The aim of our experiments was to assess the accuracy and dynamics of binocular eye movements toward memorized targets in terms of direction and depth (version and vergence). We found significant differences in horizontal gaze errors between the left and the right eye that were related to viewing direction. When the memorized target was located on the left side of the subject, such that the left eye had to make an abducting movement (temporal) and the right eye an adducting (nasal) movement toward the remembered target, gaze error of the right eye was larger than that of the left eye. When the target LED was placed the right side of the subject, the opposite was found; gaze errors were in this case significantly larger in the left (adducting) eye. This means that in orienting gaze toward memorized targets, the abducting eye is more accurate than the adducting eye. This also excludes the possibility that eye dominancy influenced the task, because in the control experiment, in which the same subjects participated, the abducting eyes remained most accurate. Because version (the average gaze of both eyes) is a commonly used measure to quantify directional gaze shifts, we also compared version with gaze of each eye separately. When version errors alone are used (shown in
Table 2), one may conclude that significantly larger errors are made in the “near” condition than in the “far” condition. However, gaze errors of each eye separately show that in both conditions, equally large errors are made. Thus, when version alone is used to analyze memory-guided saccades, important information on the performance of each eye separately might be missed.
We also compared memory-guided binocular gaze shifts that had either a convergence or divergence component. In one condition the initial point of fixation of both eyes was at far distance, thus requiring convergent eye movements toward the remembered location of the target LED; in contrast, in the other situation the initial point of fixation was at near distance, requiring divergent memory-guided eye movements. We found that under the condition that required divergence, the error in vergence was significantly smaller than when convergence was required. Adding additional visual depth cues during the flash had no effect on vergence errors. In both the “near” and “far” conditions, the remembered vergence angles toward the target LED were too small. One explanation is that the subject underestimated the target distance. However, this is contradicted by the fact that adding additional depth cues did not improve the vergence error. Another explanation might be that the eyes have a tendency to diverge because of a limitation in vergence control from memory. This suggests that passive release of vergence is an easier task than active increase of vergence. This is in line with the finding that without visual input the eyes have a physiological resting vergence position.
25 Because in our experiment gaze errors were unequal in the two eyes, with the abducting eye being more accurate, vergence errors were mainly present in the adducting eye.
To our knowledge this difference in accuracy of abducting and adducting memory-guided eye movements has not been described before. The differences in neural pathways that are involved in the control of abducting and adducting eye movements offer a possible explanation for this finding. Innervation of the lateral rectus muscle, which abducts the eye, is a pathway directly driven by the abducens nucleus and nerve (VI), whereas the medial rectus muscle (which adducts the eye) is indirectly controlled by the abducens nucleus via the medial longitudinal fasciculus (MLF) and the oculomotor nucleus (III). The abducting eye has small gaze errors, and its movement is also faster than that of the adducting eye. Differences in peak velocity were previously shown by Collewijn and colleagues using visually guided tasks.
26
The experimental design (with the target LED on the left side) was such that based on geometry, different-sized gaze shifts were required to fixate the target LED. When gaze shifts were made from the “far” fixation toward the target LED, the right (adducting) eye had to make a larger gaze shift than the left eye. However, in the “near” condition, the left eye (abducting eye) had to make a larger gaze shift than the right eye. Under visually guided conditions, this amplitude difference between the left and right eyes was achieved by generating unequal-sized saccades. For memory-guided saccades, the abducting eye was always faster than the adducting eye. Thus, under this condition, subjects had to use postsaccadic vergence, mediated by the adducting eye, to direct their gaze to the memorized position in 3-D space. Our data are consistent with the findings of Kumar et al.
21 Those authors, however, considered only peak saccade and peak vergence velocity ratios and did not analyze relative timing aspects. Our study shows that under visually guided conditions, the abducting eye was initially faster but that the adducting eye continued to increase its velocity, reaching its peak after the peak velocity of the abducting eye.
Our data show that different strategies are used to achieve 3-D gaze shifts under different conditions. An important issue is how our findings relate to the longstanding debate on the internal organization of binocular oculomotor control. The prevailing theory is Hering's law, which describes different vergence and conjugate commands that are summed by the motor neurons of each eye.
1 Helmholtz, however, postulated that each eye is individually controlled.
27 In support of Hering's theory, it has been shown that in the midbrain, different types of neurons are involved in the control of vergence.
6,28,29 These studies have demonstrated that vergence tonic cells, located in the mesencephalon, increase firing either before convergence or before divergence eye movements. It is considered that these vergence signals are added to conjugate saccadic motor commands. Thus, in this view, binocular saccadic eye movements are primarily controlled at the brainstem level by conjugate burst cells. However, more recently, monocular burst neurons encoding monocular commands for left and right eye saccades have been identified in the midbrain.
11 It has also been demonstrated that the brainstem burst generators, which were commonly assumed to drive only the conjugate component of eye movements, carry substantial vergence-related information during disconjugate saccades.
13 As pointed out by Kumar et al.,
21 changes in binocular behavior that are quantitatively different for saccades and vergence are inconsistent with the independent eye control theory. Such changes were found during convergence, but not during divergence, both in our study and in the study of Kumar et al.
21
Our findings suggest that the internal circuitry for making 3-D gaze shifts is dependent on continuous visual and local feedback when making intrasaccadic convergence eye movements during binocular gaze shifts. This does not exclude the possibility that there is also a memory system that keeps track of where a target is in 3-D space and that can be accessed by the oculomotor control system: Otherwise subjects could not make memory-guided binocular gaze shifts at all. A possible candidate is the caudal frontal eye field.
30,31 However, to use this information, the oculomotor system has to rely more on a sequential processing using fast and slow systems than on a simultaneous processing of saccade and vergence commands. The fact that during divergence the situation is different may very well reflect that for near vision there is already a state of tonic vergence. This is readily released at an early stage of the binocular gaze shift.