Fusional vergence is necessary to perceive a 3D image on a 2D display such as a TV or a computer monitor. When one sees a 3D image with crossed disparity, convergence occurs to achieve binocular fusion, and the image is perceived to be in front of the monitor. However, when the human eyes converge, accommodation and miosis should occur to achieve synkinetic eye movement.
9 Such accommodation is not necessary to appreciate 3D images because the practical distance between the eye and the display plane would not be affected whether the image is perceived in front of or behind the plane. Thus, theoretically more accommodation than is necessary occurs while viewing a 3D image with crossed disparity than when viewing a 2D image. This conflict between accommodation and vergence has been suggested as the cause of 3D image-induced eye fatigue.
3,4 Although the exact mechanism of myopia and its progression has not been fully understood, the possibility that such unnecessary accommodation may induce and/or promote myopia cannot be excluded since nearwork and accommodation are reported as important environmentally based factors in the development and progression of myopia.
7,10,11 NITM is one of many possible environmentally based factors contributing to nearwork-induced myopigenesis.
7,12 Ciuffreda and Vasudevan
7 reported converging evidence from clinical, laboratory, and modeling studies that reveal the relationship between permanent myopia and NITM. They also mentioned that NITM might be a factor in the development of permanent myopia rather than simply reflecting an accommodative abnormality.
7
Our results indicate that viewing a 3D image induces a greater degree of NITM than viewing a 2D image. Some subjects showed a hyperopic change after watching the 2D movie, whereas no one showed it after watching the 3D movie. Such hyperopic change after watching a 2D movie implies that viewing 2D images in a conventional manner at a viewing distance of 50 to 70 cm would not necessarily induce NITM. These findings concur that viewing a 3D image may induce greater NITM. If greater NITM is related to the development and progression of myopia, it can be postulated that viewing in 3D rather than in 2D may cause increased myopia. Moreover, there was also a tendency for the NITM to persist longer after viewing 3D images because the initial degree of NITM in 3D images was greater than that of 2D images. It was reported that prolonged NITM can affect the development and progression of myopia.
7,13 However, the degree of NITM after watching the 3D movies differed according to the image disparity in the present study. Reading the 3D text with crossed disparity induced more NITM than that with uncrossed disparity. The results of reading the 3D text with uncrossed disparity were not different from the results of viewing the 2D image. When one sees a 3D image with uncrossed disparity, fusional divergence occurs instead of fusional convergence. Viewing a 3D image with uncrossed disparity does not induce a greater degree of NITM because fusional divergence does not accompany unnecessary accommodation. However, the results were taken at an intermediate viewing distance. The outcome with uncrossed disparity could be considerably different at a nearer (e.g., 33 cm) viewing distance.
We also can postulate that, if viewing a 3D image with crossed disparity induces greater NITM and accommodation, then the conflict between accommodation and vergence would increase. This situation would subsequently cause more eye fatigue than viewing a 3D image with uncrossed disparity. It is conceivable that, when producing 3D movies, crossed disparity should be restricted to some degree. Images with uncrossed disparity would be preferred with regard to ocular comfort and myopia. For example, the 3D image with a background that appears to be located behind the screen would be better than the 3D image with characters that are perceived to be located in front of the screen. There needs to be an adequate period of visual rest between 3D image viewings to reduce prolonged unnecessary accommodation and to prevent NITM. These suggestions should increase the safety of 3D image and movie viewing.
There are some limitations in our study. The number of enrolled subjects was small, and the age groups did not include the elderly and children. We cannot exclude the possibility that the elderly with presbyopia and naturally decreased accommodative power would respond differently to 3D vision. Including children as subjects would also have affected the responses. The ages of refractive development and myopia onset are younger than the ages of our participant age group. Another limitation was that our study did not include emmetropic and hyperopic populations. The degree of NITM might be different in these groups. A larger study including various age groups with a broad range of refractive error is necessary to expand our findings. We cannot conclude that watching 3D images induces myopia and its progression. However, before viewing in 3D becomes more popular, its relationship with myopia development needs to be evaluated further because prolonged exposure to 3D images may affect the development of the visual system in children. Our study merely provides an assumption that needs further study. A longitudinal, observational study of children who are in their emmetropization years will provide more informative data regarding the relationship between 3D viewing and myopic progression.
In conclusion, viewing 3D images induces greater NITM than viewing 2D images. Viewing a 3D image with crossed disparity resulted in a greater degree of NITM than viewing a 3D image with uncrossed disparity. Further studies are warranted to elucidate whether NITM induced by viewing a 3D image is actually related to myopic progression.