We report three principal results: First, even 10 minutes of wearing positive, but not negative, lenses resulted in a thickening of the choroids over the subsequent 1 to 2 hours. Second, wearing negative lenses for 1 hour caused choroidal thinning. Third, an hour of wearing positive lenses reduced the amount of ocular elongation over the next 2 days.
For an eye to infer the sign of the defocus it experiences by a trial-and-error procedure, its refractive error must change by an amount greater than its depth of focus during the visual episode, so that it can judge whether its current direction of growth (toward myopia or toward hyperopia) is the correct one to reduce the defocus. We found that a 10-minute period of wearing positive lenses led to the vitreous chamber’s becoming shallower by 18 μm at 30 minutes after the start of lens wear and by 32 μm at its peak change at 1 hour (data from experiment 2). The first of these would correspond to 0.3 D of hyperopic shift and the second to 0.5 D, on the basis that 1 mm of vitreous chamber depth corresponds to 17.5 D, according to the formula in Wallman et al.,
19 using the mean ocular length of 9.0 mm measured in these experiments. The depth of focus is estimated as at least 0.7 D in chickens of the age used in the present study, calculated from the retinal ganglion cell spacing, eye size, and pupil diameter.
20 This seems to be the lowest estimate of the depth of focus, in that it includes all the ganglion cells, whereas any particular retinal output would use a subset of the ganglion cells, which would therefore have a greater depth of focus. However, if the retinal circuitry used by the emmetropization mechanism constituted a subpopulation of bipolar or amacrine cells, the depth of focus could not be estimated without knowing which cells are involved, but it might well be less than 0.7 D. Finally, if we roughly estimate the “acuity” of the emmetropization process by averaging the spatial frequencies of stimuli that prevent form-deprivation myopia and those that do not, a greater depth of focus of 1.4 D is predicted.
20 21
It seems from our measurements that the amount of change in refractive status during 10 minutes of lens wear would probably be substantially less than 0.3 D and thus would be unlikely to be detectable by the chick eye. The amount of change during 1 hour of lens wear may be 0.5 D, which is still below the lowest estimate of the depth of focus. Finally, our finding of significantly increased choroidal thickness in 88% of eyes wearing positive lenses for 10 minutes must not simply reflect a bias toward choroidal thickening occurring whenever blur increases, because it did not occur in those eyes wearing negative lenses for 10 minutes. Thus, it seems highly unlikely that the chick eye can use the miniscule changes in retinal position during 10 minutes of lens wear to infer whether it is growing in the appropriate direction.
It has been argued that the eye could emmetropize by trial and error, even without remembering the degree of blur if it had access to the rate of change of blur or of image-degradation to guide eye growth.
22 We are skeptical of the applicability of this hypothesis to our results for two reasons. First, it would seem to require an unreasonably high sensitivity to the rate of change of blur to respond to the change in blur caused by eye growth or choroidal expansion—at most, 0.01 D/min—according to the calculations we have just described. Second, one would expect that imposing a spectacle lens would change the magnitude of defocus much more than anything the eye could do in the 10 minutes of lens wear. Thus, when we suddenly increased the amount of blur, either by fitting positive lenses to chicks restrained in the center of a drum (because the walls were beyond the far point of the lens-wearing eye) or by fitting negative lenses to birds in a cage with most objects nearby, we should have caused the initial response to be in the same direction; instead we got responses in opposite directions.
Beyond the question of whether eyes can discern the sign of the blur, our results point out two additional differences between wearing positive and negative lenses: The positive lenses are more potent than the negative ones, in that they require less lens wear to cause changes in choroidal thickness
(Fig. 1) , and the effect of one episode of wearing positive lenses is more enduring than one of wearing negative lenses
(Fig. 3b) . These results are consistent with the findings reported in three recent papers. Winawer and Wallman
13 found that if birds wear positive and negative lenses alternately for 30 minutes each, four times a day, the choroids thicken, showing that the myopic defocus of the positive lenses dominates. This thickening occurs irrespective of whether the sign of defocus alternates every 6 seconds, 75 seconds, or 15 minutes.
23 Furthermore, Zhu et al.
14 found that 2 minutes of wearing positive lenses four times a day while wearing negative lenses the rest of the time causes a significant increase in choroidal thickness relative to that of the untreated fellow eye. All of these differences between the effects of positive and negative lens wear may be part of a conservative growth strategy, in that an excessive ocular elongation in response to hyperopic defocus could leave the eye permanently too long, whereas excessive inhibition in response to myopic defocus could be subsequently corrected.
We found that wearing a positive lens for 1 hour, but not 10 minutes, caused a significant change in ocular elongation when measured 2 days later. Might one argue that the scleral response, which determines the elongation rate of the eye, still operates by trial and error, even though the choroidal response does not? We cannot rule this possibility out entirely, because the vitreous chamber depth changed by the equivalent of 0.5 D during the 1 hour of lens wear. However, there was no change in ocular elongation during this period. Therefore, even if the scleral response relies on visual feedback, the feedback cannot come from changes in ocular elongation; rather, a scleral trial-and-error mechanism would have to rely on the visual consequences of the choroidal response, a mechanism that itself apparently does not operate by trial and error. Moreover, we find, on the one hand, that among those eyes that elongated less than their fellow eyes over the 2 days after wearing positive lenses for 1 hour, there is a modest correlation (
r = 0.6) between the degrees of ocular elongation and choroidal thickening at 2 hours, suggesting that both responses may be coupled. However, there is substantial evidence of a dissociation between visual effects on these two ocular parameters.
13 16 It thus seems parsimonious to conclude tentatively that neither of these two components of lens compensation operates by a trial-and-error mechanism. In lens compensation, and presumably also in emmetropization, the eye appears to know which way to grow.
This conclusion adds weight to previous evidence that it is not the blurring of vision, per se, that is important in guiding eye growth, but whether blur is myopic or hyperopic. Furthermore, short periods of myopic blur are as effective as longer periods of hyperopic blur.
13 14 23 Given that the temporal properties of the emmetropizing mechanism appear similar across very different species,
24 it seems prudent to suppose that the progression of myopia in children may be influenced by the temporal characteristics of the child’s visual experiences. If long periods of mild hyperopic defocus during reading increase myopia, brief doses of clear vision or myopic defocus when one looks up from reading may signal the eye to reduce the progression of myopia.