Data were successfully collected from 103 of the 118 subjects who participated in the study (
Table 1). Data from 15 subjects were excluded because they had anisometropia >0.5 D in the vertical meridian or they failed to provide usable data in at least two anisometropia conditions. Of the remaining three criteria, individual responses were typically rejected because they failed to meet the criterion of correlation between gaze and stimulus position.
Two-factor ANOVA (viewing condition × age) of response frequency showed significant main effects of both viewing condition (
F (5,448) = 11.13;
P < 0.001) and age (
F (4,448) = 37.53;
P < 0.001) but no interaction (
F (20,448) = 1.19;
P = 0.26;
Fig. 2). A post hoc Games Howell test (with no assumption about equal variance in each group) for viewing condition showed that the response frequency for the binocular condition was statistically significantly larger than the −4 D anisometropia condition only (
P < 0.001). The four anisometropia conditions were not statistically significantly different from each other (
P > 0.7 for all conditions), indicating that a similar proportion of usable epochs was included in all anisometropia conditions. The monocular condition was statistically significantly different from the binocular, −2 D, and +4 D anisometropia conditions (
P < 0.01 for all conditions). A post hoc test for age indicated that the proportion of usable epochs included in the analyses gradually increased with age, with the response frequency in each age group significantly larger than all the preceding ones (
P < 0.005 for all comparisons).
Raw accommodative and vergence data from a representative 2.3-year-old subject for different viewing conditions is shown in
Figure 3. These raw data are representative of accommodative and vergence performance across all age groups. Visual inspection shows that the monocular accommodative and vergence responses were more attenuated than their binocular counterparts. When compared with the binocular condition, the accommodative and vergence responses reduced only marginally with different levels of induced anisometropia, with the size of the accommodative responses changing slightly more than those of the vergence responses. Typically, the eye without the lens (i.e., the effectively less ametropic eye) was more closely focused on the stimulus at all times, and the accommodative response in this eye appeared to track the stimulus. The fellow eye typically generated a consensual, accommodative response, resulting in chronic defocus of that eye's retinal image throughout the period of the experiment. Accommodative responses obtained using the PR were not calibrated for absolute accuracy in this study (they were calibrated only for their gain or slope as described earlier); therefore, quantitative analyses of the absolute states of accommodation in the two eyes were not performed.
The accommodative gain of the left eye (no lens) did not change dramatically as a function of the subjects' age or level of induced anisometropia (
Fig. 4). The main effect of viewing condition on the accommodative gain was statistically significant (
F (5,466) = 14.7;
P < 0.001), the main effect of age approached statistical significance (
F (4,466) = 2.3;
P = 0.06), and their interaction was not significant (
F (20,466) = 1.5;
P = 0.09). A post hoc Games Howell test for viewing condition indicated that the binocular accommodative gain was statistically significantly larger than the gains under −4 D (
P = 0.006), +2 D (
P = 0.001), and +4 D (
P = 0.02) anisometropia conditions. The four anisometropia conditions were not statistically significantly different from each other (
P > 0.14 for all conditions). The monocular accommodative gain was statistically significantly smaller than the gains under all other viewing conditions (
P < 0.001 for all conditions). These results indicate that the presence of induced anisometropia (2 D or more for myopic and 4 D for hyperopic anisometropia) significantly decreased the gain of accommodation from binocular conditions, but they all remained greater than the gains under monocular conditions.
Consensuality of accommodative gain in the right and left eyes was quantified in the binocular, +2 D, and −2 D anisometropia and monocular conditions. The +4 D and −4 D anisometropia conditions were not included in this analysis because the lens-induced shift in the raw accommodative data reached the limits of the PR's linear operating range in a number of subjects. Since the difference between left and right eye accommodative gain did not show any strong age-related trends, data from all ages were pooled for this analysis. Two-factor ANOVA (viewing condition × eye) of the accommodative gain showed a significant main effect of viewing condition (F (3,634) = 31.3; P < 0.001) but not of eye (F (1,634) = 2.26; P = 0.13). The interaction between viewing condition and eye was significant (F (3,634) = 3.88; P = 0.009), indicating that the difference between left and right eye accommodative gain was not uniform across the four viewing conditions. However, the mean (±95% confidence interval [CI]) difference in accommodative gain in the binocular, +2 D, and −2 D anisometropia and monocular conditions were −0.09 ± 0.35, 0.08 ± 0.41, −0.11 ± 0.42, and 0.08 ± 0.48, respectively, indicating that there was minimal bias in the data and that the difference across viewing conditions should have little clinical significance. Overall, these results indicate that accommodative changes in the right and left eyes were consensual under the binocular, monocular, and +2 D and −2 D anisometropia conditions. Visual inspection of the raw data indicated that the accommodative responses were also consensual in the +4 D and −4 D anisometropia conditions.
Like accommodation, the vergence gain also did not change dramatically as a function of the subject's age and the level of induced anisometropia (
Fig. 5). The main effect of viewing condition on the vergence gain was statistically significant (
F (5,466) = 51.9;
P < 0.001), whereas the main effect of age was not (
F (4,466) = 1.19;
P = 0.32). The interaction between viewing condition and age was only marginally significant (
F (20,466) = 1.66;
P = 0.04). A post hoc Games Howell test for viewing condition indicated that the binocular vergence gain was not statistically significantly different from the gains in any anisometropia condition (
P > 0.39 for all conditions) except for −4 D of anisometropia (
P < 0.001). The vergence gain in the −4 D anisometropia condition was statistically significantly smaller than the gain in the +2 D (
P = 0.004) and +4 D (
P = 0.05) anisometropia conditions but only approached significance in the −2 D anisometropia condition (
P = 0.07). The gains across the remaining three anisometropia conditions (−2 D, +2 D, and +4 D) were not statistically significantly different from each other (
P > 0.8 for all conditions). The monocular vergence gain was statistically significantly smaller than the vergence gains under all other viewing conditions (
P < 0.001 for all conditions). Some level of binocular fusion, therefore, appears to remain even with ±4 D of induced anisometropia, as indicated by the gains being statistically significantly higher than under monocular conditions.
Figure 6 plots the difference in accommodative and vergence gains between binocular and 11% induced aniseikonia conditions as function of the mean gain under the two viewing conditions for the 23 subjects (3.0 months–24.9 years) who participated in this condition (viewing was binocular; the 11% afocal magnifier was placed in front of the right eye). Mean (±SE) accommodative (binocular, 0.77 ± 0.03; aniseikonia, 0.80 ± 0.06) and vergence (binocular, 0.87 ± 0.04; aniseikonia, 0.85 ± 0.04) gains in the two viewing conditions were not statistically significantly different (accommodation,
P = 0.63; vergence,
P = 0.77). The difference in gains between the two viewing conditions was distributed around zero, with the mean difference in accommodative gain −0.03 (95% CI, ±0.24) and the mean difference in vergence gain 0.02 (95% CI, ±0.25). These results suggest that the aniseikonia induced by the 11% afocal magnifier did not significantly influence the gain of accommodation and vergence in typically developing infants and children.