tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki. 
Eye Movement Recording
Eye movements were measured in three dimensions using a video-oculography technique (VOG) at a sampling rate of 50 Hz (Strabs system; Sensomotoric GmbH, Teltow, Germany). The equipment consisted of infrared video cameras fitted to a face mask attached to the head with a rubber strap (Fig. 1) . Pupil tracking was used to derive horizontal and vertical movements. A segment of the iris was tracked to measure torsional eye movements. The system has a spatial resolution of 0.03°, 0.02°, and 0.1° and a linearity of ±3.8%, ±3.2%, and ±1.4% full-scale reading for horizontal, vertical, and torsional eye movements, respectively (company specifications). The range of linear measurement was ±25°, ±20°, and ±18° for horizontal, vertical, and torsional eye movements, respectively. Noise for the setup was estimated from the torsional recordings as 0.1° to 0.2°/s root mean square for torsional angle and 0.1° to 0.15°/s root mean square for torsional velocity. The digitized ASCII file output for horizontal vertical and torsional data was converted to software files (Spike 2; Cambridge Electronic Design, Cambridge, UK) for analysis. 
Each subject sat upright with the head stabilized on a chin rest placed 120 cm away from the stimulus. The height of each subject was adjusted so that the center of the stimulus and the subject’s eyes were at the same level. The cameras of the VOG system were adjusted while the subject viewed the stationary stimulus so that pupil size, threshold, and contrast levels could be set at the experimental conditions to ensure the highest quality of recording. 
A five-point calibration of each eye was performed monocularly, with the nonviewing eye covered before the testing procedure began. Subjects fixated points centrally, 15° in depression, right gaze, in elevation, and left gaze. Recordings were taken monocularly, and a cover was used for the nonviewing eye during testing. Torsional measurements were calibrated within the VOG setup, determined from rotations of the iris. The torsional angle was defined with reference to the initial image measured when the experiment was set up. 
The experimental stimulus was projected onto a 1.75 × 1.17-m rear projection screen using an LCD projector (resolution 1024 × 768 pixels; model EMP 703; Epson, Long Beach, CA). Stimuli were generated using a visual stimulus projector (VSG 2/5; Cambridge Research Systems, Rochester, UK) and consisted of a rotating sinusoidal grating pattern of 90° cycle size subtending 50.8° in diameter (Fig. 1) . The luminance of the grating pattern varied from 0.45 to 23.0 cd/m2, giving a luminance contrast of 96%. The stimulus revolved around its central axis at 40°/s and 400°/s in clockwise and counterclockwise directions. The subjects were asked to stare at the center of the stimulus keeping it in focus. Each stimulus was presented for 30 seconds, followed by a blank phase of 15 seconds during which the subject was asked to fixate a black screen. 
Four of the subjects who showed no responses at 40°/s and 400°/s (all men; aged 56, 59, 63, and 72 years) were tested across a wider range of stimulus velocities—20°/s, 100°/s, 200°/s, 800°/s, and 1000°/s—in both directions. 
Data Analysis
A section of the iris, the “signature segment,” was selected from a reference video frame to include significant landmarks in the iris from which luminance levels were measured. Torsional eye position was derived from angular displacement of the defined segment. To estimate the angular displacement, the luminance levels from subsequent video frames were cross-correlated for the corresponding segment with the original signature segment. The cross-correlation value also provided a measure of correspondence between the signature segment and the subsequent image that was used as an estimate of the quality of the recording. Only data that exceeded a torsional quality of 0.5 were used for analysis (a correlation close to 1 implied the best data quality). Poor-quality data could result from an iris segment without many landmarks and changing pupil size because of altered illumination, leading to changes in the position of landmarks. They could also result when the pupil was not accurately detected because of small size or interference from surrounding ocular structures (e.g., dark eyelashes, lashes with heavy eye makeup, droopy eye lids). Subjects were instructed to “open their eyes wide” when it was considered from online displays of the eyes that it was possible the eyelids were occluding the recording. A drop in torsional quality of the data in the blank phase, caused by changing pupil size, meant that measurement of torsional optokinetic afternystagmus was unreliable. Torsional quality of the data during measurement of tOKN did not significantly change with age in the study (linear regression: r 2 = 0.02, P = 0.2). 
Smoothed velocity traces of the torsional data were created using a simple five-point low-pass differentiator filter (linear). A velocity threshold of 10°/s was used as a default to determine saccades in the torsional recording. Because the level of noise varied, depending on the quality of the torsional recording, the velocity threshold could be adjusted manually (invariably reduced) to just exceed the noise level evident in the smoothed velocity trace. The mean velocity threshold selected across all subjects was 6.98°/s (SD 1.79°/s). This equates to discrimination of saccades of 0.55° or larger (determined from peak velocity/amplitude characteristics for the setup). For a mean beat frequency of 1.46 Hz (the mean of the subjects showing tOKN in this study), this approximated to a slow-phase velocity of 0.80°/s. 
For subjects classified as responders, the mean velocity over a minimum of 10 slow phases from each 30-second trace was used to give the mean slow-phase velocity (MSPV) for each stimulus. Because MSPVs were not normally distributed across the subjects, median values were used for analysis. Subjects classified as nonresponders had no slow phases within the 30-second analysis section. One subject was excluded from the original study group of 31 subjects because the quality trace of the torsional segment fluctuated during testing; hence, a constant noise-free recording was difficult to achieve. 
As monocular eye movements were recorded, the relative direction of rotation of the eye differed according to which eye was fixing (e.g., clockwise stimulation to the right eye produced a slow phase that extorted the right eye and intorted the left eye and vice versa). To avoid misinterpretation, the direction of the eye rotation and the velocity of the stimulus were defined in relation to intorsion/extorsion of the viewing monocular eye. 
Statistical Analysis
Changes in the proportion of nonfunctional tOKN with age were analyzed using logistic regression. The odds ratio in this analysis was used to estimate the risk of being a nonresponder with age. The correlation between MSPV and age in subjects with measurable responses was also analyzed using the simple linear regression (Pearson product-moment correlation coefficient). 
Results
Examples of original eye movement recordings are displayed in Figure 2showing monocular eye movement recordings from a 21-year-old subject (Fig. 2A) , a 58-year-old subject (Fig. 2B) , and a 72-year-old subject (Fig. 2C)at 400°/s stimulation in both rotational directions. The 21-year-old subject showed a clear tOKN response with slow phase in extorsion (Fig. 2Ai)and intorsion, (Fig. 2Aii) , directions displaying no real difference in response. The 58-year-old subject showed a diminished tOKN response in both rotation directions (Fig. 2B) . tOKN response was absent in the 72-year-old subject (Fig. 2C)
Figure 3shows the correlation between age and MSPV in the intorsion and extorsion directions of all subjects at stimulus velocities of 40°/s (Fig. 3A)and 400°/s (Fig. 3B) . The number of nonresponders (open circles for extorsion and crosses for intorsion) clearly increases with age. Consequently, logistic regression showed a significant change with age for stimulus rotations of 40°/s (P = 0.0029 [odds ratio, 0.91; 95% CI, 0.86–0.97] and P = 0.0023 [odds ratio, 0.90; 95% CI, 0.84–0.96] for extorsion and intorsion, respectively) and 400°/s (P = 0.0026 [odds ratio, 0.90; 95% CI, 0.84–0.96] and P = 0.108 [odds ratio, 0.76; 95% CI, 0.61–0.93] for extorsion and intorsion, respectively). For every 1-year increase in age, the risks were 9% and 10% of not responding to tOKN stimuli at 40°/s and 10.3% and 24.3% of not responding to tOKN stimuli at 400°/s in extorsion and intorsion directions, respectively. Within the age brackets of 19 to 40 years (n = 10; median age, 30; range, 19–36), 41 to 65 years (n = 10; median age, 52; range, 44–65), and older than 65 years (n = 10; median age, 68, range, 66–72), the number of responders (i.e., subjects who showed at least one response to stimuli in any direction of rotation at either stimulus velocity) were 10 of 10, 6 of 10, and 1 of 10, respectively. 
Simple linear regression was used to investigate change in mean slow-phase velocity with age in subjects who displayed a measurable response (i.e., excluding zero values). Results of this analysis were not significant for either the 40°/s (extorsion: r = 0.16, P = 0.6; intorsion, r = −0.050, P = 0.88) or the 400°/s (extorsion: r = −0.33, P = 0.23; intorsion: r = 0.34, P = 0.22) stimulation. 
Of the four older subjects (aged 56, 59, 63, and 72 years) tested at further stimulus velocities of 20°/s, 100°/s, 200°/s, 800°/s, and 1000°/s, the subjects aged 63 and 72 years demonstrated no detectable response when eye movement traces were analyzed across all stimulus velocities. The 56-year-old subject responded only at the stimulus velocity of 20°/s, in the counterclockwise direction, with an MSPV of 0.88°/s, and the 59-year-old subject responded at stimulus velocities of 200°/s in the clockwise direction and 20°/s in the counterclockwise direction, with MSPVs of 1.04°/s and 0.7°/s, respectively. 
Discussion
This study shows for the first time that tOKN is affected by aging. This effect was consistent over two stimulus velocities and occurred at approximately the same rate in extorsion or intorsion directions. 
In a prospective cross-sectional study (249 subjects; age range, 18 days-89 years), horizontal OKN mean gain has been shown to undergo a small but significant decline after age 50. 2 This is in agreement with other studies comparing younger subjects with older subjects; superior horizontal OKN gains were always observed in the younger subjects. 3 4 5 Similarly, vertical OKN was also reduced in healthy elderly subjects (mean age, 70 ± 8 years), who had lower tracking gain and greater phase lag than healthy young subjects (mean age, 30 ± 6 years). 7  
The contrasting feature between our results and previous studies on horizontal and vertical OKN, however, is that the tOKN response was virtually undetectable in elderly subjects (older than 65 years; median age, 68), with only 1 in 10 demonstrating a response. The lack of response in the older subjects was also consistent when other stimulus velocities were used to assess the tOKN response in four subjects who originally did not respond to stimuli rotating at 40°/s or 400°/s. The older subjects, aged 63 and 72 years, did not show any response to other stimulus velocities, whereas the two younger subjects, aged 56 and 59, showed minimal response to stimuli rotating at 20°/s and 200°/s. Although previous studies showed reduced horizontal and vertical OKN gain in the elderly, a response was still evident. 
Possible explanations for this could be that tOKN response has a very small gain (eye velocity in relation to stimulus velocity), with stimuli up to 200°/s yielding a maximum response of approximately 3°/s. 1 Stimulus velocities of 40°/s and 400°/s were considered suitable for use because these had previously elicited a good response that was easily differentiated from the normally occurring noise in the recording. 1 However, it is possible that tOKN responses are closer to threshold than horizontal and vertical OKN responses, making them more sensitive to aging. Another possibility is that tOKN responses are present in the elderly but fall below the level of system noise. In general, VOG yields larger signal-to-noise ratios than torsion for horizontal and vertical eye movement recordings. 
The tOKN response is essentially involuntary and not influenced by voluntary pursuit mechanisms. In general, we have limited capacity to make voluntary torsional eye movements in the primary position, though one early previous report by Balliet and Nakayama 11 in 1974 suggests that, with training, torsional pursuit can be generated voluntarily. The torsional “pursuit” they describe, however, does not involve the classic pursuit mechanism of tracking a single object of interest. Rather, it is driven by the alignment of static stimuli spanning the visual field. It is possible that tOKN is more prone to deterioration with age than horizontal and vertical OKN because pursuit cannot contribute to its generation. 
Although the pursuit mechanism has been shown to decrease with age along the horizontal and vertical meridians, 3 7 12 recent findings from a longitudinal study in healthy elderly persons (older than 75 years) 13 describes horizontal smooth pursuit gains at two different velocities as not greatly affected by aging. In comparison, horizontal OKN measures showed a gradual significant age-related decline over a 9-year follow-up period. It is possible that the rates of horizontal and vertical OKN decline could be slowed by contribution of the pursuit system; tOKN shows a sharper decline. 
The importance of higher cortical pathways compared with subcortical (retinogeniculate) pathways used for motion detection have been investigated. 6 Subjective measures of motion perception and objective measures of horizontal motion detection when viewing a random dot display were investigated in subjects from 19 to 92 years of age. Interestingly, the authors found an age-related linear decrease in objective OKN responses and a subjective increase in the motion perception thresholds; however, they did not find an association between the two factors, suggesting that aging affects the neural mechanisms behind motion perception and motion detection at different cortical and subcortical levels. 
The effects of aging on torsional eye movements generated through vestibular stimulation have also been described. Jahn et al. 14 measured torsional eye movements with VOG during stimulation of the vestibular nerve through galvanic vestibular stimulation (GVS) in 57 healthy subjects aged 20 to 69 years. They found that the magnitude of induced static ocular torsion and torsional nystagmus increased from the fourth to the sixth decade but decreased in the seventh decade. Listing’s plane, the axis of rotation that governs the torsional position of the eye at all gaze positions, 15 has also been examined with respect to age. It was found that torsional position was more variable in older subjects (i.e., Listing plane was thicker) than in younger subjects when the whole body was repositioned to different static roll-and-pitch positions. 16  
In conclusion, we have shown for the first time an age-related deterioration of the tOKN responses in healthy subjects between 19 and 72 years of age. The responses appeared to be virtually eliminated in subjects older than 65 years of age. 
 
Figure 1.
 
Experiment setup for recording tOKN.
Figure 1.
 
Experiment setup for recording tOKN.
Figure 2.
 
Original eye movement recordings of a 21-year-old subject (A), a 58-year-old subject (B), and a 72-year-old subject (C) at 400°/s stimulation. A downward slope indicated the eye making a slow phase in the extorsion direction, and an upward slope indicated an eye movement in the intorsion direction. tOKN was clearly seen in the 21-year-old man but was less obvious in the 58-year-old woman. Although some fluctuations in torsion were also seen in the 72-year-old man, tOKN was not apparent. Original data were smoothed using a five-point boxcar filter.
Figure 2.
 
Original eye movement recordings of a 21-year-old subject (A), a 58-year-old subject (B), and a 72-year-old subject (C) at 400°/s stimulation. A downward slope indicated the eye making a slow phase in the extorsion direction, and an upward slope indicated an eye movement in the intorsion direction. tOKN was clearly seen in the 21-year-old man but was less obvious in the 58-year-old woman. Although some fluctuations in torsion were also seen in the 72-year-old man, tOKN was not apparent. Original data were smoothed using a five-point boxcar filter.
Figure 3.
 
Scatterplots of mean slow-phase velocity in °/s compared with age in years at (A) 40°/s and (B) 400°/s stimulation. Legend: responders and nonresponders are distinguished. Gray dotted line: estimate of the level at which tOKN can be detected.
Figure 3.
 
Scatterplots of mean slow-phase velocity in °/s compared with age in years at (A) 40°/s and (B) 400°/s stimulation. Legend: responders and nonresponders are distinguished. Gray dotted line: estimate of the level at which tOKN can be detected.
FarooqSJ, ProudlockFA, GottlobI. Torsional optokinetic nystagmus: normal response characteristics. Br J Ophthalmol. 2004;88:796–802. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
ValmaggiaC, RutscheA, BaumannA, et al. Age related change of optokinetic nystagmus in healthy subjects: a study from infancy to senescence. Br J Ophthalmol. 2004;88:1577–1581. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
SimonsB, ButtnerU. The influence of age on optokinetic nystagmus. Eur Arch Psychiatry Neurol Sci. 1985;234:369–373. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
KatoI, IshikawaM, NakamuraT, et al. Quantitative assessment of influence of aging on optokinetic nystagmus. Acta Otolaryngol Suppl. 1994;511:99–103. [PubMed]
SpoonerJW, SakalaSM, BalohRW. Effect of aging on eye tracking. Arch Neurol. 1980;37:575–576. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
TranDB, SilvermanSE, ZimmermanK, FeldonSE. Age-related deterioration of motion perception and detection. Graefes Arch Clin Exp Ophthalmol. 1998;236:269–273. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
DemerJL. Effect of aging on vertical visual tracking and visual-vestibular interaction. J Vestib Res. 1994;4:355–370. [PubMed]
BonoF, OliveriRL, ZappiaM, AgugliaU, PuccioG, QuattroneA. Computerized analysis of eye movements as a function of age. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 1996;22:261–269.
KnoxPC, DavidsonJH, AndersonD. Age-related changes in smooth pursuit initiation. Exp Brain Res. 2005;165:1–7. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
ThiloKV, ProbstT, BronsteinAM, ItoY, GrestyMA. Torsional eye movements are facilitated during perception of self-motion. Exp Brain Res. 1999;126:495–500. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
BallietR, NakayamaK. Training of voluntary torsion. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1978;17:303–314. [PubMed]
HuamanAG, SharpeJA. Vertical saccades in senescence. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1993;34:2588–2595. [PubMed]
KerberKA, IshiyamaGP, BalohRW. A longitudinal study of oculomotor function in normal older people. Neurobiol Aging. 2006;27:1346–1353. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
JahnK, NaesslA, SchneiderE, StruppM, BrandtT, DieterichM. Inverse U-shaped curve for age dependency of torsional eye movement responses to galvanic vestibular stimulation. Brain. 2003;126:1579–1589. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
LeighRJ, ZeeDS. The neurology of eye movements. Contemporary Neurology Series: 70. 2006;13.Oxford University Press Oxford.
FurmanJM, SchorRH. Orientation of Listing’s plane during static tilt in young and older human subjects. Vision Res. 2003;43:67–76. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1.
 
Experiment setup for recording tOKN.
Figure 1.
 
Experiment setup for recording tOKN.
Figure 2.
 
Original eye movement recordings of a 21-year-old subject (A), a 58-year-old subject (B), and a 72-year-old subject (C) at 400°/s stimulation. A downward slope indicated the eye making a slow phase in the extorsion direction, and an upward slope indicated an eye movement in the intorsion direction. tOKN was clearly seen in the 21-year-old man but was less obvious in the 58-year-old woman. Although some fluctuations in torsion were also seen in the 72-year-old man, tOKN was not apparent. Original data were smoothed using a five-point boxcar filter.
Figure 2.
 
Original eye movement recordings of a 21-year-old subject (A), a 58-year-old subject (B), and a 72-year-old subject (C) at 400°/s stimulation. A downward slope indicated the eye making a slow phase in the extorsion direction, and an upward slope indicated an eye movement in the intorsion direction. tOKN was clearly seen in the 21-year-old man but was less obvious in the 58-year-old woman. Although some fluctuations in torsion were also seen in the 72-year-old man, tOKN was not apparent. Original data were smoothed using a five-point boxcar filter.
Figure 3.
 
Scatterplots of mean slow-phase velocity in °/s compared with age in years at (A) 40°/s and (B) 400°/s stimulation. Legend: responders and nonresponders are distinguished. Gray dotted line: estimate of the level at which tOKN can be detected.
Figure 3.
 
Scatterplots of mean slow-phase velocity in °/s compared with age in years at (A) 40°/s and (B) 400°/s stimulation. Legend: responders and nonresponders are distinguished. Gray dotted line: estimate of the level at which tOKN can be detected.
×
×

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

Sign in or purchase a subscription to access this content. ×

You must be signed into an individual account to use this feature.

×