Thirteen elderly subjects, eight male and five female (mean age, 70.76 ± 4.14 [SD] years) were recruited from a group of volunteer patients who attend the University of Bradford Eye Clinic for teaching purposes. The tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki were observed, and the study gained approval from the University ethics committee. Informed consent was obtained from the participants after the nature of the study had been fully explained. An assessment was made to ensure that all subjects had no history of falls. For this study, a fall was defined as falling all the way to the floor or ground, falling and hitting an object such as a chair or stair, or falling from one level to another, for example from bed to the ground.
29 Subjects were also screened using a self-report health questionnaire. Those with cardiac arrhythmias, vestibular disturbances, diabetes, or severe arthritic conditions and medications affecting balance were excluded. Scores on the Lawton Activities of Daily Living (ADL) questionnaire
30 were high, with all subjects scoring a maximum 16 points. This indicated that the subjects were all independently mobile.
Measurements of visual acuity (VA) and ocular screening using slit-lamp biomicroscopy, tonometry, indirect ophthalmoscopy, and central visual field were undertaken. To ensure that vision loss was entirely due to refractive blur or the cataract simulation, subjects with a history of amblyopia, strabismus, eye disease, or ocular surgery; binocular VA less than 0.0 logarithm of the minimum angle of resolution (logMAR; Snellen equivalent ∼20/20); and/or any visible ocular disease were excluded. A subjective refraction was performed to obtain the subject’s optimal refractive correction at 4 m. Binocular visual function was subsequently assessed by VA and CS measurements. Binocular VA was measured (mean VA −0.07 ± 0.03 logMAR; Snellen equivalent ∼20/15) with the Early Treatment Diabetic Retinopathy Study (ETDRS) logMAR chart, with by-letter scoring, chart luminance of 160 cd/m
2 and a 4-m working distance. Binocular CS was measured (mean 1.68 ± 0.08 log CS) with the Pelli-Robson chart at 1 m, with by-letter scoring and a chart luminance of 200 cd/m
2. Binocular VA and CS were subsequently remeasured with additional binocular blur lenses of +1, +2, +4, and +8 DS and a cataract simulation
31 (light-scattering goggles; Vistech Consultants Inc., Dayton, OH) in a randomized order. The cataract simulation used in this study has been shown to mimic the wide angle (between 5° and 20°) light-scattering properties of cataract, in that it scatters light proportional to the inverse of the glare angle.
31 In addition, this cataract simulation was chosen because it has been shown to produce greater effects on Pelli-Robson CS than VA,
31 which is the opposite of refractive blur, which has a greater effect on VA.
25 By comparing postural stability changes with refractive blur and the cataract simulation, we intended to determine whether increases in postural instability are driven by reduction in CS or VA or in both.
Postural stability measurements were determined while subjects stood stationary on two adjacent force platforms (OR6-7; Advanced Medical Technology Inc., Boston, MA) mounted flush with the floor. Outputs from each of the force plates were combined to derive displacements of a global center of pressure (COP) in the anterior–posterior (A-P) and medial–lateral (M-L) directions.
Fluctuations in the displacement of the CP signal were quantified using the root mean square (RMS) of the amplitude. These fluctuations reflect the response of the central nervous system (CNS) to displacements of the center of mass.
32 33 The subjects were asked to stand still on the force plate for 30-second periods with their arms by their sides and one foot on each of the adjacent force platforms placed at a distance one tenth of the subject’s height apart, and the long axis of each foot was externally rotated by 15°.
34 To ensure that this stance position was maintained throughout the test procedure, a template was made for each subject according to height and the length of the foot, and placed over the force platform during each trial. Having the subject’s feet placed on two separate platforms allowed the vertical forces exerted by each limb and A-P and M-L force moments to be obtained to assess LLA,
28 which was determined as the ratio of the average (over the 30-second period) body weight placed on the more loaded limb to that on the less loaded limb
\[\mathrm{LLA}\ {=}\ \frac{\mathrm{average\ weight\ on\ the\ more\ loaded\ limb}}{\mathrm{average\ weight\ on\ the\ less\ loaded\ limb}}\]
According to this definition, an LLA of 1.0 would denote perfect symmetry.
Subjects were asked to keep looking at the middle of one of four visual targets, which consisted of a horizontal and vertical square-wave pattern.
10 23 Two patterns had a fundamental spatial frequency of 2.5 cyc/deg and two had a fundamental spatial frequency of 8 cyc/deg. The targets either had a Weber contrast of approximately 25%, which we assumed to be representative of contrast levels typically found in a home environment, or a Weber contrast of approximately 95%, which is representative of high-contrast black-on-white targets. Each of the targets covered an area of 1.1 m
2 and had a viewing distance of 1 m. The targets were adjusted for height for each subject so that its center was at eye level. Viewing was binocular, and vision in each subject was corrected with the optimal 4-m refractive correction and a 0.75-DS working-distance lens with full-aperture lenses in a trial frame at a distance of 1 m.
Standing postural stability and LLA were measured under two conditions: first, normal (bare platform) standing, and, second, standing on a 1.8-cm-thick dual-density polyurethane surface (1 cm at 270 kg/m3 and 0.8 cm at 430 kg/m3). The high-density polyurethane layer prevents localized compression under the typical areas of contact (metatarsal, malleolus, and hallucis) during upright standing, and hence maintains the compliant nature of the surface throughout the experimental procedure. The compliant nature of the foam makes it difficult for the kinesthetic system to provide accurate body orientation information in relation to the ground, and this disrupts somatosensory system inputs.
Subjects attended a familiarization session that involved their standing on the foam surface so that they could become familiar with standing with somatosensory input disrupted. Subjects were also exposed to the various visual conditions. Under each of the surface test conditions, standing balance and LLA were measured with the optimal refractive correction for the 1-m working distance and under six blur conditions for each of the four visual targets. The blur conditions included binocular dioptric blur levels of 0, +1, +2, +4 and +8 D and with diffusive blur using the cataract-simulating goggles.
31 35 In addition, standing balance and LLA were measured during normal standing and during standing on the foam surface with eyes closed. The order of the standing, visual, and target conditions were completed in a randomized order, and subjects were given a rest period of 1 minute (in which they could be seated) between each 30-second trial period.
Changes between conditions in the COP RMS were analyzed with a generalized estimating equation (GEE) population-averaged model that accounted for the correlation of readings within subjects (Stata, ver.7.0 statistical program; Stata Corp., College Station, TX). An exchangeable correlation structure was judged to be appropriate, given the experimental design. The terms in the model are:
-
A-P/M-L, a fixed factor with two levels: A-P and M-L directions of stability
-
Sensory disruption, a fixed factor with the two levels described earlier
-
Blur, a fixed factor with six levels: eyes open with no blur and 1-, 2-, 4-, and 8-D blur and cataract simulation
-
Spatial frequency, a fixed factor with two levels: high (8 cyc/deg) and low (2 cyc/deg)
-
Contrast, a fixed factor with two levels: high (Weber contrast 95%) and low (Weber contrast 25%)
The interactions of blur and sensory disruption and of blur and A-P/M-L were also included in the model.
For each of the standing and target conditions, differences between LLA measures in the eyes-open condition and each level of refractive blur, the cataract simulation and the eyes closed condition, were assessed by means of analysis of variance (ANOVA).