When measuring visual function in glaucoma, the clinician is measuring the visual consequences of retinal GC loss. PGRA and DLS are two such methods.
10,27 However, tests of PGRA and DLS determine GC function with very different stimuli and test paradigms. It is, therefore, of interest to compare local estimates of GC density (per millimeter squared) from the two functional tests and relate these to RNFL thickness. This approach may also allow for comparison between functional tests, independent of measurement scale and the dynamic range of the instrument. Appreciably, these estimates of GC density may be influenced by any factor that causes the functional measure to be variable. In the current study, the area and presentation duration of the two stimuli, as well as the response criteria, differ greatly. The degree to which the visual system responds to a stimulus depends on the extent of retina covered by the stimulus. The stimulus is firstly sampled by the photoreceptors and GCs at the retinal level and further processed by second stage spatial filters.
10,16 In order to obtain optimal resolution performance, the grating should contain at least 5 to 6 cycles.
46 The extent of the PGRA stimulus in the current study was, therefore, much larger than that of the conventional Goldmann III stimulus used in SAP (0.43° diameter). It has been argued in previous literature that, on one hand, a larger stimulus (whatever its form) pools responses from neighboring retinal areas and yields more information about the functional integrity of densely defective regions in advanced glaucoma.
3,47,48 On the other hand, it has been argued that smaller stimuli are preferred in the detection of highly localized damage, as one can detect smaller localized areas of functional loss without intrusion by otherwise healthy retina.
3,47 While it might be that our stimuli measure different attributes of visual function, a similar argument exists here. It is entirely possible that a large stimulus might recruit more neighboring
damaged areas than the smaller Goldmann III, especially when one considers the relatively wide spacing between targeted retinal areas in conventional perimetry.
49 In such a case, PGRA would sample a larger area of retina and may signal a dropout of GCs before conventional measures of DLS. Conversely, the smaller DLS stimulus might allow identification of more specific localized defects, while resolution of the larger grating stimulus may be less vulnerable to heterogeneous (patchy) loss. In such a scenario, although local aliasing might occur within the grating stimulus at an area of heterogeneous loss, a more global veridical percept could mask this; a phenomenon known as supra-Nyquist acuity.
50 Thus, subtle localized damage may well be missed by PGRA and estimates of GC density may be somewhat overestimated with respect to those estimated using DLS. Also worth considering is that if ganglion cell density declines such that the stimulus window contains fewer than 5 to 6 cycles, GC density may increasingly become underestimated.
46 In the current study we find that GC density estimated with DLS declines at a slightly greater rate than that estimated with PGRA, for a given rate of decline in RNFL thickness.