To our knowledge, this study provides the first population-based data on the prevalence and distribution of refractive errors in elderly Asians. Myopia is particularly prevalent in East Asia, especially among the Chinese.
24 Most studies in Asia have been conducted in relatively young populations
7 9 10 11 or have included only a small proportion of the elderly.
8 12 The present study provides an opportunity to compare the prevalence of myopia and other refractive errors with other ethnic populations in similarly aged elderly groups.
Figure 4 presents the prevalence of myopia and hyperopia in selected population-based surveys. For the purpose of comparison, only data from similarly aged elderly groups are shown.
12 14 15 25 26 27 In the present study, the prevalence of myopia (SE < −0.5 D) was 19.4%, which is lower than that reported in similar age groups in Tanjong Pagar, Singapore
12 and in Barbados, West Indies.
25 By contrast, the prevalence of myopia in our Chinese population is slightly higher than that in similarly aged elderly white populations: 14.7% in the Beaver Dam Eye Study,
15 11.1% in the Blue Mountains Eye Study,
27 and 17.9% in the white group in the Baltimore Eye Survey.
14 Despite the lower prevalence of myopia in blacks than in whites and Asians in previous studies,
14 24 the prevalence of myopia is fairly high in the elderly black group in the Barbados Eye Study
(Fig. 4) . Because lens nuclear opacity is significantly associated with myopia
12 25 26 and subjects who had undergone cataract surgery were usually excluded from the analysis in the studies of refractive error, the lower frequency (3%) of cataract surgery and the higher prevalence (41%) of lens opacity in the black participants in Barbados,
28 compared with other ethnic populations, may account for the high prevalence of myopia.
The present study supports the theory of cohort effects in the age-related trends in prevalence of myopia. First, the prevalence in this elderly Chinese population is much lower than that previously reported in younger generations in Taiwan
11 and other Asian countries.
7 29 30 The prevalence of myopia (defined as SE < −0.25 D) among schoolchildren aged 16 to 18 years in Taiwan was as high as 84%,
11 which is much higher than a recalculated prevalence of 22% in the present study, using the same definition. It is unlikely that such a great difference could be explained by an intrinsic decrease in the amount of an individual’s myopia as part of the aging process.
31 Changing environmental factors, most notably increased near-work activity and stringent educational studies in the past two decades,
11 may account in part for the increasing prevalence of myopia among younger generations of Chinese people. Although our data suggest that there is only a weak association between myopia and educational level, this may reflect lower access to education or less near-work demands in the past for this elderly population. Second, the prevalence of myopia in this elderly Chinese population is not much higher than in similarly aged elderly white populations
(Fig. 4) ,
14 15 27 compared with a much greater difference in myopia rates among younger Chinese versus similar white age groups.
6 24 The attenuated ethnic difference in the prevalence of myopia in the elderly again supports a greater contribution of recent environmental factors explaining the higher prevalence of myopia in younger generations of Chinese people.
Changes in refractive error with age are noteworthy
(Fig. 4) . Data from cross-sectional studies have shown that after 40 years of age, older persons tend to have lower rates of myopia and higher rates of hyperopia, than do younger persons.
12 14 15 26 27 This trend is referred to as the hyperopic shift.
32 33 34 However, after 60 or 70 years of age, as depicted in
Figure 4 , the hyperopic shift seems less prominent.
14 15 26 27 Some studies (such as the Barbados Eye Study
25 and the Tanjong Pagar Survey
12 ) and ours even show a reverse trend—that is, an increasing prevalence of myopia and a decreasing prevalence of hyperopia with advancing age in the elderly groups. It has been suggested that axial lengths and vitreous chamber depths are the most important predictors of refraction in adults.
34 35 Shorter axial lengths and vitreous chamber depths in older than in younger adults may explain the observed hyperopic shift. In the elderly, lens nuclear opacity becomes an additional significant predictor of refractive error.
12 25 26 33 36 This is consistent with our findings that the degree of nuclear opacity was positively associated with the prevalence of myopia and inversely associated with the prevalence of hyperopia. Changes in the refractive index of the lens substantially influence the shift of refraction. Thus, denser nuclear opacity in the elderly may drive the refractive error in the minus direction, which makes the hyperopic shift less evident. This is supported by data from the longitudinal Beaver Dam Eye Study,
37 which showed that after a 10-year period, younger adults became more hyperopic, whereas older adults and elderly people became more myopic, and much of this may have been related to increasing nuclear opacity.
Adult Chinese appear to have a greater prevalence of high myopia than whites.
12 The prevalence was 2.3% in our elderly Chinese population (SE < −6.0 D), and 4.7% in Singapore Chinese aged 60 years and more (SE < −5.0 D).
12 By contrast, the prevalence was only 0.87% in whites aged 60 years and more in the Baltimore Eye Survey.
14 The prevalence of high myopia is an important concern, because the consequences of high myopia, such as macular degeneration, glaucoma, and cataract
6 36 may contribute significantly to visual impairment. In a previous study, we found that high myopia macular degeneration contributes to 25% of visual impairment in adult Chinese.
38
In our study, women had a higher prevalence of hyperopia than men, a finding similar to those in other reports.
8 12 25 27 This may be because women’s eyes have a shorter axial length and shallower anterior chamber depth than those of men,
34 and hence a higher probability of being hyperopic. Women’s eyes tended to have steeper corneas than those of men in the present study (data not shown), which also has been shown by others.
34 39 Nevertheless, this cannot fully offset the effect of the shorter axial lengths on hyperopia. Our subsequent analysis shows that after adjustment for corneal curvature, women still have a 1.5-fold (95% CI: 1.2–1.9) higher risk of hyperopia than men.
There are few population-based data available on the prevalence of astigmatism in the elderly. In the present study, almost 75% of the subjects had astigmatism (cylinder < −0.5 D). The prevalence of astigmatism in the Chinese population in the Tanjong Pagar Survey was 51% for the 60- to 69-year age group and 68% for the 70- to 79-year age group.
12 The prevalence in other ethnic populations is relatively lower. The Baltimore Eye Survey found rates of astigmatism below 49% and 39% in whites and blacks, respectively, in those aged 60 and more.
14 Chinese appear to have a higher prevalence of astigmatism than other populations, but this must be confirmed.
Anisometropia (SE difference > 1.0 D) was present in 21.8% of the participants in the present study. This is close to the 26% rate among those aged 60 and more in the Tanjong Pagar Survey.
12 The prevalence reported from the Baltimore Eye Survey in similarly aged elderly groups was lower, ranging from 5.5% to 18.1% (with variation by gender and race).
14 Similar to these and other studies,
12 14 40 we observed patterns of higher rates of anisometropia in the older age groups.
We found no significant difference in refractive error between people with and without diabetes. The finding is consistent with the Beaver Dam Eye Study.
15 As expected, other factors, such as hypertension, smoking, and alcohol intake, did not significantly affect the prevalence of refractive error.
Population-based studies in the elderly are usually limited by a low response rate. Elderly people usually have a higher frequency of morbidity and disability that hinders travel and motivation to participate. The response rate in the present study was 66.6%. This compares favorably with 65% in the Salisbury Eye Evaluation Study,
16 17 which targeted the same age group (≥65 years) and was better than in other studies of this age group that include lengthy clinical examinations—for example, the Cardiovascular Health Study had a 55% response rate.
41 However, there are still limitations to the present study. First, we acknowledge that nonparticipants tended more often to be older and female
(Table 1) , and this may bias our estimates. Because the prevalence of myopia, astigmatism, and anisometropia increases with age, underrepresentation of older people may result in underestimation of the crude prevalence. For hyperopia, we found that older age and female gender affected the prevalence in opposite directions. Thus, we cannot know exactly whether nonparticipants are more or less likely than participants to have hyperopia. Second, we did not collect data on income level and other socioeconomic factors, such as occupation, which might be important potential confounders of the association with refractive error.
In summary, this study provides epidemiologic data on refractive errors in an elderly Chinese population in Taiwan. This elderly population had a much lower prevalence of myopia than the younger generations of Chinese in Taiwan. However, the prevalence was not much higher than in similarly aged white populations, compared with a greater difference between younger Chinese and white people. This suggests that changing environmental factors—most notably, increasing education and near-work demands—account for the increased prevalence of myopia in recent birth cohorts of Chinese. In contrast, the shift toward myopia in the elderly was mainly related to increasing lens nuclear opacity.