Figure 5compares the group performance of the older control and glaucoma groups for the steady pedestal condition. A repeated-measures ANOVA (within factors: location, spatial frequency; between factor: group) showed that the glaucoma group performed significantly more poorly than the control subjects (F
(1,30) = 8.83,
P = 0.006). There was no significant three-way interaction between location, spatial frequency, and group (F
(6,180) = 1.44,
P = 0.20). The two-way interaction between location and group was significant (F
(2,60) = 6.10,
P = 0.004); however, the two-way interaction between spatial frequency and group was not (F
(3,90) = 0.57,
P = 0.63), implying that the difference between the groups depended on the location tested but not the spatial frequency. To analyze the performance at each visual field location, we performed a repeated-measures ANOVA for each location, with a Bonferroni-corrected
P = 0.016 being considered significant. The glaucoma group performed significantly worse than the control subjects in the area of abnormal visual field (F
(1,30) = 25.61,
P < 0.001), but not at the other two locations (fovea: F
(1,30) = 1.48;
P = 0.23; normal peripheral location: F
(1,30) = 3.18,
P = 0.09).
Figure 6compares the performance of glaucoma and older control groups for the pulsed-pedestal condition. A repeated-measures ANOVA demonstrated that the glaucoma group had significantly reduced sensitivity relative to control subjects (F
(1,30) = 9.96,
P = 0.004). There was no significant three-way interaction between location, group, and spatial frequency (F
(6,180) = 1.79,
P = 0.10). The two-way interaction between location and group was significant (F
(2,60) = 6.22,
P = 0.004), demonstrating that the difference between the groups varied, depending on the location. Inspection of
Figure 6shows the smallest difference between groups foveally and the largest difference in the midperipheral location with abnormal visual field sensitivity. There was no significant two-way interaction between spatial frequency and group (F
(3,90) = 1.73,
P = 0.17). The glaucoma group performed significantly worse (
P < 0.016 after Bonferroni correction) than control subjects in both peripheral areas of visual field (abnormal area: F
(1,30) = 12.15,
P = 0.002; “normal” area: F
(1,30) = 8.78,
P = 0.006) but not foveally (F
(1,30) = 2.62;
P = 0.12).
Effect sizes were determined, to compare across tasks the magnitude of the difference in performance between control and glaucoma groups
(Table 2) .
Figures 3d and 4dshow that the pulsed- and steady-pedestal tasks only clearly measure performance for separate mechanisms for the two lowest spatial frequencies (0.25 and 0.5 cyc/deg). Hence, to compare the magnitude of the effect within the presumed M and P pathways, we determined the average effect size for the 0.25 and 0.5 cyc/deg stimuli only and present these in
Table 2 . Although the interpretation of effect sizes is intended to be somewhat qualitative,
d = 0.5 is considered a medium effect size, and
d ≥ 0.8 is considered a large effect.
34 As expected,
Table 2demonstrates that the largest difference between the control and glaucoma groups was present at the location of abnormal visual field sensitivity. The effect size for the pulsed-pedestal task was more consistent in magnitude across the three locations tested. This could be interpreted as indicating that the pulsed task has some advantages in the detection of diffuse loss or in the detection of very early loss, as the magnitude of effect was moderate both in the fovea and in the area of normal peripheral field. In contrast, the steady task showed its greatest effect in the area of abnormal visual field, possibly implying that the task was better able to discriminate focal loss. This interpretation should be considered cautiously, as effect sizes are not intended to be strictly quantitatively compared and the sample size of the present study was relatively small.