Abstract
purpose. To compare the distribution of spherical equivalent refraction (SER) and other ocular parameters and to assess the contribution from oculometric parameters to SER in two age-specific, cross-sectional samples of children, and in two ethnic groups (European Caucasian and East Asian).
methods. A random-cluster design was used to recruit predominantly 6-year-old (1765 participants, 78.9% response) and 12-year-old children (2353 participants, 75.3% response) from schools across Sydney, Australia. Data collection included questionnaires and eye examination (keratometry, biometry, and cycloplegic autorefraction). Results of three analytical methods (Pearson correlation, partial correlation coefficient, and linear regression analyses) are reported for 6- and 12-year-old children.
results. Kurtosis for SER and axial length (AL) in the 12-year-old children (14.3 and 2.1, respectively) was similar to that previously reported for the 6-year-old children (11.3 and 0.5). AL showed high correlation (r) with SER in the 6- (r = −0.44) and 12-year-old (r = −0.61) children. Lower correlations for SER with corneal radius (r ≤ 0.09) and with lens power (r ≤ 0.13) were noted in both samples. In multivariate models, AL accounted for 24% and 49% of the variations in SER for the 6- and 12-year-old children, respectively. In these older children, correlations between AL and SER were greater in the East-Asian ethnic group (r = −0.79 vs. −0.47), and multivariate analyses showed that AL accounted for a greater proportion of the variation in refraction in East-Asian children (68%) than in European-Caucasian children (24%).
conclusions. In the samples of predominantly 6- and 12-year-old children, the main determinant of SER was AL. The greater contribution of AL in the older sample and in East-Asian children corresponds well with recently proposed models of continuing axial elongation in the absence of compensatory lens changes.
Since the classic studies of ocular biometry and the relationship with spherical equivalent refraction (SER) were reported,
1 2 3 4 it has been widely accepted that the age-related myopic shift in refraction in school children is due to axial elongation. In initial studies, Sorsby et al.
2 4 reported ambiguous findings for corneal power; however, recent studies using more precise instrumentation and detailed analytical methods have established that after the first 2 years of life, corneal power is relatively stable throughout development, whereas axial length (AL) continues to increase, with a corresponding shift in refraction in the myopic direction.
5 6 7 8 There is also a gradual reduction in lens power with age.
5 9 10
Other studies have shown that there is an early phase of refractive development spanning approximately the first 2 years of life that is characterized by a rapid reduction in predominantly hyperopic refractive errors in conjunction with changes in AL and corneal and lens power.
11 12 13 Also evident during this phase is the transformation of the distribution of refractive error from a normal distribution at birth into a tight and peaked distribution. Such change is generally interpreted as reflecting active processes that are intended to achieve emmetropia by matching the AL to the optical power of the eye. Formal statistical analyses of skew and kurtosis for the distributions of refraction and ocular components, however, have rarely been reported.
In this study of two cross-sectional population-based samples of 6- and 12-year-old children, we compared the skew and kurtosis of refraction and ocular biometric parameters and assessed the contribution of ocular biometry to the variability in SER by age and ethnicity.
The Sydney Myopia Study is a population-based survey of eye health in children attending primary and high schools across the metropolitan area of Sydney, Australia. This project forms part of the larger Sydney Childhood Eye Study. The present report focuses on the findings in high school students, who were predominantly 12 years of age, referred to hereafter as 12-year-old children. Some of the findings in primary school children (predominantly 6 years of age) are also included for comparison. Approval for the study was obtained from the Human Research Ethics Committee of the University of Sydney, the New South Wales Department of Education, and the Catholic Education Office. The study adhered to the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki.
Details of the sampling method, examination procedures and characteristics of the participating 6- and 12-year-old children have been described.
8 14 15 In brief, stratified random cluster sampling was used to select primary and secondary schools across the Sydney metropolitan region for participation. A total of 55 schools, with a proportional mix of public and private/religious schools were included. Examinations in the 6-year-old children were conducted from August 2003 to October 2004, and in the 12-year-old children, between November 2004 and November 2005. Informed consent from at least one parent and verbal assent from each child was obtained before examination.
Overall, 1765 6-year-old children (78.9% response) and 2353 12-year-old children (75.0% response) had parental permission to participate. Of these, 38 children (24 6-year-old children and 14 12-year-old children) were not examined due to absence during the in-school examination period. The mean ages of participants in the two samples were 6.7 years and 12.7 years, respectively. Ethnic origins of the participating children were predominantly European Caucasian (60.0% of 6-year-olds; 64.5% of 12-year-olds) and East Asian (17.2% and 15.0%, respectively).
Cycloplegic autorefraction was performed with an autorefractor (model RK-F1; Canon, Tokyo, Japan). This instrument generated five reliable readings of refraction in each eye; the median reading was used for analysis. Cycloplegia was induced using cyclopentolate 1% (1 drop), 2 minutes after corneal anesthesia with amethocaine 0.5%. Tropicamide 1% (1 drop) and phenylephrine 2.5% (1 drop) were also used in some children to obtain adequate mydriasis (a minimum pupil diameter of 6 mm). Autorefraction was repeated 25 minutes after the last drop.
Measurement of ocular biometric parameters was performed with an optical biometer (IOLMaster; Carl Zeiss Meditec, Oberkochen, Germany). AL was measured as the distance from the anterior corneal vertex to the retinal pigment epithelium along fixation, automatically adjusted for retinal thickness. The validity of AL increments was assessed using the signal-to-noise ratio (SNR), where SNR ≥ 2.0 indicated a reliable result. Corneal radius of curvature, determined from the reflection of a hexagonal array of lights on the cornea, was measured along the flattest and steepest meridians. Three consistent keratometry readings were used in analysis, with consistency based on a variation of 0.1 D or less in corneal astigmatism. Anterior chamber depth was measured as the distance from the anterior corneal surface to the anterior lens surface by image analysis of an optical section. These measurements were considered valid if individual measurements varied by no more than 0.15 mm.
Anthropometric measures in the children included assessment of height and weight. The child’s height was measured with shoes off, using a freestanding height rod. Body mass index (BMI) was calculated (BMI = weight in kilograms/height in square meters).
Data were analyzed with commercial software (SAS software ver. 9.1.3; SAS, Cary, NC). As biometric data for the right and left eyes were highly correlated, analyses were performed using data for the right eye only. The overall distributions of refraction and ocular biometric parameters were assessed for kurtosis and skew. Kurtosis is a measure of how data points are concentrated around the mean of a distribution; higher kurtosis values indicate a sharper peak than the normal distribution.
The associations of ocular biometric parameters to SER were assessed using three methods: Pearson correlation, partial correlation coefficient (PCC), and β coefficient from linear regression models. The PCC was used to describe the relative contribution of individual biometric parameters to the variability in SER, adjusting for demographic variables such as age, gender, and ethnicity. Linear regression models were used to assess the impact of a unit change in biometric parameter on the SER. Analyses were performed in all participants and separately for children from the two major ethnic groups in the study (European Caucasian and East Asian). Confidence intervals (CI) are 95%.
Overall, in the 6- and 12-year-old children, correlations between SER and individual biometric variables were all statistically significant (all
P < 0.05). The strongest biometric correlations for SER in the two samples of children were with AL/CR ratio, AL, and anterior chamber depth, in terms of both Pearson correlation coefficients and PCCs
(Table 2) .
In the sample of 12-year-old children, linear regression analyses showed that an increase of 1.0 mm in AL or anterior chamber depth would be associated with a myopic shift of 0.96 to 0.97 D. AL accounted for 37% of the variability in SER, after adjustment for age, gender, and ethnicity. Models that included AL, age, gender, and ethnicity explained 49% of the variation in SER. Other parameters, such as corneal radius of curvature and lens power explained smaller proportions of the variability in SER after adjustment for age, gender, and ethnicity (PCC < 0.12).
In the two ethnic groups within the 12-year-old sample, the correlation between AL and SER was higher for children of East Asian ethnicity than for children of European Caucasian ethnicity (
r = −0.79 vs. −0.47). Linear regression analyses also predicted greater myopic shifts in refraction for increases in AL within the East Asian ethnic group (β coefficient = −1.42 vs. −0.61). Other notable differences between the two ethnic groups were evident in linear regression analyses of anterior chamber depth and lens power
(Table 2) .
In the sample of 6-year-old children, per unit increases in AL had a significantly lower impact on SER (β coefficient = −0.53) and accounted for a smaller proportion of the variability in refraction (PCC = 0.19) than that reported for the 12-year-old children. For the two ethnic groups in this younger sample, correlation coefficients were quite similar, with substantial overlap of the confidence intervals for the β coefficients in linear regression analyses.
The impact of nonbiometric factors (parental myopia, demographic measures of socioeconomic status, BMI, outdoor activity, and near work) on childhood refraction was also assessed individually and in combined models using linear regression in the 12-year-old sample. Unadjusted linear models showed that parental myopia alone accounted for up to 8.6% of the variability in refraction (PCC for at least one myopic parent = 0.015; for two myopic parents = 0.086), and that demographic measures (home ownership, parental employment, and parental education) each accounted for less than 2.0%. The contributions from near work and outdoor activity were 0.9% and 3.5%, respectively, while childhood BMI accounted for less than 0.1% of the variability in refraction.
A multivariate model of statistically significant nonbiometric parameters (age, gender, ethnicity, parental myopia, parental employment, near work, and outdoor activity) explained 27.3% of the variation in childhood refraction. The addition of childhood AL to the model increased the proportion to 56.2%. According to this final model, a 1.0-mm increase in AL was associated with a −0.91-D difference in refraction (P < 0.0001), whereas 1 hour spent out of doors each week was associated with a difference in refraction of +0.13 D (P < 0.0001). Near work did not have any discernible effect on refraction in this multivariate model (0.00 D; P = 0.97), although a significant impact from East Asian ethnicity persisted (β coefficient = −0.75, P < 0.0001).
Oculometric differences between other ethnic groups have been reported. In a study of Malay (
n = 904) and Melanesian (
n = 753) children aged 6 to 17 years, Garner et al.
37 reported that the individual correlations of SER with AL, anterior chamber depth, and lens power were higher in the Malay than in the Melanesian children; however, correlations for corneal power were similar. The sample of Melanesian children, however, was somewhat younger than the sample of Malay children, and so age may have been a confounding variable. Measurement bias was also possible in this comparison of Malay and Melanesian children, since different measurement instruments were used in the two samples.
Among the 6-year-old children, we found that the correlation between SER and AL was similar in both the European Caucasian and the East Asian ethnic groups. The variability in refraction attributed to AL was also similar (19% and 13%, for European Caucasian and East Asian children, respectively). These findings suggest that the factors underlying ethnic differences in refraction most likely occur after the age of 6 years.
Overall, our results in the 12-year-old children confirm that in the two ethnic groups of East Asian and European Caucasian ethnicity, AL was the main contributor to SER, although there were various degrees of impact. Recently presented data suggest that the relationship between AL and SER is nonlinear and that at longer AL there is a greater impact on SER (Rose KA et al. IOVS 2007;48:ARVO E-Abstract 1535). The longer mean AL in the East Asian children, may therefore account for some of the stronger associations in this ethnic group.
In the 12-year-old sample, longer AL in the East Asian ethnic group also appear to explain the apparent ethnic differences in other ocular biometric parameters, since linear regression analyses excluding myopic children were quite similar between the two ethnic groups
(Fig. 2) . Continuing axial elongation, which appears to be more common in the East Asian ethnic group, was found to be associated with greater variability in corneal curvature, but little further increase in anterior chamber depth and/or reduction in lens power. Overall, these results suggest that the underlying processes of refractive development are similar in the two ethnic groups.